ebook img

Where Mathematics Come From: How The Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics Into Being PDF

498 Pages·2000·10.913 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Where Mathematics Come From: How The Embodied Mind Brings Mathematics Into Being

WhereM athematics ComesF rom HowT HEE MBODIEDM IND BRINGS MATHE MAT IC S°'"' ""''""""mec�,,.._,,n�-,,m-u=;; INTO BEING GeorLgaek off RafEa.Ne uln ez BASIC B BOOKS A Member of the Perseus Books Group Alfilg uwreerdser abwyRn a fEa.Ne 1l1 fiweiztt,hhe ex ceopftt ihfoeon l lowing. Fig1u.ro1enp a1g7ei rse prbiynp teerdm ifsrsoNimao tnu( rKWe.y n"nA,d diatnSiduo bnt rbayHc utmiaonn InfaVnot3ls5.,78 "4: 9 -c7o5p0y)1r,9i 9gM2ha,tc miMlalgaanzL itnde.s Thger apihnF iicg1 u.ro2enp a2g0ei a sd apftreoGdm.M andalneBdrJ. S . h e"bSau,b iAtniA znianlogyIf:st iss CompoPnreoncte JsosuerosEfn, xa"pl e riPmseyncthaGolel noeg1ry1a(:1)l 1 :1,- 22. Fig1u.ro3enp ag2e5i mso diffireoAdmt loafHs u maAnn atobymF yr aNnekt (tpelr9a 9"t,Me e nianngde s Braicno"p)y,1r 9i8gb9hyH ta vMaesd iMedia. DrawiinFn iggs2u l.rao e np a3g2ea nFdi g2u.ro3enp a4g0ea rcer edtioGt iendBg eerri nger. Fig2u.ro4enp a4g5ed, r abwyRn a fEaN.eu lfi eiazsd, a pftreoTdma schedneMbrau tchhe mbayItN .i. Bk r on­ steainKnd.A .S emend2j5atejhde iwtc,io opny,1r 9i9gb1hyB t G. . T eubVneerrl agsgSetsuet!tlgsacrhta.f t, Fig1u2r.ea2sn1a d2 .o2npb a 2g6ea9 r fer oEml emoetfnh tTeso poolPfol gaySn eeot Pfso ibnyMt .sH .A .N ew­ manc,o py1r9i9gb2hyD t o vPeurb licIanRtcei,po rnibsnyp t eerdm ission. Fig1u2r.eo2 npc a g2e6i 9rs e prbiynp teerdm iosfOs pieoCnno uPrutb liCsohmipnaagnd iyv,i osfCi aornn s PublishIiLnf,gr ,oF moP uenrduao,tfG i eoonmse tDrayvH iibdly b ceorpty,1r 9i7gb1hyO t p eCno uPrutb lish­ inCgo mpany. Fig1u2r.ep2 ad2g 6ei9 fs r oWmh aItMs a themAantE ilcesm!eA nptparrotyaoI c dhea ansMd e thsoedcso,n d editbiyRo inc hCaorudra annHdte rbReorbtbe idnisbt,yIe adSn t ewcaorpty,1r 9ig4bh1ytt h lea Rtiec hard CourHaenrtb,e rta nRrdoe bvbibisyIne aSsdnt ewUasrebtdyp. e rmiosfOs xifoUonnr idv ePrrseIisntscy,. Fig1u2r.oe2n pe a 2g6ei9 fs r oTma schedneMbrau tchhe mbayItN .i.B k r onasntKde.A i .Sn e mend2j5atjhe w, editcioopny,1r 9i9gb1hyB t G. . T eubVneerrl agsgSetsuetlRtlegspacrrhiwtani.ftt teh,d permission. Fig1u2ro.enp4 a 2g8ei5 ms o diffireoTdmoI nfinainBtdey y oAnC du:l tHuirsatolotf rh Iyen finbiyEt lMeia or, copy1r9i8gb7hyB t i rkhBiiousstIeonrnwc ,i. tp,he rmission. FigCuSr4 .eo5 np a4g4ei 3as p ictofRu arfeEa N.eu lfi efza'tshs elrir'dusel( eT.h aDnakds!,) Copyr©i g2h0bty0G 0e oLragkeao nfRdfa fEaN.eu lfi ez PublibsyBh aesBdio co ks, AM emboetfr hP ee rsBeouoGskr so up Alrli grhetsse Prrvienidttn.eh Uden iStteadot Afem se rNiocp aao.rtf th biosom kab yer eproidanun cye dm an­ newrh atswoietvheoru tp·ewrrmiitestxseciineont pn htc e a osbfer iqeufo taetmiboondisinc e rdi atritciacll es anrde viFeoiwrns f.o rmaadtdirBoeanssB,sio co 3k8sP7,a rAkve nSuoeuN tehwY, o rNkY1, 0 016-8810. DesibgynR eadc Hheegla rty LibroafCr oyn gcraetsasl ogingd-aiitnasa-v pauibllaibclaet.i on ISB0N- 465-03771-2 ISB9N7 8-0-465-03771-1 EBA 061 09 8 7 6 5 ToR afaepla'rse nts, CesNaurn aenzEd l iEarnraa zuriz, toG eorwgief'es, KathFlreuemnk in, antdot hmee moorfty w doe farri ends JamDe.Ms c Cawley and FranJcV.ia srceol a Contents Acknowledgments ix ��� ri IntrodWuhcyCt oigonni:St ciiveMena ctett eoMr ast hematic1s Partl THEE MBODIMEONFBT A SIACR ITHMETIC 1 ThBer aIinnn'Asar tiet hmetic 15 2 BAr iIenft rodtuotc htCeio ognn itivoef SEtcmhibeeo ndMciieen dd2 7 3 EmbodAireidt hmTehGteri ocu:n Mdeitnagp hors 50 4 WheDroet hLea wosAf r ithCmoemtei cF rom? 77 ParItI ALGEBRLAO,G IACN,D S ETS 5 EsseanncAdel gebra 107 6 BoolMee'tsa pChloarsa:sn eSdsy mboLloigci c 121 7 SeatnsHd y persets 140 ParItl l THEE MBODIMEONFIT N FINITY 8 ThBea sMiect apohfIo nrfi nity 155 9 Real NuamnLbdie mrist s 181 VII VIII CONTENTS 10 Transfinite Numbers 208 11 Infinitesimals 223 Part IV BANNING SPACE AND MOTION: THE DISCRETIZATION PROGRAM THAT SHAPED MODERN MATHEMATICS 12 Points and the Continuum 259 13 Continuity for Numbers: The Triumph of Dedekind's Metaphors 292 14 Calculus Without Space or Motion: Weierstrass's Metaphorical Masterpiece 306 Le tnroorum and: A CLASSIC PARADOX OF INFINITY 325 PartV IMPLICATIONS FOR THE PHILOSOPHY OF MATHEMATICS 15 The Theory of Embodied Mathematics 337, 16 The Philosophy of Embodied Mathematics 364 Part VI e'ITi + 1 = 0 A CASE STUDY OF THE COGNITIVE STRUCTURE OF CLASSICAL MATHEMATICS Case Study 1. Analytic Geometry and Trigonometry 383 Case Study 2. What Is e? 399 Case Study 3. What Is i? 420 Case Study 4. e'ITi + 1 = 0-How the Fundamental Ideas of Classical Mathematics Fit Together 433 References 453 Index 473 Acknowled ments g T without a lot of help, espe­ HIS BOOK WOULD NOT HAVE BEEN POSSIBLE cially from mathematicians and mathematics students, mostly at the University of California at Berkeley. Our most immediate debt is to Reuben Hersh, who has long been one of our heroes and who has supported this endeavor since its inception. We have been helped enormously by conversations with mathematicians, es­ pecially Ferdinando Arzarello, Elwyn Berlekamp, George Bergman, Felix Brow­ der, Martin Davis, Joseph Goguen, Herbert Jaeger, William Lawvere, Leslie Lamport, Lisa Lippincott, Giuseppe Longo, Robert Osserman, Jean Petitot, David Steinsalz, William Thurston, and Alan Weinstein. Other scholars con­ cerned with mathematical cognition and education have made crucial contri­ butions to our understanding, especially Mariolina Bartolini Bussi, Paolo Boero, Janete Bolite Frant, Stanislas Dehaene, Jean-Louis Dessalles, Laurie Edwards, Lyn English, Gilles Fauconnier, Jerry Feldman, Deborah Forster, Cathy Kessel, Jean Lassegue, Paolo Mancosu, Maria Alessandra Mariotti, Joao Filipe Matos, Srini Narayanan, Bernard Plancherel, Jean Retschitzki, Adrian Robert, and Mark Turner. In spring 1997 we gave a graduate seminar at Berkeley, called "The Metaphorical Structure of Mathematics: Toward a Cognitive Science of Mathematical Ideas," as a way of approaching the task of writing this book. We are grateful to the students and faculty in that seminar, especially Mariya Brod­ sky, Mireille Broucke, Karen Edwards, Ben Hansen, Ilana Horn, Michael Kleber, Manya Raman, and Lisa Webber. We would also like to thank the students in George Lakoff's course on The Mind and Mathematics in spring 2000, expe­ cially Lydia Chen, Ralph Crowder, Anton Dochterman, Markku Hannula, Jef­ frey Heer, Colin Holbrook, Amit Khetan, Richard Mapplebeck-Palmer, Matthew Rodriguez, Eric Scheel, Aaron Siegel, Rune Skoe, Chris Wilson, Grace Wang, and Jonathan Wong. Brian Denny, Scott Nailor, Shweta Narayan, and IX X ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Katherine Talbert helped greatly with their detailed comments on a preliminary version of the manuscript. A number of readers have pointed out errors in the first printing, some typo­ graphical, some affecting content. We extend our thanks for this assistance to Joseph Auslander, Bonnie Gold, Alan Jennings, Hector Lomeli, Michael Reeken, and Norton Starr. We are not under the illusion that we can thank our partners, Kathleen Frumkin and Elizabeth Beringer, sufficiently for their immense support in this enterprise, but we intend to work at it. The great linguist and lover of mathematics James D. McCawley, of the Uni­ versity of Chicago, died unexpectedly before we could get him the promised draft of this book. As someone who gloried in seeing dogma overturned and who came to intellectual maturity having been taught that mathematics provided foundations for linguistics, he would have delighted in the irony of seeing argu­ ments for the reverse. Just before the publication of this paperback edition, our friend the brilliant neuroscientist Francisco J. Varela, of the Laboratory of Cognitive Neurosciences and Brain Imaging in Paris, died after a long illness. His support had been fun­ damental to our project, and we had looked forward to many more years of fruit­ ful dialog, which his untimely death has brought to an abrupt end. His deep and pioneering understanding of the embodied mind will continue to inspire us, as it has for the past two decades. It has been our great good fortune to be able to do this research in the mag­ nificent, open intellectual community of the University of California at Berke­ ley. We would especially like to thank the Institute of Cognitive Studies and the International Computer Science Institute for their support and hospitality over many years. Rafael N\'.1ftez would like to thank the Swiss National Science Foundation for fellowship support that made the initial years of this research possible. George Lakoff offers thanks to the university's Committee on Research for grants that helped in the preparation and editing of the manuscript. Finally, we can think of few better places in the world to brainstorm and find sustenance than O Chame, Cafe Fanny, Bistro Odyssia, Cafe N efeli, The Musi­ cal Offering, Cafe Strada, and Le Bateau Ivre. Preface W E ARE COGNITIVE SCIENTISTS-a linguist and a psychologist-each with a long-standing passion for the beautiful ideas of mathematics. As specialists within a field that studies the nature and structure of ideas, we realized that despite the remarkable advances in cognitive science and a long tradition in philosophy and history, there was still no discipline of mathe­ matical idea analysis from a cognitive perspective-no cognitive science of mathematics. With this book, we hope to launch such a discipline. A discipline of this sort is needed for a simple reason: Mathematics is deep, fundamental, and essential to the human experience. As such, it is crying out to be understood. It has not been. Mathematics is seen as the epitome of precision, manifested in the use of symbols in calculation and in formal proofs. Symbols are, of course, just sym­ bols, not ideas. The intellectual content of mathematics lies in its ideas, not in the symbols themselves. In short, the intellectual content of mathematics does not lie where the mathematical rigor can be most easily seen-namely, in the symbols. Rather, it lies in human ideas. But mathematics by itself does not and cannot empirically study human ideas; human cognition is simply not its subject matter. It is up to cognitive sci­ ence and the neurosciences to do what mathematics itself cannot do-namely, apply the science of mind to human mathematical ideas. That is the purpose of this book. One might think that the nature of mathematical ideas is a simple and obvi­ ous matter, that such ideas are just what mathematicians have consciously taken them to be. From that perspective, the commonplace formal symbols do as good a job as any at characterizing the nature and structure of those ideas. If that were true, nothing more would need to be said. XI XII PREFACE But those of us who study the nature of concepts within cognitive science know, from research in that field, that the study of human ideas is not so sim­ ple. Human ideas are, to a large extent, grounded in sensory-motor experience. Abstract human ideas make use of precisely formulatable cognitive mecha­ nisms such as conceptual metaphors that import modes of reasoning from sen­ sory-motor experience. It is always an empirical question just what human ideas are like, mathematical or not. The central question we ask is this: How can cognitive science bring sys­ tematic scientific rigor to the realm of human mathematical ideas, which lies outside the rigor of mathematics itself? Our job is to help make precise what mathematics itself cannot-the nature of mathematical ideas. Rafael Nunez brings to this effort a background in mathematics education, the development of mathematical ideas in children, the study of mathematics in indigenous cultures around the world, and the investigation of the founda­ tions of embodied cognition. George Lakoff is a major researcher in human con­ ceptual systems, known for his research in natural-language semantics, his work on the embodiment of mind, and his discovery of the basic mechanisms of everyday metaphorical thought. The general enterprise began in the early 1990s with the detailed analysis by one of Lakoff's students, Ming Ming Chiu (now a professor at the Chinese Uni­ versity in Hong Kong), of the basic system of metaphors used by children to comprehend and reason about arithmetic. In Switzerland, at about the same time, Nunez had begun an intellectual quest to answer these questions: How can human beings understand the idea of actual infinity-infinity conceptual­ ized as a thing, not merely as an unending process? What is the concept of ac­ tual infinity in its mathematical manifestations-points at infinity, infinite sets, infinite decimals, infinite intersections, transfinite numbers, infinitesi­ mals? He reasoned that since we do not encounter actual infinity directly in the world, since our conceptual systems are finite, and since we have no cognitive mechanisms to perceive infinity, there is a good possibility that metaphorical thought may be necessary for human beings to conceptualize infinity. If so, new results about the structure of metaphorical concepts might make it possible to precisely characterize the metaphors used in mathematical concepts of infinity. With a grant from the Swiss NSF, he came to Berkeley in 1993 to take up this idea with Lakoff. We soon realized that such a question could not be answered in isolation. We would need to develop enough of the foundations of mathematical idea analysis so that the question could be asked and answered in a precise way. We would need to understand the cognitive structure not only of basic arithmetic but also of sym-

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.