ebook img

Revision Cognitive and Instructional Processes PDF

231 Pages·2004·3.687 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Revision Cognitive and Instructional Processes

Revision Cognitive and Instructional Processes STUDIES INWRITING VOLUME 13 Series Editor: GertRijlaarsdam,University ofAmsterdam, TheNetherlands EditorialBoard: LindaAllal,UniversityofGeneva,Switzerland EricEsperet, University ofPoitiers, France DavidGalbraith,Staffordshire University, UK JoachimGrabowski, University ofHeidelberg, Germany Ronald Kellogg,St.Louis University, USA LuciaMason, University ofPadova, Italy MartaMilian, Universitat AutonomaBarcelona, Spain SarahRansdell,Florida Atlantic University, USA LilianaTolchinsky, University ofBarcelona,Spain MarkTorrance, Staffordshire University, UK AnniePiolat, University ofAix-en-Provence, France Paivi Tynjala,University ofJyvdskyla, Finland CarelvanWijk, Tilburg University, TheNetherlands Kluwer Academic Publishers continues to publish the international book series Studies in Writing, founded by Amsterdam University Press. The intended readers are all those interested in the foundations of writing and learning and teaching processes inwrittencomposition. The seriesaimsat multipleperspectives ofwriting, education and texts. Therefore authors and readers come from various fields of research, from curriculum development andfrom teacher training.Fields of research covered are cognitive, socio-cognitive and developmental psychology, psycholinguistics, text linguistics, curriculum development, instructional science. The series aim to cover theoretical issues, supported by empirical research, quantitative as well as qualitative, representing a wide range of nationalities. The series provides a forum for research from established researchers and welcomes contributionsfromyoungresearchers. Thetitlespublishedinthisseriesarelistedattheendofthisvolume. Revision Cognitive and Instructional Processes edited by Linda Allal University 0/ Geneva, Switzerland Lucile Chanquoy University 0/ Nantes, France Pierre Largy University 0/ Toulouse Le Mirail, France SPRINGER SCIENCE+BUSINESS MEDIA, LLC Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Title: Revision: Cognitive and Instructional Processes Editors: Linda Allal, Lucile Chanquoy, Pierre Largy ISBN 978-94-010-3776-1 ISBN 978-94-007-1048-1 (eBook) DOI 10.1007/978-94-007-1048-1 A c.I.P. Catalogue record for this book is available from the Library of Congress. Copyright © 2004 Springer Science+Business Media New York Originally published by Kluwer Academic Publisher in 2004 Softcover reprint of the hardcover 1s t edition 2004 All rights reserved. No part of this work may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, microfilming, recording, or otherwise, without the written permission from the Publisher, with the exception of any material supplied specifically for the purpose ofbeing entered and executed on a computer system, for exclusive use by the purchaser of the work. Printed on acid-free paper. TABLE OFCONTENTS INTRODUCTION:REVISIONREVISITED LindaAllal & Lucile Chanquoy WHATTRIGGERS REVISION? 9 John R.Hayes PROCESSINGTIMEANDCOGNITIVEEFFORTINREVISION: 21 EFFECTSOFERRORTYPEANDOFWORKINGMEMORYCA- PACITY AnniePiolat, Jean-YvesRoussey, Thierry Olive, & Murielle Amada ORTHOGRAPHICREVISION:THECASEOFSUBJECT-VERB 39 AGREEMENTINFRENCH Pierre Largy, Lucile Chanquoy,&AlexandraDedeyan REVISIONINTHECONTEXTOFDIFFERENTDRAFTING 63 STRATEGIES DavidGalbraith &Mark Torrance AUDIENCEPERSPECTIVEINYOUNGWRITERS' COMPOSING 87 ANDREVISING. READINGASTHEREADER DavidR. Holliway & DeborahMcCutchen REVISIONOFFORMANDMEANINGINLEARNINGTOWRITE 103 COMPREHENSIBLETEXT AmosVanGelderen & Ron Oostdam INSIGHTSFROMINSTRUCTIONAL RESEARCHONREVISION 125 WITHSTRUGGLING WRITERS Charles A.MacArthur, Steve Graham, &Karen R.Harris INTEGRATED WRITINGINSTRUCTIONAND 139 THEDEVELOPMENT OFREVISIONSKILLS Linda Allal EFFECTSOFCOLLABORATIVE REVISIONONCHILDREN'S 157 ABILITYTO WRITEUNDERSTANDABLE NARRATIVETEXTS PietroBoscolo & KatiaAscorti COLLABORATIVEREVISION ANDMETACOGNITIVEREFLEC- 171 TION INASITUATION OF NARRATIVETEXT PRODUCTION YvianeRouiller THESTUDY OFREVISION ASAWRITING PROCESS AND ASA 189 LEARNING-TO-WRITE PROCESS.TWO PROSPECTIVERE- SEARCHAGENDAS GertRijlaarsdam, Michel Couzijn, & Huub Van denBergh REFERENCES 209 AUTHORINDEX 225 SUBJECT INDEX 231 LISTOFCONTRIBUTORS 235 INTRODUCTION Revision Revisited LINDA ALLAL* & LUCILE CHANQUOY** *University ofGeneva, SWitzerland, **UniversityofNantes, France Revision is a fundamental component of the writing process. So fundamental that for some specialists writing is largely a matter of revising, or as Murray (1978) stated, "Writing isrewriting..." (p. 85).Experience with writing does not, however, automatically translate into increased skill in revision. Learning to revise is a lengthy, complex endeavor. Beginning writers do little revision spontaneously and even experienced writersencounter difficulties in attempting to improve the quality oftheirtexts(Fitzgerald, 1987). Althoughrevisionhasbeenextensively dealtwith in thewriting andlearning-to write literature, this book proposes to "revisit" theory and research in this area through a series of new contributions. The introduction begins with an overview of what revision encompasses. It thenexamines two parallel interrogations that under lie the chapters assembled here, namely: (1) What are the implications of research on cognitive processesfor instruction in revision? (2) What are the questions raised by instructional research for the investigation of cognitive processes of revision? A finalsectionpresentsthechaptersofthisbook. 1. ANOVERVIEWOFREVISION OurstartingpointwillbeFitzgerald's (1987)definition ofthe activityofrevision: Revisionmeansmakinganychangeatanypointinthewriting process.It involves iden tifyingdiscrepancies between intendedtextandinstantiatedtext,deciding whatcouldor should bechanged inthetext,howtomakedesired changes andoperating, thatis,mak ingthedesired changes. Changes mayormay not affectmeaning oftext and they may bemajororminor.(p.484) 2 L. ALLAL &L.CHANQUOY A substantial number of researchers have proposed additional conceptual distinc tions which enlargeor refine thisdefinition (see Alamargot&Chanquoy,2001, for a detailed presentationof major models andassociatedresearch). Since the introductionofthe wel1-known,hierarchical models ofwriting, starting with Hayes andFlower (1980), it isgeneral1yaccepted that revision can intervene at any point in the writing process. The temporal relations between revision and the other sub-processes of writing - planning and translating or transcribing - can be characterized in three broad categories (Witte, 1985):(1) pretextual revision, which affects intentions, plans or mental formulations of text before transcription has oc curred; (2) on-line revision, which is integrated in the process of transcription and entails changes made while reviewing a word or group of words that has just been written;(3)deferredrevision,which takes place once arelativelycompletedraft- of atext or asizeablepartof atext (e.g.,achapter) - hasbeen written. Revision has been conceptualized primarily as a problem solving process acti vated by the identification of "discrepancies between intended text and instantiated text," as stated by Fitzgerald.This perspective is a central feature ofthe models de veloped by Hayes and his co-workers (Hayes & Flower, 1980; Hayes, Flower, Schriver, Stratman & Carey, 1987; Hayes, 1996), as wel1 as of those proposed by other researchers, such as the CDO (compare- diagnose - operate) model ofBere iter and Scardamalia (1987),or the procedural model ofButterfield,Hackerand Al bertson (1996).The pervasiveness of this perspective is evidenced by the fact that expressions like "problem detection," "problem diagnosis," "problem resolution," have often been treated as synonymous with revision. Some researchers have sug gested, however, that revision may be initiated by discoveries made while writing, without any prior diagnosis of a problem present in the current instantiated text (Galbraith, 1992).The wayan idea has been formulated may suggest a new orienta tion to be given to the text, and thus operate as a bottom-up form ofproactiveregu lation of what is next written. The orientation taken by emerging text can also lead, retroactively, tomodificationsofalready written text. Revision includes a number ofsub-processes that have been defined in various ways bydifferentauthors (see reviews by Alamargot&Chanquoy,2001; Fitzgerald, 1987). Several main sub-processes are nevertheless present in al1 models, under varying designations.Revisionentails, first ofal1, anactivity ofreviewing, that is,of reading or (re)processing existing text or existing mental formulations oftext. Re viewing general1ytakes place with the aim ofevaluating the adequacyof"textthus far" with respect to the writer's intentions or in relation to some other reference (e.g., conventions used in proof-reading for ajournal). Hayes and Flower's (1980) initial conceptionofreviewing was later expanded to include several different types ofreading (reading to comprehend, to evaluate, todefine problems:see Hayes et aI., 1987), and was subsequently reformulated in broader terms as a "text processing" (Hayes, 1996). The reviewingprocess leads todecisions regardingactions to betaken.Itmaybe decided that no action is warranted:A draft is reviewed, reflections are made about possible changes, but none are final1ycarried out. If, however, a modification is considered necessary or desirable, several strategies may be used, depending on the writer's goal. Revision strategies can be classified in two major categories: editing, REVISIONREVISITED 3 which entails error correction and modifications designed to improve the adequacy of text without changing its general meaning;and rewriting, which entails transfor mations of text content (addition or deletion of segments),changes intextorganiza tion (sequencing of segments), and modifications of the meaning conveyed by a segment. These two strategies can be combined in a revision process that corrects errors whileatthesametimetransforming content. The execution ofthese strategies involves the coordination of the writer's goals with a wide range of knowledge components (knowledge of content, of text grammar, of syntax, of spelling, etc). Depending on the writer's degree of expertise, some components intervening in re vision are activated automatically whereas others require deliberate reflection. The progressive atomization of basic skills allows the writer to exercise intentional, re flective control over increasingly complex components.For example, the automati zation of skill in spelling allows greater investment of the writer's cognitive re sources in revisions linked to text organization and elaboration of content (Allal, Betrix Kohler, Rieben, Rouiller Barbey, Saada-Robert, & Wegmuller, 2001; McCutchen, Covill,Hoyne, & Mildes, 1994). Although the distinction between internal revision (what the author formulates mentally) andexternal revision (what is markedon atext) hasexisted forsome time (Murray, 1978), many researchers have tended to restrict the term revision to the observed marks written on a text (e.g., Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1986). This usage has been reinforced by the development of various taxonomies of revision focused on different characteristics of observed changes in text. Most taxonomies (e.g., AI lal, 2000; Chanquoy, 1997; Faigley & Witte, 1981; Hayes et aI., 1987; Sommers, 1980) propose multidimensional classifications taking into account the meaning preserving or meaning-transforming nature of a modification, the level oflanguage affected by the change, the operations used to carry out a revision (addition, dele tion, substitution, reordering), the effect of the revision (positive,neutral, negative). Inorder todifferentiate more clearly the overall process of revision from the result ing products, Allal (2000) suggests using the term "transformation" for the changes actually carried out. This allows the term revision to keep an inclusive meaning re ferring to theprocesses which lead or, insomecases, do not lead totransformations ofexisting text. Recent research has provided more in-depth specification of the cognitive proc esses involved in revision, including the role of working memory (McCutchen, 1996; Olive Kellogg & Piolat, 2001), the interaction between cognitive and meta cognitive factors (Butterfield et aI., 1996;Plumb, Butterfield, Hacker & Dunlosky, 1994), the progressive structuring of linguistic knowledge needed for writing and revision (Chanquoy & Negro, 1996; Totereau, Barouillet, & Fayol, 1998). Several chapters inthisbookpursuethese linesofinvestigation. 2. COGNITIVE ANDINSTRUCTIONALPROCESSES INREVISION This book approaches the relations between the cognitive processes intervening in revision and the instructional processes brought into play in situations where stu dents are learning to writeand torevise.This intersection is stilllargely unexplored, 4 L. ALLAL&L. CHANQUOY but several examples can be given to illustrate the potential fruitfulness of these complementaryand interactingdirectionsofinquiry. 2.I Implications ofResearch on Cognitive Processesfor Instruction in Writingand Revision The development of programs to teach writing and revision, as well as current re search inthis area, almost always refer tostudies of thecognitiveprocesses involved in revision. The models developed by Hayes and co-workers and by Bereiter and Scardamalia are well-known and the research on cognitive processes has had a sub stantial impact on instructional researchand development. Two significantexamples of this impact will be mentioned. First and foremost, research has fostered recogni tion that writing is a complex activity involving several sub-processes that require specific forms of instructional support. This has led to the development of instruc tional sequences that provide time for student investment in the different sub processes (planning, translating, revision) and material (guidelines, examples, checklists, reminders) designed to facilitate each aspect of writing. In addition, di rect instruction in cognitive strategies has been incorporated in many instructional approaches, such as the Self-regulated strategydevelopmentapproach of Harris and Graham (1996) and the Cognitive strategy instruction in writing, proposed by Englert(1992). Asecond exampleofthe influenceofcognitiveprocessresearch on instructional research concerns the role of individual differences in writing. The importance of these differences (Berninger, Abbott, Whitaker, Sylvester& Nolen, 1995; Galbraith, 1996; McCutchen et ai, 1994) means that any effective instructional approach needs to include ways ofidentifying the relevant types ofstudent variation and, more im portantly, the means ofdealing with this variation.Student differences can be taken into accountinan instructionalprogrambytwo types of measures: • compensatory measures, such as providing some students with forms of proce dural facilitation (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1987) that are not needed by other students; • proactive, adaptive measures which allow students to make choices among sev eral procedures for carrying out a writing task (e.g., letting students decide if they want to revise on-line or in a deferred manner; if they want to plan and then write, oruse amultipledraftingstrategy,Cf. Galbraith, 1996). 2.2 Implications of Research on Instructional Processes for the Investigation of CognitiveProcesses inWritingandRevision Most research on cognitive processes in revision has been conducted with little at tention paid to the findings of instructional research. This is undoubtedlydue to the out-dated, but still widespread tendency to considerinstructional research as an "ap plication" of research on cognitiveprocesses. We believe that instructional research can in fact provide ideas and raise questions that may provide new directions for cognitiveprocess research. To illustratethis assertion we willdiscuss two examples.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.