PoPularity in the Peer SyStem PoPularity in the Peer SyStem edited by antoniuS h. n. CilleSSen DaviD SChwartz lara mayeux t G P he uilford ress new york london © 2011 The Guilford Press A Division of Guilford Publications, Inc. 72 Spring Street, New York, NY 10012 www.guilford.com All rights reserved No part of this book may be reproduced, translated, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, microfilming, recording, or otherwise, without written permission from the Publisher. Printed in the United States of America This book is printed on acid-free paper. Last digit is print number: 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Popularity in the peer system / edited by Antonius H. N. Cillessen, David Schwartz, and Lara Mayeux. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 978-1-60918-066-9 (hbk. : alk. paper) 1. Popularity. 2. Peer pressure. 3. Social acceptance. 4. Social interaction in children. 5. Social interaction in adolescence. 6. Social status. I. Cillessen, Antonius H. N. II. Schwartz, David, 1962– III. Mayeux, Lara. HQ784.P43P67 2011 303.3′27—dc22 2010045001 To our wonderful nieces and nephews, the young lights in our lives who have taught us so much: Maaike, Jop, Aafke, Susanne, and Yvonne —A. H. N. C. Jessica, Lucas, and Sadie —D. S. Nathan and Taylor —L. M. about the editors Antonius H. N. Cillessen, PhD, is Professor and Chair of Developmental Psychology in the Behavioural Science Institute and Vice Dean of the Fac- ulty of Social Science at Radboud University in Nijmegen, The Nether- lands. Dr. Cillessen’s research interests include peer relationships in child- hood and adolescence, popularity, aggression and antisocial behavior, the development of social cognition, and research methods for developmen- tal psychology (sociometric methods, social network analysis, observa- tional research, and longitudinal design and analysis). He has served on the editorial boards of Developmental Psychology, the Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, and the International Journal of Behavioral Development. David Schwartz, PhD, is Associate Professor in the Department of Psy- chology at the University of Southern California in Los Angeles. Dr. Schwartz’s research is broadly concerned with the links between social problems in the peer group during childhood and adolescence and the development of psychopathology. He has published widely on topics related to bully/victim problems in school peer groups, community vio- lence exposure, peer relationships across cultural contexts, friendship, aggression, and popularity. Dr. Schwartz has also served on the editorial boards of Child Development, Developmental Psychology, and the Jour- nal of Abnormal Child Psychology. Lara Mayeux, PhD, is Associate Professor in the Department of Psychol- ogy at the University of Oklahoma in Norman. Dr. Mayeux’s primary research interests are peer relationships and the development of peer status in middle childhood and adolescence, with a particular focus on popularity. Her work, funded by the National Science Foundation, has focused on behavioral, social-cognitive, and gender issues in popularity. vi Contributors Julie Wargo Aikins, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut Amy Bellmore, PhD, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, Wisconsin B. Bradford Brown, PhD, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin–Madison, Madison, Wisconsin William M. Bukowski, PhD, Department of Psychology and Centre for Research in Human Development, Concordia University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada Antonius H. N. Cillessen, PhD, Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Karmon D. Dyches, MS, Department of Psychology, University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma Gary C. Glick, BS, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri Andrea Hopmeyer Gorman, PhD, Department of Psychology, Occidental College, Los Angeles, California Sandra Graham, PhD, Department of Education, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California John J. Houser, MS, Department of Psychology, University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma Scott D. Litwack, MA, Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut Peter E. L. Marks, MA, Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota Lara Mayeux, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Oklahoma, Norman, Oklahoma vii viii Contributors Don E. Merten, PhD, Department of Anthropology, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana Adrienne Nishina, PhD, Department of Human and Community Development, University of California, Davis, Davis, California Anthony D. Pellegrini, PhD, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota Amanda J. Rose, PhD, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri Cary J. Roseth, PhD, Department of Counseling, Educational Psychology, and Special Education, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan Marlene J. Sandstrom, PhD, Department of Psychology, Williams College, Williamstown, Massachusetts David Schwartz, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California Rhiannon L. Smith, MA, Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, Missouri David W. Solberg, BA, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota Mark J. Van Ryzin, PhD, Oregon Social Learning Center, Eugene, Oregon Preface a n 11-year-old girl cries to her parents one day that the popu- lar crowd at school made fun of her, and then days later asks if she may invite those same peers to her birthday party. A well-known high school athlete is involved in a fistfight with a less popular peer, and the athlete is reprimanded and sent back to class, while the peer’s parents are called and he is removed from the school. The news that an admired “super- couple” has engaged in sexual intercourse for the first time spreads quickly across the high school campus, and the boy receives pats on the back while his girlfriend is covertly called a “slut” by other students. The often paradoxical dynamics of high-status children and adoles- cents and their peers are puzzling yet fascinating to parents, educators, and researchers. Feature films of the last 20 years and recent nonfiction books such as Queen Bees and Wannabes (Wiseman, 2002) attest to the popular interest in “the cool kids.” Although there has been a recent surge of interest in popularity and the behaviors of the “in” crowd, social and scientific interest in this topic is not a recent phenomenon. The first empirical study on popularity among children actually appeared in Child Development in the 1930s (Koch, 1933), and Moreno’s classic book Who Shall Survive? was published just a year later. In his book, Moreno (1934) described what he called “sociometric stars”—those individuals who were looked upon favorably by the majority of their peers and stood out as highly accepted members of the classroom. These early investiga- tions of high-status children conceptualized popularity as a combination of acceptance, liking, and positive feelings toward a child on the part of the social group. It was considered a universally desirable characteristic in children, thought to be associated with all kinds of positive attributes. ix x Preface A long tradition of studying well-liked children, called “popular” chil- dren (or “sociometrically popular” children), was born, and hundreds of studies on the topic were published in the decades following Koch’s and Moreno’s writings. Developmental psychologists continued their interest in so-called popular children well into the 1980s and 1990s, developing unique and highly technical methods of measuring status and behavior in the peer cul- ture. These researchers broadened our understanding of these high-status children in many ways, describing their typical peer-directed behaviors, social cognitions, and emotional development, and “popularity” came to be understood as an indicator of any number of socially competent behaviors and positive outcomes. Over time, however, researchers began to question what exactly they were measuring when they assessed “peer popularity,” particularly among adolescents. Research findings that did not fit the traditional pattern began to emerge. Consider the experience of this volume’s first editor. Along with a colleague, he conducted an investigation of children’s per- ceptions of what makes a peer popular. Children were asked directly to list characteristics of popular children, and the researchers expected to hear responses such as “kind,” “funny,” “helps others out,” and the like. Although those responses did appear, so too did other responses of a very different nature: “mean,” “snobby,” “hurts other people”—not at all the traditional understanding of popular children. It became clear over time that the term popularity meant something very different to children than it did to the developmental psychologists studying them. (Researchers in other fields, such as sociology of educa- tion, had already been conducting ethnographic studies of popular ado- lescents for some years with much success; the results of their investiga- tions are also discussed in this book.) To children and adolescents, “being popular” suggests a complex interplay of social behaviors, attitudes, and benefits, not all of which are positive, easily understood, or even fair. The scenarios described in the first paragraph of this preface are representa- tive of the often confusing and frustrating nature of social status dynam- ics, but they are certainly not exhaustive. Since the 1990s, a growing number of studies from a variety of fields have illuminated the distinction between being liked (our old definition of popularity) and being socially powerful. Our understanding of the various forms of high status, how they are alike and different, and how they fit into the ecology of the social group has been greatly improved by the efforts of these researchers. This book describes that body of work. Drawing from develop- mental psychology, sociology of education, social psychology, and other fields, we present here a summary of the empirical research on popularity as a social developmental phenomenon. To clarify the terminology we
Description: