ebook img

Language Learner Autonomy: Policy, Curriculum, Classroom: A Festschrift in Honour of David Little PDF

417 Pages·2010·4.628 MB·
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Language Learner Autonomy: Policy, Curriculum, Classroom: A Festschrift in Honour of David Little

Contents Leo van Lier Foreword: Agency, Self and Identity in Language Learning ix Lorna Carson and Breffni O’Rourke Introduction: Language and Learners, Interdependence and Autonomy xix Acknowledgements xxv Part I Language Learner Autonomy Viljo Kohonen Autonomy, Agency and Community in FL Education: Developing Site-based Understanding through a University and School Partnership 3 Lienhard Legenhausen Group Work, Weak Learners and the Autonomous Classroom: Indirect Support for the Interaction Hypothesis? 29 Ema Ushioda Researching Growth in Autonomy through I-Statement Analysis 45 Naoko Aoki and Osaka University students A Community of Practice as a Space for Collaborative Student Teacher Autonomy 63 vi Leni Dam Developing Learner Autonomy with Adult Immigrants: A Case Study 79 Dieter Wolff Developing Curricula for CLIL: Issues and Problems 103 Bernd Rüschoff Authenticity in Language Learning Revisited: Materials, Processes, Aims 121 Bernd Voss Language Teaching in Higher Education and the TCD Language Modules 135 Lorna Carson Innovation and Autonomy in an Institution-wide Language Programme 151 Part II Second and Minority Language Education Ewelina Debaene and David Singleton The Language Educational Experience of Polish Migrants in France and Ireland 171 Lorraine Leeson Supporting Academic Success for the Irish Deaf Community 189 John Harris Attitude Motivation in the Early Learning of Foreign Languages and Minority Second Languages 213 vii Muiris Ó Laoire Change and Challenge in the Teaching of Irish 231 Pádraig Ó Riagáin Implementing International Standards for Minority Language Policy: The Framework Convention for the Protection of National Minorities and Moldova between 2000 and 2005 251 Waldemar Martyniuk Assessing Competences at the End of Compulsory Schooling: The Polish Case 273 Androula Yiakoumetti and Edith Esch Educational Complexities inherent in Bidialectal Communities and the Potential Contribution of the Common European Framework of Reference to Second-dialect Development 291 Part III Implementing the Common European Framework of Reference and the European Language Portfolio Joseph Sheils Council of Europe Policy for Plurilingual Education 315 Rolf Schärer The European Language Portfolio: Goals, Boundaries and Timelines 327 Hanne Thomsen This is My Portfolio: Telling the Story of My Learning 337 Manolis Sisamakis The Motivational Potential of the European Language Portfolio 351 viii Philip Riley Reflections on Identity, Modernity and the European Language Portfolio 373 Appendix Chronological Bibliography of Publications by David Little 387 Notes on Contributors 399 Index 409 Leo van Lier Foreword: Agency, Self and Identity in Language Learning David Little has influenced language education in Europe profoundly and in many ways. He is probably best known for his pioneering work on the notion of autonomy in language teaching and learning, but the present col- lection shows that there are many other aspects to his work and influence: the CEFR, the ELP, the teaching of Irish as a native and second language, language education for immigrant workers, and more. It is not hard to find something to write about that is relevant to David’s lifelong endeavous. The challenge is to push ahead, that is, to use his work as an inspiration to further explore the visions and ideals that he has championed over many years. My aim is to give a bit of a push, or at least a modest nudge, encour- aged by the many excellent contributions of David’s students, colleagues and friends collected in this volume. I start with the notion of autonomy, but I suggest that the underlying issues that are raised all relate to a central theme that foregrounds the notion of agency and, indeed, advocacy and purpose. None of these things are neutral or uncontested; they all require constant effort and argument, as well as of course action. In this contribution I examine some constructs that are used increas- ingly in our field, and that are deemed to be of significant importance in the learning of additional languages – though we appear still to be struggling to find empirical support. These constructs are agency, self and identity. The first of these constructs, agency, is close to the notion of autonomy that has played a central role in the work of David Little. In fact, I would be hard put to give a precise delineation of the similarities, and especially the differences, between agency and autonomy. For now, let’s regard them as synonymous, noting only that they arise from different – though quite compatible – traditions of scholarship. The second and third notions, self x Leo van Lier and identity, are often lumped together in the various social sciences that discuss them, e.g., psychology, sociology, anthropology, philosophy – and applied linguistics. I shall argue, however, that they are best conceptualised as two different attributes of a person, in our case a language learner. More precisely, the self is a reference point, and identity is a family of processes and activities that co-reference and co-create self and world in a number of ways, with the goal of achieving an ecological fit (the ecology being comprised of physical, social and symbolic relations). 1. Self I will begin with the notion of self, since I take this to be ontologically primary, even though it may be hard to disentangle it from agency; in fact self and agency are probably coeval. In our relations with the world, the self “sprouts” identities, which are ways of relating self to world. The self is basically anything and everything we call “me” or “I” (Harter, 1999). Then, agency refers to the ways in which, and the extents to which, the person (self, identities, and all) is compelled to, motivated to, allowed to, and coerced to, act. Noticing (ironically) the passive in all these statements, I hasten to add that agency refers equally to the person deciding to, wanting to, insisting to, agreeing to, and negotiating to, act. And with these two last sentences, one intransitive and the other transitive, we have captured nicely the complexities of the notion of agency (more about this below). The grammatical choice of passive and active voice in the two parts of the definition above illustrates the Janus-faced nature of the notion of autonomy (and self, identity and autonomy, by extension): it looks in two directions at the same time, to the personal, and to the social, to “myself”, and to “the other”. This harks back to the age-old dualism of subject and object, but we need to understand that “I/me” and “you/it” both contain objective and subjective memories and projections, thus illustrating that the subject-object dichotomy is a false one. Foreword: Agency, Self and Identity in Language Learning xi Philosophically speaking, there are several views on the self. Sceptics, like David Dennett, claim that there is no such thing as the self, and he uses the analogy of a termite colony to explain what he means. A termite colony seems to act in highly coordinated and purposeful ways, even though there is no central command centre or government (i.e., a sort of controlling “self” of the colony as a whole) that tells any individual ant what to do. By analogy, as people, we do things one way or another way, but to claim that we have a “self” that makes decisions is merely a fiction, or at best a convenient metaphor (Dennett, 1991). Counter to Dennett and other sceptics, the neuroscientist Antonio Damasio (1999) does argue for the existence of the notion of self, and he supports his argument by describing a number of cases that illustrate the destruction of self due to reasons of disease or trauma. In accounts of self such as those of Dennett, Damasio and others, self is taken to be close to or identical with consciousness. In addition, as Dennett also points out, consciousness is closely coupled with action or doing things, hence our need to invoke agency in the description of self above. You cannot describe self without describing agency. A person and his or her actions define each other. A final, crucial observation is that in the view of many writers about the self, including Dennett and Damasio (the first philosopher, the second neuroscientist), but also Vygotsky (1987) and Dewey (1916), the self (or consciousness, which as noted to many writers appears to be essentially synonymous) is in equal measure knowledge (cognition) and emotion. John Dewey decried the separation and even opposition between the intellect and the emotions in education. As he put it, professional educators seem to think that “the intellect is a pure light; the emotions are a disturbing heat” (1916, p. 321). He argued that this dualism had a crippling effect on educa- tion. Perhaps as an expression of this perceived split between intellect and affect, or knowing and feeling, there is now a need to think of different kinds of intelligence, instead of just one (Gardner’s multiple intelligences (1993), and Goleman’s emotional intelligence (1995), are examples of this). xii Leo van Lier 2. Agency As I mentioned, I find it hard to distinguish agency from autonomy, or indeed from other related constructs such as self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997), locus of control (Deci & Ryan, 1985), or self-regulation (Vygotsky, 1987). For David Little, learner autonomy is “essentially a matter of the learner’s psychological relation to the process and content of learning … a capac- ity for detachment, critical reflection, decision-making, and independent action” (Little, 1991, p. 4). We can relate autonomy in this sense also to intrinsic motivation, as I did in an earlier discussion (van Lier 1996). We can also relate it to Edward Deci’s pedagogically very useful distinction between “controlling” and “autonomy-supporting” discourse in the class- room (Deci & Flaste, 1995). A much-cited definition of agency is the one by the anthropologist Laura Ahearn: “agency is the socioculturally mediated capacity to act,” which she wisely refers to as a “provisional definition” (2001, p. 112). It is indeed provisional. In a recent paper (2008) I explored a number of L2 classroom interactions in terms of the agency they might display, and found this issue to be far from straightforward. In addition (perhaps more contro- versially), I do not think that the capacity to act has to be socioculturally mediated. For example, if I am running along a forest path and encounter a small creek, I may display agency in deciding to jump across that creek without getting my feet wet. There is nothing sociocultural in that. Relat- ing that same idea to language, if someone asks me when I am in Paris, “Voulez-vous du café? Dessert?” my answer (yes or no) may be mediated more by the contents of my wallet or my stomach than by sociocultural considerations. In other words, we should not assume that there is socio- cultural mediation in everything we do. Foreword: Agency, Self and Identity in Language Learning xiii 3. Identity Above I mentioned that the self “sprouts identities” in its relations with the world. This statement presupposes a distinction between self and iden- tity. A reasonable question to ask is, why make such a distinction, in other words, what is the conceptual and empirical basis for it? The answer to this question is not easy. To begin with, empirically speaking there is very little evidence for the existence of self or identity, let alone its or their impact on L2 development. Neuroscientific case reports and clinical studies, such as those of Damasio (1999), do provide strong grounds for postulating a notion one might call self, associated with brain structures such as the hippocampus, the frontal lobe, and the amygdala, but a distinction between self and identity does not have a great deal of empirical support to date. In addition, many other words are used to refer to the various phenomena associated with self and identity, including notions such as personality, persona, and role, with various assumptions as to their origin, stability, or changeability. Be that as it may, we do know that people have different identities (to use the term our field appears to have settled on – see Block, 2007a). Norton has characterised identity as “a site of struggle” (1995), and I would like to interpret this as the struggle of a person (self) in a particular environment. The struggle is one of, on the one hand, maintaining the integrity of the self and, on the other hand, adapting to (getting along, finding one’s place, surviving or thriving in) the environment. The notion of identity struggle, or of developing new or different iden- tities, is most relevant in a situation in which a person experiences changed or changing environments, including educational, vocational, linguistic or intercultural environments. I would not go so far as to say that in every different environment we enact an entirely different identity, though this depends of course in large measure on how broadly or narrowly we con- strue “identity”. In most cases there will be constancy as well as variation as we move from setting to setting. In stable situations we may move effort- lessly between settings, say home, office, classroom and pub, and it might

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.