ebook img

From beliefs to dynamic affect systems in mathematics education: Exploring a mosaic of relationships and interactions PDF

415 Pages·2015·6.57 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview From beliefs to dynamic affect systems in mathematics education: Exploring a mosaic of relationships and interactions

Advances in Mathematics Education Birgit Pepin Bettina Roesken-Winter E ditors From beliefs to dynamic aff ect systems in mathematics education Exploring a mosaic of relationships and interactions Advances in Mathematics Education Series Editors: Gabriele Kaiser, University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany Bharath Sriraman, The University of Montana, Missoula, MT, USA International Editorial Board: Ubiratan D’Ambrosio (São Paulo, Brazil) Jinfa Cai (Newark, NJ, USA) Helen Forgasz (Melbourne, Victoria, Australia) Jeremy Kilpatrick (Athens, GA, USA) Christine Knipping (Bremen, Germany) Oh Nam Kwon (Seoul, Korea) More information about this series at www.springer.com/series/8392 Birgit Pepin (cid:129) Bettina Roesken-Winter Editors From beliefs to dynamic affect systems in mathematics education Exploring a mosaic of relationships and interactions Editors Birgit Pepin Bettina Roesken-Winter Sør-Trøndelag University College Professional School of Education Trondheim , Norway Department of Mathematics Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin Berlin , Germany ISSN 1869-4918 ISSN 1869-4926 (electronic) ISBN 978-3-319-06807-7 ISBN 978-3-319-06808-4 (eBook) DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-06808-4 Springer Cham Heidelberg New York Dordrecht London Library of Congress Control Number: 2014953525 © Springer International Publishing Switzerland 2015 T his work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or part of the material is concerned, specifi cally the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfi lms or in any other physical way, and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed. Exempted from this legal reservation are brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis or material supplied specifi cally for the purpose of being entered and executed on a computer system, for exclusive use by the purchaser of the work. Duplication of this publication or parts thereof is permitted only under the provisions of the Copyright Law of the Publisher’s location, in its current version, and permission for use must always be obtained from Springer. Permissions for use may be obtained through RightsLink at the Copyright Clearance Center. Violations are liable to prosecution under the respective Copyright Law. T he use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specifi c statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use. While the advice and information in this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication, neither the authors nor the editors nor the publisher can accept any legal responsibility for any errors or omissions that may be made. The publisher makes no warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material contained herein. Printed on acid-free paper Springer is part of Springer Science+Business Media (www.springer.com) Forewo rd From Hidden Dimensions to Dynamic Systems in Affect Research Abstract I n this preface, I turn briefl y to the genesis of an infl uential forerunner to the present volume on affect and mathematics education, namely, B eliefs: A Hidden Variable in Mathematics Education? , published over a decade ago. Some brief snapshots of the contents of the earlier manuscripts are presented and areas then identifi ed in need of further, or more nuanced, exploration are high- lighted. Foregrounding these issues serves as an expedient link between the present volume and the earlier work. Introduction The M athematisches Forschungsinstitut Oberwolfach (The Mathematical Research Institute of Oberwolfach) is idyllically situated in Germany’s Black Forest. Over the years, the Institute has hosted many week-long workshops in which specialist math- ematics topics are explored by experts in the fi eld. “The Institute brings people together for a short but intense period, providing them with ideal conditions under which to pursue research activities which will i nfl uence and stimulate the future development of the fi eld ” (Mathematisches Forschungsinstitut Oberwolfach n.d., emphasis added). The week of November 21 to November 27 in 1999 marked a particularly impor- tant time in the mathematics education research calendar. That week, uniquely, the issues for debate and exploration at the Institute were not confi ned to the fi eld of mathematics per se. Instead the focus was fi rmly on a topic of great relevance to the fi eld of mathematics education: M athematical beliefs and their impact on teaching and learning of mathematics. v vi Foreword Consideration of issues raised during the rich and diverse fl ow of presentations and discussions extended well beyond the life of the workshop. The tradition of many of the previous, and indeed subsequent, workshops held at Oberwolfach was maintained. So, seemingly inevitably, follow up activities were pursued, c ulminating in the preparation of a volume designed to add to existing knowledge and literature in the fi eld explored so intensively during the time spent at the Institute. The gestation period between conception and birth of the book about the infl u- ence and impact of beliefs and aspects of the teaching and learning of mathematics was substantial. Core ideas initially presented and discussed in Oberwolfach were added to. Finally, in 2002, the manuscript B eliefs: A Hidden Variable in Mathematics Education? (Leder et al. 2002) was ready for publication. The Foundations: Inspection What, more than a decade ago, caught the attention of researchers and practitioners concerned with beliefs and their impact on the teaching and learning of mathemat- ics? The “then” state of the art, as captured in Leder et al. (2002), was covered in three distinct but overlapping sections: Beliefs: Conceptualization and measure- ment; Teachers’ beliefs; and S tudents’ beliefs. Inevitably, as in the current volume, affective components beyond beliefs also attracted much attention. Beliefs: Conceptualization and Measurement “The contributions in the fi rst section,” Leder et al. (2002, p. 4) clarifi ed, are particularly concerned with examining what is meant by mathematical beliefs and how they differ from other, related concepts. The authors draw extensively on existing literature, highlight consensus and confusion in the ways various terms have been used in earlier work, and indicate directions for further research without – collectively – offering a unifi ed view on the main theoretical concepts explored: belief, conception, and knowledge. The overall thrust is on the diversity of different starting points which typically correspond to different emphases. Reports of empirical research studies were largely absent in this section. Capturing the essence of beliefs was a common theme – a task made more diffi cult because of the multiple and often loose usage of the term “belief”. This practice, it was frequently argued, should be challenged. That those working in the fi eld are unlikely to embrace a single defi nition of the term belief was also generally agreed. So, it was asked, both explicitly and implicitly, should the fi eld settle for another option: to aim for a commonly accepted set of defi nitions of beliefs, depending on the setting, the context, and the audience being addressed? How realistic, and achievable, is a different, related, and persistently elusive goal: to use more refi ned Foreword vii and more comprehensive instruments to tap beliefs, use more careful terminology to describe and characterize beliefs, and distinguish beliefs from other terms often used virtually interchangeably? T he magnitude of this goal was sketched evocatively by one of the reviewers of Beliefs: A Hidden Variable in Mathematics Education? “The fi rst stumbling block,” wrote Mason (2004, p. 347) “is to work out what beliefs actually are, and where they fi t into an entire alphabet of associated inter- linked terms.” These he listed as: A is for attitudes, affect, aptitude, and aims; B is for beliefs; C is for constructs, concep- tions, and concerns; D is for demeanor and dispositions; E is for emotions, empathies, and expectations; F is for feelings; G is for goals and gatherings; H is for habits and habitus; I is for intentions, interests, and intuitions; J is for justifi cations and judgements; K is for knowing; L is for leanings; M is for meaning-to; N is for norms; O is for orientations and objectives; P is for propensities, perspectives, and predispositions; Q is for quirks and quid- dity; R is for recognitions and resonances; S is for sympathies and sensations; T is for ten- dencies and truths; U is for understandings and undertakings; V is for values and views; W is for wishes, warrants, words, and weltanschauung; X is for xenophilia (perhaps); Y is for yearnings and yens; and Z is for zeitgeist and zeal. (Mason 2004, p. 347) Whether written partly in jest or not, Mason’s creative beliefs-alphabet captured an important challenge faced by researchers and followers of the fi eld: the need to combat the loose and colloquial usage of the term belief and to adopt more con- strained terminology. W hat other directions were nominated in the book’s introductory chapters for further or more nuanced explorations? Beliefs are not observable but are typically inferred from observations, from responses to interview probes, or answers to ques- tionnaires. How can we be sure that the inferences we draw are accurate – a question that is posed frequently in the book and in the wider research community? Might other measures, physiological or neurological and/or laboratory based, provide new insights? How easy is it to make individuals aware of their beliefs about themselves and about mathematics, and how might these beliefs be changed if that is required for optimum mathematics learning? As noted by D. McLeod and S. McLeod (2002), the chapters in this section “are not the last word on attempts to defi ne beliefs…. Writers in the future will shape the research using terminology that meets their needs” (p. 119). That many have already done so is evident from the contributions in this and the other two parts of Leder et al. (2002). The extent to which later researchers have also adopted this pragmatic approach can be judged from inspection of the present, new volume, on research on beliefs and affect more broadly. Teachers’ Beliefs The synthesis of the second section, T eachers’ beliefs, read in part: A ll the authors see a cyclical relationship between changing beliefs and changing practices; wherever one starts they affect each other. … My reading of these chapters suggests that viii Foreword Chapman,1 Llinares, and Philippou and Christou come down on the side of changing beliefs leading to change in practices, Lloyd and Hart on the side of changing practices leading to changes in beliefs, and the review chapter by Wilson and Cooney remains with the dialec- tic…. The chapters provide rich evidence of changing practices in teaching mathematics as seen through a lens of discourse of beliefs as mental objects that are both cognitive and affective. They are constituted in teachers’ prior experiences and they need to become the subject of refl ection and analysis. A whole range of activities can bring about change towards reform and that change will come about as beliefs change… The picture is rightly a complex one. (Lerman 2002, p. 235) Exploring teachers’ beliefs and their development are important topics in themselves. How these beliefs affect their instructional strategies and in class behaviors were dominant themes addressed, in different ways, by the contribu- tors in this section. Typically, however, the proposed implications and applica- tions for constructive practice have been tentative and nebulous. To quote Wilson and Cooney (2002, p. 145): “understanding context and developing alternative explanations for phenomena require researchers to dig deeply. … The human condition is always beset with a strange mixture of rationality and irrationality that defy sharp lines of demarcation.” That interviews and observations provide insights into the ways in which teachers behave and make sense of their world is clearly acknowledged. But, and under what circumstances, is the information thus gained suffi cient to predict future actions? Much in this complex fi eld remains untilled. Welcome additional insights are provided in the present volume. E xperimental data, generally based on work with small samples, were reported in several chapters in this section. While instructive, such fi ndings are normally of questionable generalizability and unlikely to generate new theories that stretch the boundaries of the fi eld. What other avenues could be fruitful? An alternate, or perhaps more appropriately thought of as a complementary approach to the examination of changing classroom practices and the role of teach- ers’ beliefs in this process, was advocated by Lerman (2002). Instead of regarding beliefs and belief systems as psychological constructs, as, he argued, is done by the authors of the chapters in this section, and indeed often in research about beliefs and belief systems more widely, why not look “at the issue of changing practices in mathematics teaching and learning (by) applying a s ociological gaze rather than a psychological one” (p. 235, emphasis in the original). In parenthesis, it is worth not- ing that most of the authors of the work contained in the next section in the book “clearly acknowledge the tremendous infl uence socio-cultural context has on the formation of beliefs” (Lester 2002, p. 353). Readers of the current volume can themselves determine whether, and if so how, Lerman’s challenge is taken up in the current publication. 1 D etails of the work of the authors mentioned in this quotation can be found in Beliefs: A hidden variable in mathematics education? They are not detailed in the reference list. Foreword ix Students’ Beliefs R esearch on students’ beliefs is relatively new. “It is only during the past 15–20 years that beliefs research has come to be viewed as an essential ingredient in mathemat- ics education research programs” according to Lester (2002, p. 346). Core issues investigated by the contributors to the fi nal section of Leder et al. (2002) were sum- marized by Lester as follows: (cid:129) Do students’ beliefs infl uence their interest in and motivation to learn mathematics? (cid:129) Why don’t students worry about whether their solutions to word problems make sense? (cid:129) Do students’ beliefs infl uence their ability to connect real-world and school mathematics? (cid:129) How do classrooms infl uence the development of beliefs? (cid:129) How do students’ intuitive beliefs about mathematical operations affect their thinking processes? O nce again a multiplicity of methods and settings were adopted to explore students’ beliefs about mathematics and the learning of mathematics. When included, experimental data were generally again confi ned to small samples. Inevitably, there were shortcomings in the various studies reported: the scope of instruments used to gather students’ beliefs was necessarily constrained, and the need to infer beliefs from students’ answers or actions remained a hurdle. At the time, and also with hindsight, the fi ndings reported – singly and collec- tively – were, and are, more appropriately considered as markers of progress in the fi eld than as end points of research. Lester’s (2002) concluding comments were indeed apt. I am skeptical (he wrote) about the credibility and reliability of the data presented by these fi ve reports because I doubt that they accurately indicate what the students really believe…. I do not think most students really think much about what they believe about mathematics and as a result are not very aware of their beliefs. So, although I think these researchers are leading the way in efforts to develop good methodological tools for studying students’ beliefs, a considerable amount of work remains to be done. (p. 353) Building on the Foundations S ome years ago, Freud (1940/1986, p. 286) wrote: “the concept of the unconscious has long been knocking at the gates of psychology and asking to be let in. Philosophy and literature have often toyed with it, but science could fi nd no use for it.” This contention is now heavily challenged, and indeed refuted, by the active and sus- tained research agenda on the interaction between affect and the teaching and learn- ing of mathematics, spawned in the later part of the twentieth century and continuing unabated in the twenty-fi rst century. Despite this activity, there continues to be room

Description:
This book connects seminal work in affect research and moves forward to provide a developing perspective on affect as the “decisive variable” of the mathematics classroom. In particular, the book contributes and investigates new conceptual frameworks and new methodological ‘tools’ in affect
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.