This section presents contemporary commentary on articles previously published in English Teaching Forum. E n g l i s h T E a c h i n g F o r u m | Number 2 2012 21 From Unity to Diversity… to Diversity within Unity BY DIANE LARSEN-FREEMAN For the 25th anniversary issue of English Teaching Forum, theme by pointing to the shifts that had taken place in each published in 1987, I wrote about the diversification of the of the angles of the triangle. In the learners/learning angle, language teaching field. My point then was that during the where at one time habit formation was the dominant view preceding years from 1962 to 1987, the language teaching of language learning, in subsequent years several compet- field had diversified: Where earlier there had been a unified ing theories were proposed to explain language acquisition: approach to language teaching, by 1987, many options exist- innatism, cognitivism (initially, rule formation and, later, ed. Today, as I write on the occasion of the 50th anniversary the setting of parameters on principles of universal gram- of Forum, I perceive a different pattern. I see diversity within mar), imitation of frequently occurring sequences of words, unity, not as an alternative to it. I begin this article by briefly and interactionism. In the language angle, I pointed to the summarizing the main theme of my 1987 article. Next, I variety of syllabus types that were being implemented. turn to the triangle that I used then to depict the language While structural syllabi still seemed commonplace, there teaching field. In each of the three angles of the triangle, I were other options: notional-functional, topical, situational, placed the major categories of language/culture, language procedural, competency-based, text-based, content-based learners/learning, and language teachers/teaching. syllabi, or some combination of these. In the teachers/teach- ing angle, I noted the diversity of methods that were being practiced in 1987. To represent the diversity, I singled out five “innovative methods”: the Silent Way, Suggestopedia, Community Language Learning, the Comprehension Ap- proach, and the Communicative Approach. I made the point that at the time none of them had dominated the field to the degree that Audiolingualism had before them. I explored other facets of the field as well. All this variety, which I have just selectively recounted, I proposed to support my theme that the language teaching field had moved from unity to diversity. In the present article, In this article, I revisit each of the angles, making ob- my theme is different. I believe that diversity in our field servations that support my contention that today there is remains, but it has a different relationship to unity. The diversity within unity. I conclude by stepping back from diversity is not in opposition to the unity, but rather can be this analysis of the field and by stating why I feel that the located within it. To elaborate on this new theme, I revisit theme that I have chosen for my article in this 50th anni- the three angles of the triangle. versary issue is important to us all. Language/Culture Then and Now Language has been seen to be a rule-governed system. In 1987, the major theme of my article was that the While this view of language remains, more recently, many language teaching field had gone from a period of unity more linguists have subscribed to a view of language as a in 1962 to a period of diversity in 1987. I supported this pastiche of patterns. Some are socially situated sentences or 22 2012 Number 2 | E n g l i s h T E a c h i n g F o r u m utterances, such as the following: “Can I come in?” “Are unitary entity, it is now theorized that one’s language re- you ready?” “He’s busy right now.” “Have some more.” sources are composed of thousands of language chunks: Others are lexicalized sentence stems, which start off with phrases, lexicalized sentence stems, sentences/utterances, a more or less fixed pattern, but which are open ended, such which can be arrayed along a continuum of fixity to flex- as “I am writing/emailing/calling to say….” Still others ibility. Or, in other words, while we still might speak of “the are entirely phrasal, such as “of course” and “as a matter of English language” as if it were a unified thing, we see from fact.” While some of these are accounted for by grammati- closer inspection that it might be better to conceive of a lan- cal rules, not all are; for example, the phrase “by and large,” guage as affording its speakers a variety of forms with which where a preposition is followed by a conjunction, and then to construct meaning in a way appropriate to their purposes. an adjective, is not the product of English rules of grammar. In this vein we speak of a speaker’s (heterogeneous) language Speakers of a given language have a repertoire of such lan- resources rather than a homogeneous internal grammar. guage patterns or chunks, numbering in the tens of thou- We also acknowledge that speakers’ language resources sands (Pawley and Syder 1983). As the examples indicate, are much more mutable and individual than previously some of these patterns are fixed, but many also allow for thought as a result of each speaker’s unique ongoing experi- variation. Knowledge of these many patterns constitutes a ence in using language. In other words, speakers’ language speaker’s language resources and allows the speaker to pro- resources are stable, but they are always open to change. Of duce and understand language fluently in real time. course, there is enough overlap among the resources of vari- This characterization of language has been bolstered ous speakers to achieve mutual intelligibility; nonetheless, by access to the large digitized corpora that exist these days. speakers have their own unique resources or idiolects. The Certainly, in the past, linguists have made use of collec- enactment of their resources is a dynamic process, one that tions of language utterances. However, using computers to has been termed “grammaring” (Larsen-Freeman 2003) or store and search vast databases makes it possible to discover “languaging” (Swain 2006). and investigate patterns, which traditional grammars and The use of corpora has also highlighted the importance descriptions of language may have overlooked. In so doing, of culture. When we focus on language in use, rather than corpus linguists have uncovered the largely phraseological language as an abstract formal system, we see it rooted in nature of English. Mining the data of electronic corpora the context and culture of the local speech community to shows us common collocations and their “semantic proso- which the participants belong. Given the increasing social dy” (Sinclair 1991) or the negative or positive associations and economic mobility of many people these days, English with such patterns. For example, corpus analysis reveals has become an international lingua franca that is not that when the English word border is followed by on, the really owned by any one group of speakers. Within a global- phrase “bordered/bordering on” can have a geographic ref- ized community, then, any attempt to establish one model erence, but it is also used often in reporting an undesirable of English as the “target,” be it a native variety or an in- state, e.g., “bordering on arrogance” (Schmitt 2005). Step- digenized variety (Indian English, Nigerian English, etc.), ping back from the individual patterns, we become aware is problematic (Jenkins 2000; Seidlhofer 2001). Certainly, of how much of our language use is conventionalized. By within a multilingual context, in which English is the lin- this, I mean that grammar rules may generate an infinite gua franca among non-native speakers, standard norms are number of sentences, but only a few of the combinations becoming less relevant (Wei 2010). are actually used. For example, it would be customary to propose marriage by asking your beloved “Will you mar- Attempting to define stable ELF [English as a lin- ry me?” You would not likely say “I desire you to become gua franca] models, whether linguistic or cultural, married to me.” And, although perfectly grammatical, the with which to replace the traditional ‘native’ mod- probability of your proposal being successful would likely el, seems to be going against the richness of EIL diminish if you were to say “Your becoming my spouse is [English as an international language] in a global- what I want” (Pawley and Syder 1983)! ized community, characterized by infinite variety. The shift from conceiving of language as an internal Lingua-cultural identities seem to be increasingly rule-governed system to language as patterns-in-use has multiple rather than single and unitary….” (Pro- had profound consequences. Language is not produced by dromou 2008, 13) simply filling in the slots of a syntactic pattern with lexical items. Lexis and grammar are intertwined. Where once it To summarize so far, gone is the notion of a homogenized was thought that language competence was a homogenized language competence and a monocultural identity. In its E n g l i s h T E a c h i n g F o r u m | Number 2 2012 23 place is the recognition that one speaker’s resources overlap While of course there are some commonalities among with others, but they are also distinctive. In other words, learners, for example, that a learner’s language background within unity, there is diversity. influences the way he or she thinks and speaks a language (Slobin 1996), it is also the case that each learner maps Learners/Learning the L2 learning territory somewhat differently. Indeed, Now moving on to the second angle—that of learners researching the developing patterns of learner language and learning—it is fair to say that the population of lan- makes it clear that learners chart their own paths (Lars- guage learners is also not as homogeneous as it once was. en-Freeman 2006). For this reason, I have suggested that First of all, today there are far more second language users we should bear in mind that we are not just teaching lan- of English than there are native speakers, and the former guage; we are teaching learners (Larsen-Freeman 2003). number is expanding (Graddol 2006). This surge in the Moving on to the category of learning in the second number of English learners can be attributed to the status angle, given what I have written above about the patterned of English as an international language, and it has been fu- nature of a speaker’s language resources, it follows then that eled by the aforementioned mobility of populations around language acquisition is not seen to be a process of acquir- the world in search of jobs or better living conditions. It is ing rules, which then get applied, but as the emergence of also attributable to the fact that parents want their children variegated language resources from interrelated patterns of to have opportunities that they believe a knowledge of Eng- experience, social interaction, and cognitive mechanisms, lish will provide. Parents’ aspirations for their children and mechanisms such as noticing and remembering. the desire of ministries of education for a populace that can Thus, instead of seeing language learning as solely compete on the world stage have contributed to the tendency being a process of hypothesis testing and revision, as be- to begin English language instruction at earlier and earlier fits a rule formation view of language, patterns are said to ages. Then, too, in English-speaking countries, greater dis- emerge as learners use the language (in a bottom-up fash- persion of immigrant populations from cities into suburbs ion). The social dimension, which had been minimized in and the countryside has meant that many classroom teach- previous accounts of language learning, occupies its right- ers, not necessarily ones educated to teach English, have, for ful place from the perspective of emergentism. the first time, large numbers of English language learners in As speakers interact, they co-adapt; they adjust their their classrooms. At a further point along the age spectrum, language to each other. Language is a complex adaptive increasing numbers of international students are pursuing system (Ellis and Larsen-Freeman 2006; 2009). Through higher education in English-speaking countries. language use and co-adaption among speakers, frequently However, language learners differ not only in age, but occurring patterns get noticed, remembered, and incorpo- also along a number of dimensions. Where at one time, indi- rated into learners’ language resources. The exposure and vidual difference research was confined to investigating such opportunity to use formulae in interaction are key compo- obvious factors as language aptitude and learner motivation, nents in their acquisition (Wray 2002). Of course, exposure now individual differences, hypothesized to account for dif- and opportunity will vary from learning environment to ferential success among learners, number over one hundred. learning environment, accounting for language variation Even a general trait such as “intelligence” is seen as multiple among speakers. Indeed, when we entertain a view of lan- (Gardner 1999). The same holds for language learners’ vari- guage as a complex adaptive system, we recognize that ev- ous multiple identities: as a man or woman, adult or child, ery use of language changes the language resources of the monolingual or bilingual, etc. It is clear that each learner learner/user. The changed resources are then available for is unique. Such an observation underscores a critical ques- other members of the speech community to adopt and to tion in the field of language learning and language educa- adapt (Larsen-Freeman and Cameron 2008). tion: To what extent is it possible to make generalizations It would not be in keeping with my theme, however, about learners apart from the circumstances of, and reason to nominate emergentism as the only means of acquiring for, their learning? As Kramsch (2002, 4) has put it: another language. Certainly, with language as complex as it is, it is doubtful that there is a single means of learning It is no longer sufficient to talk about “individual it, and this is surely the case, especially for older learners, differences” in SLA [second language acquisition] who likely rely more on explicit declarative knowledge of against the backdrop of the universal learner. Dif- the language than what results from reliance on an implic- ference and variation itself have moved to the cen- it process such as the one I have just described. Thus, any ter of language acquisition research. view of language learning will have to acknowledge the 24 2012 Number 2 | E n g l i s h T E a c h i n g F o r u m contribution of multiple processes (e.g., Gagné and Med- principles. They also contribute to a professional discourse sker 1996). in which we all may engage (Freeman 1991); they challenge teachers to think in new ways; and they provide associated Teachers/Teaching techniques with which teachers can experiment to come to When it comes to explicit language teaching, the pic- new understandings (Larsen-Freeman and Anderson 2011). ture has also changed. Moving on to the last angle of the tri- I should note that critical theorists (e.g., Pennycook angle, then, it can be said that today there is a greater aware- 2001) worry that we will assume that “one size fits all” and ness of teachers as multidimensional beings themselves. inappropriately impose language teaching methods from First of all, teachers have their own individual experiences developed countries onto educators in developing coun- with learning languages to draw on. Then, too, teachers are tries. Of course, we should avoid such imposition; however, learners themselves. Indeed, teacher learning has become a there is abundant evidence that teachers’ classroom practic- major subarea of the field, with much research being con- es are highly individual and durable (Larsen-Freeman and ducted on how teachers learn to teach (Tedick 2005; Burns Freeman 2008). Such practices do not change simply on and Richards 2009). How teachers develop in their under- the basis of interventions of power instituted from the top standing of teaching is viewed less as their applying theo- down. As Widdowson (2004, 369) observes, what is needed ries from related disciplines and more as their constructing is a “shift to localization,” in which pedagogic practices are their own understandings by participating in “communi- designed in relation to local contexts, needs, and objectives. ties of practice” (Lave and Wenger 1991) with other teach- ers and formulating their own theories through such inter- For it is clear that universal solutions that are actions, filtered through their own experiences (Johnson transposed acritically, often accompanied by calls 2006). Because of the situated nature of these local theories, for increased standardization, and that ignore in- it is recognized that teachers’ views of teaching may vary digenous conditions, the diversity of learners, and considerably from one teacher to another. What is impor- the agency of teachers, are immanent in a modern- tant is for teachers to have their own sense of plausibility ism that no longer applies, if it ever did. (Larsen- (Prabhu 1990), their own understanding of why they do Freeman and Freeman 2008, 168) what they do. After all, teachers need some basis for mak- ing the decisions that they make from moment to moment Evidence for the impact of local conditions, the diver- in their classrooms. Of course, this means that teachers, too, sity of learners, and the agency of teachers lies in the fact cannot be seen categorically, but rather they should be seen that a method is enacted differently from context to con- as individuals with their own teaching practices forged in text, and indeed from teacher to teacher. This is no doubt and tailored to the local context. in part due to a teacher’s understanding; but it also differs Next, I turn to teaching. In the 1987 article, I wrote of with the learners being taught, their purpose for learning, the many language teaching methods that existed. These and the context of instruction. As with language, methods flourished in the 1970s and 1980s due to the challenge are dynamically adaptable in use. This has always been the to behaviorism, and hence to the Audiolingual method. case, of course. Anyone who has visited classes (even a few) While the Audiolingual method continues to be practiced in which teachers profess to be practicing communicative in many parts of the world today, the concept of “method” language teaching will attest to the fact that what is tak- itself has been challenged. It has been said that language ing place in such classrooms is very different, one from an- teaching is in a “post-method” phase (Kumaravadivelu other. Thus, any methodologist should anticipate, indeed 2006). However, I think that not only is the term “method” encourage, local adaptation. Perhaps the worth of a method in language teaching and language teacher education firm- should, in part, be a measure of this—how easily adaptable ly established, but I also believe that teachers need knowl- it is (Larsen-Freeman and Cameron 2008). edge of various methods. Methods are not intact packages Then, too, with innovations in technology and the de- of teaching practices imposed from above, but rather are co- velopment of increasingly fast and widely accessible Inter- herent sets of thinking-in-action links available for teach- net connections, there is more diversity in where language ers to interact with and learn from. Such investigations are learning is taking place. Language learning has increasingly vital to language teaching and to teachers’ defining their moved out of the classroom and into the computer lab, the own sense of plausibility. When methods are seen as sets Internet café, and even one’s home. Voice over IP software, of coherent principles that link to practice, they help act as such as Skype, has made it possible for international con- a foil whereby teachers can clarify their own pedagogical nections to be made and language learners/users to meet E n g l i s h T E a c h i n g F o r u m | Number 2 2012 25 virtually to discuss issues of common interest. This type Further, I take the diversity within unity to be im- of interaction, which no doubt benefits language learning, portant to us all. For diversity within unity is character- is occurring both informally and formally, the latter as a istic of natural systems: our strength lies in our diversity. supplement to in-class instruction. For instance Page (2007) shows how a decision-making Amidst the talk of all this dynamism and diversity, process inclusive of diversity can create better decisions one could reasonably point to the standards movement as and hence a better society. Diversity within unity also a counterforce. highlights our uniqueness. In addition, it recognizes that there is no common endpoint arrived at by all language The standards movement has taken hold in many learners. Diversity within unity allows us to adapt to a parts of the world and promotes the adoption of rapidly changing world, and diversity serves as a crucible clear statements of instructional outcomes in edu- in which change takes place. Learners/speakers of a lan- cational programs as a way of improving learning guage actively transform their linguistic world; they do outcomes in programs and to provide guidelines not conform to it. And this, we (native and non-natives, for program development, curriculum develop- children and adults, monolinguals and bilinguals, teach- ment, and assessment. (Richards 2008, 172) ers and students) do together. While no one could argue against improving learning References outcomes, I think that the standards movement is partially Burns, A., and J. C. Richards, eds. 2009. The Cambridge guide propelled by a reaction to all the dynamism and change to second language teacher education. Cambridge: Cam- and flux in the field. It is the educational establishment’s bridge University Press. attempt to bring things back under control. Canagarajah, A. S. 2006. TESOL at forty: What are the is- sues? TESOL Quarterly 40 (1): 9–34. The importance of diversity within unity Ellis, N. C., and D. Larsen-Freeman. 2006. Language emer- At this time, which might be called a period of “post- gence: Implications for applied linguistics. Applied Lin- modern globalization” (Canagarajah 2006), I have tried to guistics 27 (4): 558–89. highlight a pattern I perceive in the language teaching field, Ellis, N. C., with D. Larsen-Freeman. 2009. Constructing a namely, diversity within unity. Accompanying the shift of second language: Analyses and computational simula- perspective on language from static homogeneity to dynam- tions of the emergence of linguistic constructions from ic heterogeneity, and the trends of population mobility, hy- usage. Language Learning, Supplement 1: 93–128. bridity of communicative contexts, localization of practices, Freeman, D. 1991. Mistaken constructs: Re-examining the access that technology brings, and recognition of people’s nature and assumptions of language teacher educa- multiple identities is the fact that our theoretical constructs tion. In Georgetown University round table on languages no longer fit into closed boxes. Indeed, my use of the trian- and linguistics, ed. J. E. Alatis, 25–39. Washington, DC: gle 25 years ago is no longer suitable today, for I would be Georgetown University Press. the first to admit that putting aspects of the field into one Gagné, R., and K. Medsker. 1996. The conditions of learning. or other triangle or category within a triangle implies that Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace. they are discrete and that the boundaries between them are Gardner, H. 1999. Intelligence reframed. Multiple intelligences not porous. But even a little reflection will show that this is for the 21st century. New York: Basic Books. not the case. From a complex adaptive system perspective on Graddol, D. 2006. English next. British Council. language, for instance, it is difficult to distinguish theoreti- Jenkins, J. 2000. The phonology of English as an international cally between language learning and language use. language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. I also recognize that, by being selective, I have not dealt Johnson, K. 2006. The socio-cultural turn and its challeng- with significant areas of the field, such as the language skills es for L2 education. TESOL Quarterly 40 (1): 235–57. of reading, writing, speaking, and listening, each clearly Kramsch, C., ed. 2002. Language acquisition and language so- worthy of its own article. However, given the limitations of cialization: Ecological perspectives. London: Continuum. what can be accomplished in one article, what I have taken Kumaravadivelu, B. 2006. TESOL methods: Changing up is consistent with my assertion that the pattern that tracks, challenging trends. TESOL Quarterly 40 (1): characterizes the field may no longer be (if it ever was) a 59–81. reciprocal swing from unity to diversity, but rather one of Larsen-Freeman, D. 2003. Teaching language: From grammar diversity within a more generalized form of unity. to grammaring. Boston: Heinle/Cengage. 26 2012 Number 2 | E n g l i s h T E a c h i n g F o r u m Larsen-Freeman, D. 2006. The emergence of complexity, norms, ed. M. Cruz-Ferreira, 397–404. Frankfurt am fluency, and accuracy in the oral and written produc- Main: Peter Lang. tion of five Chinese learners of English. Applied Lin- Widdowson, H. G. 2004. A perspective on recent trends. In guistics 27 (4): 590–619. A history of English language teaching. 2nd ed., ed. A. P. Larsen-Freeman, D., and M. Anderson. 2011. Techniques and R. Howatt with H. G. Widdowson, 397–404. Oxford: principles in language teaching. 3rd ed. Oxford: Oxford Oxford University Press. University Press. Wray, A. 2002. Formulaic language and the lexicon. Cam- Larsen-Freeman, D., and L. Cameron. 2008. Complex systems bridge: Cambridge University Press. in applied linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Larsen-Freeman, D., and D. Freeman. 2008. Language moves: The place of “foreign” languages in classroom Diane Larsen-Freeman is a Professor of Education and language and teaching. Review of Research in Education Professor of Linguistics and a Research Scientist at the 32: 147–86. English Language Institute, University of Michigan. Lave, J., and E. Wenger. 1991. Situated learning: Legitimate She is also a Distinguished Senior Faculty Fellow at peripheral participation. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer- the School for International Training and has been a conference speaker in over 60 countries around the world. sity Press. Her latest book is the third edition of Techniques and Page, S. 2007. The difference: How the power of diversity creates Principles in Language Teaching, co-authored with Marti better groups, firms, schools, and societies. Princeton, NJ: Anderson. In 2011, Dr. Larsen-Freeman was presented Princeton University Press. the Distinguished Scholarship and Service Award by the Pawley, A., and F. Syder. 1983. Two puzzles for linguistic American Association for Applied Linguistics. theory: Nativelike selection and nativelike fluency. In Language and communication, ed. J. Richards and R. Schmidt, 191–226. London: Longman. Pennycook, A. 2001. Critical applied linguistics: A critical in- troduction. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Prabhu, N. S. 1990. There is no best method—Why? TE- SOL Quarterly 24 (2): 161–76. Prodromou, L. 2008. English as a lingua franca: A corpus- based analysis. London: Continuum. Richards, J. 2008. Second language teacher education today. RELC Journal 39 (2): 158–77. Schmitt, N. 2005. Grammar: rules or patterning? Applied Linguistics Forum 26 (2): 1–2. Seidlhofer, B. 2001. Closing a conceptual gap: The case for a description of English as a lingua franca. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 11 (2): 133–58. Sinclair, J. 1991. Corpus, concordance, collocation. Oxford: Ox- ford University Press. Slobin, D. 1996. From “thought and language” to “think- ing for speaking.” In Rethinking linguistic relativity, ed. J. Gumperz and S. Levinson, 70–96. New York: Cam- bridge University Press. Swain, M. 2006. Languaging, agency and collaboration in advanced language proficiency. In Advanced language learning: The contribution of Halliday and Vygotsky, ed. H. Byrnes, 95–108. London: Continuum. Tedick, D., ed. 2005. Second language teacher education: Inter- national perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Wei, Li. 2010. The nature of linguistic norms and their rel- evance to multilingual development. In Multilingual E n g l i s h T E a c h i n g F o r u m | Number 2 2012 27