ebook img

ERIC EJ899108: Assessing the Value of Choice in a Token System PDF

2010·0.13 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ899108: Assessing the Value of Choice in a Token System

JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2010, 43, 553–557 NUMBER3 (FALL2010) ASSESSING THE VALUE OF CHOICE IN A TOKEN SYSTEM SANDEEP K. SRAN UNIVERSITYOFTHEPACIFIC AND JOHN C. BORRERO UNIVERSITYOFMARYLAND,BALTIMORECOUNTY Responding of 4 children was assessed under conditions in which (a) no programmed contingencieswerearrangedfortargetbehavior,(b)respondingproducedtokensthatcouldbe exchanged for a single highly preferred edible item, and (c) responding produced a token that could be exchanged for a variety of preferred edible items. After assessing the effects of these contingencies,thepreferencesof3participantswereassessedusingaconcurrent-chainsschedule. Preference for the opportunity to choose from the same or qualitatively different edible items varied across participants, and findings were generally consistent with those of Tiger, Hanley, andHernandez(2006). Keywords: choice, concurrent chains,preference, token reinforcement _______________________________________________________________________________ In a series of studies, Tiger, Hanley, and descriptive rules associated with the various Hernandez (2006) assessed children’s academic options (e.g., ‘‘If you pick this option you can performance during conditions in which they pick one of five identical edible items; if you could obtain one highly preferred edible item pick that option, you receive this edible item’’). contingent on task completion (no choice) or Although practical for academic settings and select one identicaledible item from an array of very young children, the incorporation of rules five identical edible items (choice). These makesitmoredifficulttodistinguishcontrolby conditions were then compared to a condition experimentallyprogrammedcontingenciesfrom in which no edible items were arranged to control by rules (Skinner, 1953). Further, control for the simple presentation of edible completion of the response requirement in items. Tiger et al. evaluated the opportunity to Tiger et al.’s study resulted in immediate access choose using a concurrent-chains evaluation to the item. In application, this may not be the (e.g., Luczynski & Hanley, 2009), and results most practical arrangement. An alternative showed that some children preferred the strategy might involve the presentation of opportunity to choose and others did not. tokens,whichhavebeenshowntobridgedelays However, in the evaluations described by Tiger between completion of the response require- et al., experimenters presented children with ment and receipt of primary or backup reinforcers. Thisexperimentwascompletedinpartialfulfillmentof The purpose of the current study was to therequirementsoftheMasterofArtsdegreebythefirst conduct a systematic replication of the proce- authorfrom the University of the Pacific. We thank Ken BeauchampandFloydO’Brienfortheircommentsonan dures described by Tiger et al. (2006) in the earlier version of this manuscript. In addition, we thank context of a token system. The study was members of the behavior analysis laboratory at the designed to determine if the opportunity to University of the Pacific for their assistance with data collectionanddata preparation. choose backup reinforcers in a token system DirectcorrespondencetoJohnC.Borrero,Department would influence response rates associated with of Psychology, University Maryland, Baltimore County, academic tasks and children’s preferences for Baltimore,Maryland21250(e-mail:[email protected]). doi:10.1901/jaba.2010.43-553 arrangements in which the opportunity to 553 554 SANDEEP K. SRAN and JOHN C. BORRERO choose was or was not presented. Participants ratio to a percentage. These percentages were werenotgivenanyverbalinstructionsaboutthe averaged to generate session agreement. Inter- different conditions. Rather, nonverbal sched- observeragreementwasassessedforatleast32% ule-correlated stimuli were associated with the of sessions across all conditions and exceeded various operating contingencies. In this way, 80% for all participants. performance was based on contact with the Procedure arranged contingencies. During all sessions, the target response consisted of tracing numbers and uppercase METHOD andlowercaseletterswithapencil.Priortoeach Participants, Setting, and Materials session, the experimenter gave participants the Fourtypicallydevelopingchildren(all4years following instruction: ‘‘Here are some letters old) enrolled in preschool participated in this and numbers to trace. You can do as many as study. Dylan, Mira, Milo, and Luke participat- youwant,asfewasyouwant,ornoneatall.’’In ed in the preference (Fisher et al., 1992) and addition, during the first exposure to each reinforcer assessments; however, Dylan did not condition, the experimenter implemented a participate in the concurrent-chains evaluation. three-prompt instructional sequence when a Experimental sessions were conducted at each participant did not emit a response in the first participant’s preschool in an available space in 10softhefirstsession.Attheendofthe3-min the classroom. session, the experimenter removed the work- Session materials included academic work- sheet and said, ‘‘Okay, you’re finished.’’ sheets (letters and numbers that could be The reinforcer assessment was conducted traced), colored pencils, poker chips, and a using a combined reversal and multielement smock worn by the experimenter. Each of the design. The baseline condition (A) was intro- items was one of four different colors (see duced in the first phase. The second phase below). The colors were included to enhance consisted of a multielement arrangement of the discrimination. Trained undergraduate or grad- no-choice condition (B), the single-choice uate students acted as the experimenter for all condition (C), and the varied-choice condition the sessions. (D). During baseline, participants worked with All sessions lasted 3 min, and data were a green worksheet. Participants received no collected on the target response, delivery of tokens during these sessions. Instructions were tokens, and condition selection (when partici- provided to participants at the beginning and pants had the opportunity to choose). Approx- end of the session, as described previously. imately three sessions were conducted per day, Prior to the introduction of the token and data were collected manually or with conditions, the experimenter gave the partici- handheld computers. pant two to four tokens noncontingently and asked him or her to hand the experimenter one Response Measurement and tokenatatime.Eachtokenexchangeresultedin Interobserver Agreement one bite of the food. When all tokens were Interobserver agreement was obtained for at exchanged, the session commenced. During least 25% of sessions across all conditions. The sessions, the experimenter placed tokens in a partial agreement within intervals method was small receptacle positioned next to the work- used to determine interobserver agreement, sheets. which was calculated by dividing the smaller For the no-choice condition, participants response frequency by the larger response worked with a red worksheet and received a frequency per 10-s interval, and converting the tokenaccordingtoafixed-ratio(FR)5schedule VALUE OF CHOICE 555 (i.e., the token-production schedule). After Each pile of like-colored worksheets consisted completion of each 3-min session, the partici- of schedule-correlated stimuli in the initial link pant exchanged his or her tokens for only one of the chains and was arranged in a row, with type of reinforcer identified as the most each worksheet equidistant from the other. The preferreditemviaapriorpreferenceassessment. colors corresponded to the same conditions to The item was put on one plate, and each time whichparticipantshadbeenexposedduringthe one token was exchanged for a reinforcer, the reinforcer assessment. The experimenter in- plate was replenished with the same item until structedtheparticipanttochoosetheworksheet the participant had exchanged all of the tokens. by saying, ‘‘Go to the table and pick one color In the single-choice condition, the participant you would like to work with.’’ The experi- workedwithayellowworksheet,andthetoken- menter changed the placement of the colored production and token-exchange schedules were items on the table before the start of the next the same as those described in the no-choice sessionandgavetheparticipanttheopportunity condition. During token exchange, the experi- to make another initial-link response. menter arranged five identical edible items on a Afteraparticipantchosetheworksheetcolor, plate (e.g., fruit snacks). The same highly the experimenter put on a colored smock that matched the selected worksheet, and a 3-min preferred item chosen for the no-choice condi- session commenced. During the token-ex- tion was used. The experimenter gave the change period, programmed consequences for participant the opportunity to choose one item. the terminal link were available (i.e., no choice, Although we attempted to ensure that all single choice, varied choice). The experimenter stimuli were identical in the single-choice presented three opportunities to make initial- condition, it is possible that slight differences link color selections each day, except for Luke, in size, color, or shape may have influenced who received only one or two opportunities to responding.Aftertheparticipantchoseanitem, make a selection (due to unavailability). theexperimenterreplenishedtheplatesuchthat the same five items remained on the plate at all times. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION In the varied-choice condition, the participant Figure 1 depicts response rates during the worked with a blue worksheet, and the token- reinforcer assessment for all participants. These production and token-exchange schedules were data represent response rates for situations in the same as those described in the single-choice which the experimenter selected particular condition. During token exchange, the partic- conditions (e.g., no choice, varied choice). ipant could exchange each token for one item When reinforcers were arranged for target from the five most highly preferred items (e.g., behavior in the no-choice, single-choice, and one fruit snack, one piece of candy, one cookie, varied-choice conditions, responding increased one piece of chocolate, one marshmallow). The relative to rates obtained in the prior baseline experimenter placed each item on a plate and condition. Dylan’s performance in the no- replenished the item if the participant chose it choice condition was the exception. When during the token exchange. These three condi- compared to the no-reinforcement baseline tionswerepresentedonceeachdayinarandom condition (M 5 0.44 responses per minute), order.AfterthemultielementB-C-Dcondition, no change in mean level of responding was a reversal to baseline (A) was introduced. observed in the last three sessions of the no- During the concurrent-chains evaluation, choice condition (M 5 0.44); this suggested each participant was permitted to select a that there was no reinforcement effect when condition as described by Tiger et al. (2006). edible items were available but he could not 556 SANDEEP K. SRAN and JOHN C. BORRERO Figure 2. Mean percentage of selections for Mira, Milo, and Luke during the concurrent-chains evaluation. Althoughnotdepictedinthefigure,participantswerealso giventheopportunitytoselectthebaselinecondition(this never occurred). Figure 1. Results of the reinforcer assessment for all participants. Response rates are depicted by the open respectively.Resultsindicatedpreferenceforthe triangles for the no-choice condition, open circles for the varied-choice condition (42% of selections) for single-choice condition, and open squares for the varied- Mira, a strong preference for the varied-choice choicecondition. condition (81% of selections) for Milo, and a choose the item. Following the reinforcement modest preference for the varied-choice condi- condition, baseline was reintroduced, and tion (39% of selections) for Luke. response rates for all participants decreased to Results of the present study replicate those low levels, particularly during the last three reported by Tiger et al. (2006), in that sessions of the condition. preference for the opportunity to choose varied Figure 2 depicts the percentage of selections across participants. The findings of Tiger et al. for each of the conditions available, for all appear to be sufficiently robust to the extent participants who completed the concurrent- that findings of the current study were similar chains evaluation. Mira, Milo, and Luke were andwereobtainedusingtokenreinforcersinthe given 24, 21, and 24 choice opportunities, absence of rules. In addition, Tiger et al. VALUE OF CHOICE 557 focused exclusively on participant selections for down, or both) to assess how reinforcer rate various initial- and terminal-link options. This might affect preference. is useful information as it relates to preference. Choice could be a simple classroom alter- However,ofsimilarimportanceisasenseofthe ation designed to improve the performance of amount of work (e.g., response rate) that is some students (Romaniuk et al., 2002). Future completed under the various conditions. studies could assess the practical implications of One limitation of the present study is choice by investigating the effects of parametric apparent in the number of opportunities to increases in work requirements on preference choose. Preference for one condition may have foravarietyofchoice(orno-choice)conditions. been more discernible given more than three opportunities to select a condition. Another REFERENCES limitation involves the absence of a clear evaluation of the role of the tokens. In the Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison presentstudy,tokenswereneverremoved;thus, of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for definitive conclusions regarding the role of personswithsevereandprofounddisabilities.Journal tokens cannot be reached (Foster & Hacken- of AppliedBehaviorAnalysis,25,491–498. berg, 2004). Future research may involve Foster, T. A., & Hackenberg, T. D. (2004). Unit price and choice inatoken-reinforcement context. Journal procedures similar to those described in the of theExperimental Analysisof Behavior, 81,5–25. present study, but should include token-with- Luczynski, K. C., & Hanley, G. P. (2009). Do children drawal conditions. prefer contingencies? Anevaluation ofthe efficacy of and preference for contingent versus noncontingent A third limitation of the current study may social reinforcement during play. Journal of Applied be found in the reinforcement schedule. An FR Behavior Analysis,42,511–525. 5 schedule was in place that necessarily Romaniuk,C.,Miltenberger,R.,Conyers,C.,Jenner,N., decreased reinforcement rate compared to the Jurgens,M.,&Ringenberg,C.(2002).Theinfluence of activity choice on problem behaviors maintained common FR 1 schedule implemented in byescapeversusattention.JournalofAppliedBehavior preference and reinforcer assessment research. Analysis,35,349–362. It is possible that an FR 1 schedule would have Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. established higher rates of responding in one Oxford, UK:Macmillan. Tiger,J.H.,Hanley,G.P.,&Hernandez,E.(2006).An condition than another, thereby exaggerating evaluation of the value of choice with preschool preference. However, an FR 5 schedule was children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, used to reduce the possibility of satiation. 1–16. Future research may involve decreasing session Received March 26,2008 duration to control reinforcement rate or Final acceptanceJuly 2,2008 titrating the response requirement (either up, Action Editor,James Carr

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.