JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2009, 42, 609–625 NUMBER3 (FALL2009) CONSUMPTION ANDRESPONSEOUTPUT ASA FUNCTIONOF UNIT PRICE: MANIPULATION OF COST AND BENEFIT COMPONENTS XERES DELMENDO, JOHN C. BORRERO, KENNETH L. BEAUCHAMP, AND MONICA T. FRANCISCO UNIVERSITYOFTHEPACIFIC Weconductedpreferenceassessmentswith4typicallydevelopingchildrentoidentifypotential reinforcersandassessedthereinforcingefficacyofthosestimuli.Next,wetestedtwopredictions ofeconomictheory:thatoverallconsumption(reinforcersobtained)woulddecreaseastheunit price(responserequirementperreinforcer)increasedandthatthecostandbenefitcomponents that defined unit price would not influence overall consumption considerably when unit price valueswereequal.Wetestedthesepredictionsbyarrangingunitpricesuchthatthedenominator wasone(e.g.,tworesponsesproducedonereinforcer)ortwo(e.g.,fourresponsesproducedtwo reinforcers). Results showed that consumption decreased as unit price increased and that unit pricevalueswith differentcomponents producedsimilar consumption. DESCRIPTORS: behavioral economics, costs and benefits, preference assessment, reinforcerdemand, unit price _______________________________________________________________________________ Methods of assessing preferred stimuli that schedule requirements (e.g., fixed-ratio [FR] 1), will increase appropriate engagement and involving relatively low-effort responses (e.g., reduce levels of problem behavior have consti- touchingtheexperimenter’shand)thatmaynot tuted an important area of research in behavior approximate more effortful responses required analysis (Ivancic, 2000). Several methods of in appliedsettings (e.g., Fisher & Mazur,1997; determining stimulus preference and subse- Roscoe, Iwata, & Kahng, 1999). quent reinforcer efficacy have been reported The conditions under which responding is (e.g., Fisher et al., 1992; Pace, Ivancic, predicted to persist under increasing response Edwards, Iwata, & Page, 1985; Roane, Voll- requirements may be informed by methods mer, Ringdahl, & Marcus, 1998). In these used in behavioral economics. The primary studies, evaluations of reinforcer efficacy have dependent variable in behavioral economic generally been arranged under relatively low research is consumption, or the number of reinforcers obtained (Hursh, 2000; Johnson & Thisexperimentwascompletedinpartialfulfillmentof Bickel, 2006). A second dependent variable in therequirementoftheMAdegreebythefirstauthor.We thank Lucas Bradley for his comments on an earlier behavioral economic research measures spend- version of this manuscript and Adam Brewer, Patrick ing,aseitherlocaloroverallresponseoutput.In Johnson, and Greg Madden for their conceptual contri- a traditional economic system, money serves as butionsto the demand analyses. We also thank Stephany Crisolo,NoelRoss, andSandeep Sranfortheirassistance the currency or means by which one obtains with data collection and various other aspects of this necessities (e.g., water) and luxury items (e.g., project. Portions of this research were presented at the large flat-screen televisions). In behavioral 25th meeting of the California Association for Behavior economic research with nonhumans and con- Analysis in Burlingame and the 33rd meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis in San Diego. Xeres temporary behavior-analytic reinforcer assess- Delmendo is now with Nyansa Learning Corporation, mentresearch,thecurrencyisbehavior,andthe Santa Ana, California. Monica Francisco is now at the commoditiesavailableforpurchasearegenerally Universityof Kansas. AddresscorrespondencetoJohnC.Borrero,whoisnow foods or preferred activities. Consumption and at the University of Maryland, Baltimore County, spending may thus be characterized as compo- Department of Psychology, 1000 Hilltop Circle, Balti- nents of a cost–benefit arrangement, in which more,Maryland21250(e-mail: [email protected]). doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-609 the ratio of cost (response requirement) over 609 610 XERES DELMENDO et al. benefit (reinforcer magnitude) defines a simple which yields a demand function with a slope formulation of unit price, which is represented between 0 and 21. If a 1% change in price by the equation producesalessthan1%changeinconsumption, FR P~ , ð1Þ demand is characterized as inelastic. Elastic A demand describes a greater than proportional where price (P) is determined by, in this change inconsumption asa function of changes example, the FR requirement divided by the in price, which yields a demand function with a amount (A) of the reinforcer or its magnitude. slope more negative than 21 (Hursh, Raslear, Unit price may be altered by manipulating Shurtleff,Bauman,&Simmons,1988).Ifa1% reinforcer magnitude or response requirements change in price produces a greater than 1% separately or in combination (Foster & Hack- changeinconsumption,demandischaracterized enberg, 2004). Thus, if completion of an FR 2 aselastic.Inotherwords,acommodityforwhich requirement is followed by one reinforcer, this demandisinelasticisonewhoseconsumptionis is equivalent in price to completion of an FR 4 not greatly affected by changes in price (e.g., requirement that is followed by two reinforcers. consumptionmaydecreaseaspriceincreases,but Behavioral economic research characterizes decreasesarelessthanproportional).Bycontrast, performance in terms of the exchange of a commodity with elastic demand is one whose responses and reinforcers. For example, if consumptionismoreaffectedbychangesinprice ‘‘spending’’ eight lever presses (response) pro- (e.g., consumption decreases in a greater than duces one food pellet (reinforcement), a unit proportional fashion as price increases). For price analysis predicts that eight lever presses example, if the price of kiwis increased from that produce one food pellet would be $1.00 per kilgram to $2.00 per kilogram, this functionally equal to 16 lever presses that would represent a 50% increase in price. To produce two food pellets. Thus, if both the characterize demand for kiwis as elastic, con- response requirement and the reinforcer sumption would necessarily decrease by more amount increase by the same constant, eco- than 50%. However, if consumption of kiwis nomic theory suggests that consumption will decreased by only 10%, given a change in price not be affected because the unit price of the from$1.00perkilogramto$2.00perkilogram, reinforcer will be constant. then consumption for kiwis wouldbe character- Given a range of unit price values, reinforcer ized as inelastic. demand can be empirically assessed. Demand Changes in the slope of the demand function refers to the amount of the commodity (obtained by plotting reinforcer consumption consumed relative to the price for each against unit price) can be used to assess the commodity (Hursh, 1980). The effects of relative reinforcing efficacy of stimuli. Given changes in price (on consumption) are predict- ed to follow the law of demand, which states increases in price, stimuli that produce slopes that, all else being equal, consumption of a more shallow than 21 are more valuable than commodity (reinforcer) decreases as its price thosewithslopessteeperthan21(Allison,1983). increases (Freed & Green, 1991). Demand Whenplottedondoublelogarithmiccoordinates, elasticity is the extent to which consumption of thefunctionrelatingconsumptionandincreasing a commodity is influenced by alterations in unit price is positively decelerating, and the price for that commodity (Green & Freed, function relating response output to increases in 1993),and two types of demandarecommonly unit price is typically an inverted U-shaped discussed in the literature. Inelastic demand function (Bickel, Marsch, & Carroll, 2000). describes a less than proportional change in Several recent studies have evaluated rein- consumption as a function of changes in price, forcerefficacyunderprogressive-ratioschedules, COST AND BENEFIT COMPONENTS 611 in which response requirements increase within academic performance has been shown to (e.g., Francisco, Borrero, & Sy, 2008; Glover, persist when one reinforcer is delivered for Roane, Kadey, & Grow, 2008; Roane, Call, & every 10 responses. Assume further that a Falcomata,2005;Roane,Lerman,&Vorndran, teacher’s attention is diverted from the student 2001) and across (e.g., DeLeon, Iwata, Goh, & for a period of time such that delivery of the Worsdell, 1997; Johnson & Bickel, 2006; reinforcer following Response 10 is omitted. Tustin, 2000) sessions. Results of these studies Presumably,performance shouldbe maintained have been consistent with the law of demand in at similar levels if two reinforcers were arranged that decreases in consumption were generally for completion of 20 responses, and thus, a associated with increases in response require- potentially detrimental instance of integrity ment. failure (Peterson, Homer, & Wonderlich, Although not framed as such, the work of 1982) might be avoided by a modest manipu- DeLeon et al. (1997) may also be assessed in lation of reinforcer amount, thereby keeping terms of reinforcer demand. DeLeon et al. unit price constant. evaluated consumption of two concurrently In addition to offsetting potential treatment available edible reinforcers (e.g., a cookie and integrity problems, evaluations of reinforcer a cracker) under progressively increasing sched- demand may suggest the maximum schedule ule requirements. Responding for the two requirementunderwhichstimulimaintainhigh reinforcers was similar under low response value (i.e., P [the price at which peak max requirements, but a preference emerged for responding is reached], and corresponding one reinforcer (cookie) under increasing re- valuesalongtheinelasticportionofthedemand sponse requirements. These data illustrate that function). Inotherwords,inpractice,clinicians the value of one reinforcer (cookie) varied attempt to identify the largest amount of work depending on the other concurrently available that can be maintained with the smallest reinforcer. Results reported by DeLeon et al. reinforcer amount. If a client will complete 20 raise important issues that should be evaluated. academictasksforonereinforcer,itwouldbeill First, performance under a single schedule may advised to arrange one reinforcer contingent on not correspond to performance under concur- 10academicresponses.Similarly,itwouldbeill rent schedules (e.g., Francisco et al., 2008; advised to arrange one reinforcer contingent on Roscoe et al., 1999). That is, consumption of 50 academic responses. By setting a price that one reinforcer might differ, depending on the exceeds P , spending (or work) will decrease. max presence or absence of alternative reinforcers. By setting a price that approaches P , the max Second, in terms of behavioral programming, value of the reinforcer is maintained, and the predicted performance should be similar when maximum amount of work is produced. unit price remains the same across changes in Evaluations of demand elasticity across the cost–benefit components. In other words, if various price arrangements may be useful in maximal performance (as evidenced by the determining the conditions under which highly largest value of the inelastic portion of a preferred stimuli, identified via preference demand function) occurs when x responses assessment, will support clinically acceptable produce one reinforcer, similar performance levels of responding under increasing response should occur when 2x responses produce two requirements. For example, more valuable reinforcers (i.e., unit price is unchanged). From stimuli (i.e., those that produce relatively a practical standpoint, such a finding might shallow slopes) may be made contingent on have an impact in classroom or institutional more effortful behavior, and relatively less settings. For example, assume that a student’s valuable stimuli (i.e., those that produce 612 XERES DELMENDO et al. relatively steeper slopes) might be arranged conducted in a small room at their respective contingent on less effortful behavior. By schools. The room contained a desk, a table, incorporating greater variety in the pro- several chairs, and various teaching materials. grammed consequences for behavior, satiation, Sessions for Keelan were conducted in the at least with respect to a particular stimulus, dining room of his home. The room contained may be minimized. Although such findings a dining table, four chairs, and a shelf. Sessions would be of importance to applied researchers, wereconductedonetothreetimesperday,4to a preliminary assessment of reinforcer demand, 5 days per week for each child. As part of the withunitpriceheldconstant,isaprerequisiteto intakeandconsentprocedure,parentsindicated more complex analyses like those involving which foods were prohibited due to food qualitatively different stimuli. allergies or parental preference. Prior to the Previous operant research with humans (e.g., initiation of data collection, the university Bickel & Madden, 1999; Madden, Bickel, & institutional review board approved this study. Jacobs,2000)suggeststhatconsumptionwillbe Response Measurement and similar under equal unit price values, regardless Interobserver Agreement of the cost–benefit components. However, such findings have yet to be demonstrated in the During the preference assessment, data were context of reinforcer assessment research in- collected on consumption (defined as an edible volving academic task completion. Thus, the item passing the plane of the participant’s lips). purpose of the current investigation was to During the reinforcer assessment and reinforcer determine whether similar results would be demand evaluations, target academic responses obtained in the context of repeated reinforcer were selected according to the participant’s assessments in which unit price was held academic level, based on consultation with the constant while cost–benefit components were participant’s teachers. The target response was varied. If consumption and response output are tracing (letters and numbers) for Elijah, sorting largely uninfluenced by cost–benefit compo- beads by color for Anna, completion of writing nents, behavioral programming may be en- worksheets for Keelan, and color matching for hanced. In other words, the unit price itself Elizabeth. For the tracing task, a response was (e.g., six) may be more relevant in determining recorded when the participant completely reinforcer demand than the methods used to traced either one letter or one number. For arrangeaunitpriceofsix(e.g.,12responsesfor the bead-sorting task, a response was recorded two reinforcers or 36 responses for six reinforc- when the participant placed one like-colored ers; Roane, Falcomata, & Fisher, 2007). bead among an array of beads arranged in a sorting receptacle. For the color-matching task, the participant was required to place an object METHOD of one color in a receptacle containing like- Participants and Settings colored stimuli. Four typically developing children partici- Trained observers were seated unobtrusively pated, and parental consent, as well as child in the room and collected data using paper and assent, was obtained prior to data collection. pencil (preference assessment) or handheld Elijah was 4 years old, attended preschool 4 personal digital assistants. Interobserver agree- days per week, and attended an afterschool care ment was assessed by having a second observer program. Annaand Elizabethwere both 3 years simultaneously but independently record con- old and attended preschool 5 days per week. sumption and academic responses. Agreement Keelan was 6 years old and attended first grade. was calculated using the method of partial Sessions for Elijah, Anna, and Elizabeth were agreement within intervals (Vollmer, Borrero, COST AND BENEFIT COMPONENTS 613 Wright, Van Camp, & Lalli, 2001). Data were food was ever unavailable. Consumption was assessed by dividing the entire session into recorded using a 10-s partial-interval recording consecutive 10-s intervals, and the smaller system. It was possible for participants to number in each 10-s interval was divided by consume multiple foods during a given 10-s thelargernumber.Interobserveragreementdata interval. Preference was determined by record- during the free-operant preference assessment ing the percentage of 10-s intervals during were collected during 50%, 33%, 67%, and which consumption of each food occurred. In 67% of sessions for Elijah, Anna, Keelan, and other words, the number of 10-s intervals with Elizabeth, respectively (multiple preference consumption of each food was divided by the assessments were conducted for each partici- total number of intervals in the session, and pant, described below). Agreement for con- multiplied by 100%. Results of each preference sumptionduringthepreferenceassessmentwere assessment were averaged, and the food item 90% for Elijah, 100% for Anna, 72% for consumed during the greatest mean percentage Keelan, and 96% for Elizabeth. During the of intervals was subsequently evaluated via reinforcer assessment, agreement data were reinforcer assessment. collected during 41%, 30%, 29%, and 76% of sessions for Elijah, Anna, Keelan, and Reinforcer Assessment: Procedure and Design Elizabeth, respectively. These values were then The purpose of the reinforcer assessment was averaged for the entire session. Agreement to determine whether items consumed most values for academic responding during the during the preference assessment would func- reinforcer assessment were 92% for Elijah tionasreinforcerswhendeliveredcontingenton (range, 88% to 93%), 100% for Anna, 98% academic responding. The academic response forKeelan (range,95%to 100%),and 95%for foreachchildwasdefinedpreviouslyandserved Elizabeth (range, 86% to 100%). During the as the dependent variable during the reinforcer reinforcer demand evaluation, agreement data assessment. During baseline (A), the therapist were collected during 38%, 36%, 44%, and presented the participant with the instructional 37% of sessions for Elijah, Anna, Keelan, and materials (e.g., a tracing worksheet) and Elizabeth,respectively,andwere88%forElijah permitted the participant to engage in the (range, 74% to 100%), 95% for Anna (range, activity. However, the therapist did not deliver 81%to100%),89%forKeelan(range,60%to reinforcers for academic responding, and edible 100%) and 97% for Elizabeth (range, 86% to items were not present or in view. During the 100%). reinforcement condition (B), the therapist presented the participant with the same task Preference Assessment: Procedure used in baseline; however, the therapist deliv- Thepurposeofthepreferenceassessmentwas ered reinforcers for academic responding on an toidentifyonefooditemthatwouldfunctionas FR 1 schedule. Edible items were both present a potential reinforcer for the subsequent and in view during this condition. Reinforcer analyses. Each participant’s behavior was ex- efficacy was evaluated using an A-B-A design, posed to two (Elijah) or three (Anna, Elizabeth, and a minimum of three sessions per condition and Keelan) preference assessments using the were conducted. After demonstration of a procedures described by Roane et al. (1998). reinforcement effect, participants proceeded to During the preference assessment, only food the reinforcer demand evaluation. items were evaluated. A total of 10 foods (in bite-sizeportions)werearrangedinacircleona Reinforcer Demand Evaluation: Procedure table, and participants could consume the food The purpose of the reinforcer demand freely. Foods were replenished such that no evaluation was to assess consumption and 614 XERES DELMENDO et al. Table 1 Sequenceof Conditions andExposure Series1 Series2 Participant Edibleitem Unitprice Sessions Fixedratio Edibleitem Sessions Fixedratio Edibleitem Elijah Candy 2 7 2 1 7 4 2 4 5 4 1 5 8 2 6 5 6 1 5 12 2 8 6 8 1 5 16 2 15 5 15 1 5 30 2 5 45 1 Anna Chips 2 5 2 1 7 4 2 4 6 4 1 8 8 2 6 5 6 1 5 12 2 8 5 8 1 5 16 2 16 5 16 1 6 32 2 Keelan Chips 2 5 2 1 5 4 2 4 7 4 1 7 8 2 6 9 6 1 9 12 2 8 5 8 1 5 16 2 15 5 15 1 5 30 2 Elizabeth Cookies 2 5 2 1 5 4 2 4 6 4 1 5 8 2 6 5 6 1 5 12 2 8 5 8 1 5 16 2 performance given increasing response require- target academic response or would do so at low ments and to assess consumption and perfor- levels. mance under equal unit price values composed Prior to the first reinforcer demand session, of different cost–benefit components. Each the participant was told that he or she could participant completed two reinforcer demand earn a bite of their favorite food for completing evaluations (hereafter referred to as ‘‘series’’). In a specific number of responses, and that he or Series 1, completion of the predetermined FR she could consume the food. A minimum of schedulerequirementresultedinonereinforcer. five sessions was conducted for each unit price For example, given an FR 15 response in each series, and sessions were terminated requirement, completion of 15 academic re- when one of the following criteria was met: (a) sponses produced one bite of the participant’s 3 min elapsed without a response, or (b) the most preferred food. In Series 2, completion of participant emitted the vocal response, ‘‘I’m the predetermined FR schedule requirement done.’’ The latter termination criterion was resulted in two reinforcers. Thus, to ensure an included to reduce the likelihood of disruptive equal unit price (in this example), an FR 30 behavior that might have occurred by requiring schedule was arranged, which resulted in participants to stay seated for 3 min. delivery of two reinforcers. The sequence of exposures, specific unit price values, cost and RESULTS benefit components, response requirements, and reinforcers are summarized in Table 1. Figure 1 depicts the mean results of the Prior to Series 1, all participants had just preference assessments for all participants. completed the baseline condition of the prior Results suggested that candies, chips, chips, reinforcer assessment phase, and thus we and cookies might serve as potential reinforcers concluded that in the absence of programmed for Elijah, Anna, Keelan, and Elizabeth, contingencies, participants would not emit the respectively. COST AND BENEFIT COMPONENTS 615 contingent food was withdrawn.Based on these results, we concluded that these edible items served as reinforcers for each participant’s academic responding. Figure 3 depicts results of the reinforcer demand evaluations for all participants. Data from the last four sessions are reported because this is the method of data aggregation used in the majority of behavioral economic research with nonhumans and also eliminates data that might be anomalous due to transitions from one unit price value to the next. The objective of Series 1 (Figure 3) was to determine the level of academic responding, and by extension, the number of reinforcers obtained, when the price of the reinforcer was systematicallyincreased.Inbrief,asthenumber of academic responses required was increased, the number of reinforcers obtained decreased, consistent with the law of demand. More specifically, during Series 1 for Elijah (top left) and Anna (second left), demand was inelastic (there was a less than proportional change in consumption as a function of the change in price)whenpricewasincreasedfromUnitPrice 2 to Unit Price 4. Specifically, the slope of the function from Unit Price 2 to Unit Price 4 was 20.97 for Elijah and 20.15 for Anna. Recall that slopes between 0 and 21 define inelastic demand, which means that although academic responding did decrease for Elijah, the propor- tional decrease in reinforcers obtained was less than the proportional increase in price. Or, the Figure1. Preferenceassessmentresultsforallparticipants. value of the edible item was maintained despite Notethatthescaleoftheyaxisissettoamaximumof40. requiring more academic work to produce it. Subsequent increases in price during Series 1 Figure 2 depicts the results of the reinforcer (e.g., from Unit Price 4 to Unit Price 6) assessment for all participants. In the initial resulted in largely elastic demand (greater than baseline condition, in which edible items were proportional change in consumption as a not available contingent on academic respond- function of changes in price), which means ing, mean response rates were generally low for that at these higher prices, proportional de- all participants. When reinforcers were deliv- creasesinreinforcers obtainedweregreaterthan ered on an FR 1 schedule, mean response rates the proportional increases in price, with the increased relative to baseline levels for all exceptionofthetransitionbetweenUnitPrice6 participants, and subsequently decreased when and Unit Price 8 for Anna (slope 5 0.28). In 616 XERES DELMENDO et al. Figure2. Resultsofthereinforcerassessmentforallparticipants.Seetextforadescriptionofthecontingenciesin placeduring baseline andFR1. other words, as the price of the edible item work when the price increased but earned less increased, the number of academic responses for every instance of academic responding. required was too great and resulted in less Results for Keelan and Elizabeth illustrate the responding than would have been predicted opposite change in response output. Response given the proportional increase in price. By output decreased slightly for Keelan and contrast, slopes for Keelan (third left) and Elizabeth (slopes 5 20.72, and 20.83, Elizabeth(bottomleft)wereindicativeofelastic respectively), which means that they responded demand during transitions from even the less when the price of the reinforcer was smallest unit price values (Unit Price 2 to Unit increased, but the decrease was within the Price 4) during Series 1 (slope 5 21.70 for expected(proportional)range.Thisunderscores Keelan and 22.82 for Elizabeth) suggesting the fact that increases in academic responding that even these very modest increases in price are only contextually related to evaluations of were too much to maintain academic respond- demand (i.e., a child may emit more absolute ing at sufficiently high levels. In addition, responding under increasing price require- response output increased from Unit Price 2 ments, but this does not in and of itself suggest to Unit Price 4 for Elijah and Anna (slopes 5 greater stimulus value). If the number of 0.14, and 0.86, respectively). These data reinforcers obtained under these higher price illustrate that even though the number of requirements decreases in a greater than academic tasks completed might increase, the proportional fashion, the stimulus has still lost total amount of reinforcers obtained can some of its value. decrease(becausemore workisrequired to earn The objective of Series 2 (Figure 3) was to reinforcers when the price of a reinforcer determine the extent to which changes in increases). From a practical standpoint, this consumption would be observed when unit means that Elijah and Anna completed more pricewasheldconstantandthecostandbenefit COST AND BENEFIT COMPONENTS 617 components were varied. In other words, for at which decreases in the number of reinforcers each unit price assessed, the objective was to obtained (and by extension, tasks completed) determine the extent to which reinforcers were proportionally greater than the change in obtained and task completed would be similar price from Unit Price 4 to Unit Price 6. Thus, at the same unit price values. For 3 of 4 despite the differences in Anna’s overall con- participants (Elijah, Keelan, and Elizabeth), sumption (Figure 3),demandwas characterized there was considerable similarity in terms of aselasticinbothSeries1andSeries2,following reinforcersobtainedacrossSeries1andSeries2. thetransitionfromUnitPrice4toUnitPrice6. In other words, from the perspective of 3 Reinforcer demand in Series 2 changed some- participants, a situation in which, for example, whatforElijahcomparedtoSeries1(Figure 3). two academic responses were required for one BecausemeanreinforcersobtainedinSeries2at reinforcerwasviewedassimilartoasituationin Unit Price 15 exceeded consumption from which four academic responses were required Series 1 under Unit Price 15 (the largest unit for two reinforcers. Data for Anna, during the price value assessed in Series 1), an increase in transition from Unit Price 2 to Unit Price 4, unit price was introduced. Specifically, it appeared that the value of the commodity weretheexception(althoughtheoverallpattern changed from Series 1 to Series 2. Thus, Unit remained unchanged). A second objective of Price 45 was introduced. The transition from Series 2 was to determine the point at which Unit Price 15 to Unit Price 45 produced a demand transitioned from inelastic to elastic; decrease in reinforcers obtained that was that is, when the change in the number of considered proportional to the rather large reinforcers obtained moved from decreases that increase in price and resulted in a transition would be expected proportionally to decreases slope of 20.89 (or degree of change). that were larger proportionally than expected. Figure 4depictsdataforthelastfoursessions For all participants, these transition values (i.e., conducted at each unit price (instead of the specific price increases such as Unit Price 4 to means of those sessions depicted in Figure 3). Unit Price 6 for Elijah in Series 1 and Series 2) For all participants, reinforcers obtained (left) were identical. This can be illustrated with were fitted using the logarithmic version of the Anna’s data. For the other 3 participants, the equation proposed by Hursh et al. (1988): data paths are either fairly close to one another orontopofoneanother,atthesameunitprice. lnC~lnLzbðlnPÞ{aP, ð2Þ For Anna, reinforcers obtained for Unit Price 2 to evaluate the functional relation between total in Series 1 are greater than the number of reinforcers consumed per session (C) and unit reinforcers obtained in Series 2. The same is price(P).Lisanestimateofreinforcersobtained true for Unit Price 4 (note that open circles are thatwouldoccuratUnitPrice1,andbandaare consistently higher than closed triangles for relatedtotheinitialslopeandaccelerationofthe Anna, across all unit price values). Despite the demandfunction,respectively.Dataonacadem- differences in the number of reinforcers ob- ic responding (right) were fitted to the same tainedfromSeries1toSeries2acrossthesetwo equation,exceptthatb+1wassubstitutedforb price values, the point at which her academic (Hursh, Raslear, Bauman, & Black, 1989). responding was proportionally greater than the When the number of reinforcers obtained was increaseinpricewasstillthesamefromSeries1 zero(i.e.,AnnaatUnitPrice16duringSeries2, to Series 2. In other words, when either six Keelan at Unit Price 15 during Series 2, and responses were required for one reinforcer ElizabethatUnitPrice8duringSeries2),avalue (Series 1) or 12 responses were required for of0.1wasplottedsothatdataforallfoursessions two reinforcers, this is the price (Unit Price 6) would be visible for inspection. 618 XERES DELMENDO et al. Figure 3. Results of the demand analysis for all participants. Each participant’s data occupies one row. Each data pointrepresentsmeannumberofreinforcersearned(leftcolumn)ormeannumberoftaskscompleted(rightcolumn), across the last four sessions of the condition, for all participants (session-by-session data are available from the second author).Dataareexpressedondoublelogarithmicaxes.OpencirclesdepictdatafromSeries1,andfilledtrianglesdepict