ebook img

Developments and Social Problems PDF

278 Pages·1983·3.826 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Developments and Social Problems

RESEARCH WITH THE LOCUS OF CONTROL CONSTRUCT Volume 2 Developments and Social Problems EDITED BY Herbert M. Lefcourt Department of Psychology University of Waterloo Waterloo, Ontario, Canada 1983 ACADEMIC PRESS A Subsidiary of Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Publishers New York London Paris San Diego San Francisco Sao Paulo Sydney Tokyo Toronto COPYRIGHT © 1983, BY ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. NO PART OF THIS PUBLICATION MAY BE REPRODUCED OR TRANSMITTED IN ANY FORM OR BY ANY MEANS, ELECTRONIC OR MECHANICAL, INCLUDING PHOTOCOPY, RECORDING, OR ANY INFORMATION STORAGE AND RETRIEVAL SYSTEM, WITHOUT PERMISSION IN WRITING FROM THE PUBLISHER. ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. Ill Fifth Avenue, New York, New York 10003 United Kingdom Edition published by ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. (LONDON) LTD. 24/28 Oval Road, London NW1 7DX Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data Main entry under title: Research with the locus of control construct. Includes bibliographies and index. Contents: v. 1. Assessment methods -- v. 2. Develop- ments and social problems. 1. Control (Psychology) 2. Interpersonal relations. 3. Psychology, Pathological. I. Lefcourt, Herbert M. [DNLM: 1. Interna1-external control. BF 632.5 R^32 1981] BF611.RA7 155-2'32 8I-7876 ISBN 0-12-^3202-6 PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA 83 84 85 86 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 LIST OF CONTRIBUTORS Numbers in parentheses indicate the pages on which the authors' contributions begin. Virginia C. Crandall (53), Wright State University, School of Medicine, Fels Research Institute, Yellow Springs, Ohio 45387 Beth W. Crandall (53), Wright State University, School of Medicine, Fels Research Institute, Yellow Springs, Ohio 45387 William J. Doherty (155), Department of Family Practice, University of Iowa, Oakdale, Iowa 52319 Dennis M. Donovan (107), Alcohol Dependence Treatment Program, Veterans Administration Medical Center, Seattle, Washington 98108 Marshall P. Duke (9), Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322 Paul Harpin (187), Nordli-Wilson Associates, New Haven, Connecticut 06510 Herbert M. Lefcourt (1, 253, 269), Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario N2L 3G1 Stephen Nowicki, Jr. (9), Department of Psychology, Emory University, Atlanta, Georgia 30322 Michael R. O'Leary (107), Kiksap Psychiatric Associates, Bremerton, Washington 98310 Finetta Reese (187), Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona 85287 Irwin Sandler (187), Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona 85287 Lee Spencer (187), Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona 85287 ix PREFACE Part I of this volume pertains to one realm of locus of control research that is of signal interest to psychologists concerned with per- sonality research and theory. If locus of control is an important predic- tor of behavior, then we should know something about its origins and the role it plays at different stages of the life span. In various poetic ways we have often heard that childhood and old age share many features, especially with regard to helplessness. The scrutiny of such age-specific proclivities in order to shed light on these commonalities form a welcome addition to locus of control studies. The chapters in the first part aid in the development of such a life-span approach to locus of control research. Part II of this volume continues the approach of the first volume by focusing on applications to two clinical-social problems: marital in- stability and alcoholism. The chapters in this section review the literature in which locus of control has been used as a predictor of marital instability and of drinking behavior. Finally, in the third part, the use of locus of control as a moderator variable is explored. Here, the response to particular situational con- straints or milieu characteristics is evaluated vis-à-vis status on locus of control variables. In this way it becomes possible to speak of the specific effects of therapy or educational procedures upon persons who vary in personality characteristics such as locus of control, in much the way that the advocates of interactionism have always advised. In covering these three areas this volume may be seen as a con- tribution toward explaining the nomological network in which the locus of control construct is embedded. In studying the antecedents, xi xii : PREFACE concomitants, and far-reaching ramifications of the construct we can come to see its meaning more clearly. In each case the results are not always affirmative. However, negative results can also help to clarify the limits of a constructs domain as well as to provide a goad to re- searchers who refuse to accept findings that run in a counterintuitive direction. It is our hope that these chapters will both stimulate future research and help researchers to see more clearly how and when the construct and specific measures of it may be used most fruitfully. 1 Herbert M. Lefcourt INTRODUCTION In this second volume of the Research with the Locus of Control Construct series we present reviews of some familiar and some more esoteric research ventures. Given this admixture there is a likeness be- tween this volume and the immediately preceding one. However, rather than focus on new assessment methods, as we had in Volume 1, we turn our attention to new uses that have been made of the locus of control construct. Part I: Early Research Programs Similar to the first volume in this series, the first part of this book contains reviews of older research programs, parts of which have been familiar to psychologists concerned with the locus of control construct. One of these programs, described by Stephen Nowicki and Marshall Duke, is unique in that these investigators have been involved in the construction of sets of locus of control scales that were tailored to the ages and statuses of various subjects. It would seem a simple and com- monsensical ambition to create measures that are of immediate rele- vance to the persons who are to complete them. If people care about certain goals or experiences, their beliefs regarding their accomplish- ments would more likely be predictive of relevant behaviors than would be beliefs about experiences that are of lesser interest. Empirical dem- onstrations of this sensible proposition have been reported with regard to locus of control (Naditch & De Maio, 1975). RESEARCH WITH THE LOCUS Copyright © 1983 by Academic Press, Inc. OF CONTROL CONSTRUCT (Vol. 2): All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. Developments and Social Problems ISBN: 0-12-443202-6 1 2 : HERBERT M. LEFCOURT Though this position is appealing, few investigators have con- cerned themselves about whether the scales they administer are appro- priate to subjects' interests. For example, questions concerning school achievement and the making of friendships have been asked of elderly persons when more pertinent questions might have involved family commitments, the maintenance of privacy, and so on as are repre- sented in Reid and Ziegler's (1981) scales. Consequently, the scale construction by Nowicki, Duke, and their colleagues is welcomed. Their chapter presents a review of, as well as norms for, some of these scales. The greater range of choices of scales now available for locus of control researchers should encourage greater deliberation and more discriminating selections of assessment devices. The second chapter, from a mature program of research, is based on a paper Virginia Crandall wrote in 1973, when she was invited to address the American Psychological Association at an annual meeting. In that address Crandall described most of the research available at that time, pertaining to the familial origins of locus of control orientations. Crandall presented her own data deriving from the Fels Institute lon- gitudinal sample, which was, and possibly still is, the most interesting data set available concerning familial origins of locus of control. Based on observations of family interactions made during childhood and per- sonality assessments completed in the young adult years, Crandall's data provided an exciting addition to this literature. Although those who heard the address and had copies of her paper have referred to this study repeatedly, it has not previously appeared in print. Chapter 3 presents the material from the earlier address and contains an update of the literature pertaining to childhood antecedents of locus of control orientations. Among the data reported in this chapter, that described in the literature concerned with contingency awareness among hu- mans (Watson, 1979) and other primates (Suomi, 1980), and the work concerning childhood antecedents of competence (Sroufe, 1979), in- vestigators may be able to bring themselves up to date regarding the state of knowledge in this area. Part II: Application to Social Problems In the second part of this volume, two chapters are presented that add to the developing literature concerned with clinical applications of locus of control research. In the first volume, Worell and Tumilty (1981) described a locus of control measure pertaining to alcohol-related be- havior. At that time I had not been aware of the extensive research program about which Dennis Donovan and Michael O'Leary had been 1 INTRODUCTION : 3 writing in journals thai are primarily concerned with the problem of alcoholism. As well as contributing to the literature linking locus of control with clinical problems, Donovan and O'Leary present in Chap- ter 4 the research findings obtained with an area-specific locus of con- trol device (Drink-Related I-E Scale) that adds to the potential repertoire of investigators interested in using more-targeted locus of control de- vices. In this chapter, much of the literature concerning alcoholism and locus of control is reviewed. This is a welcome contribution, given the previously reported paradoxical findings to the effect that addicts and alcoholics often score in a more internal direction on I-E scales than more normal individuals. This set of paradoxical findings has been dis- cussed in my previous writings (Lefcourt, 1982). In the Donovan and O'Leary chapter, some elucidation of these perplexing findings is ac- complished with the use of locus of control measures that are more relevant to drinking behavior. Chapter 5 by William Doherty focuses on locus of control and mar- ital interaction. Because attentiveness to and wise use of information has been linked with locus of control expectations in previous research (Lefcourt, 1982), it would seem that spousal attention to one another and their tendency to act on their obtained information should likewise be associated with the degree to which they view marital interactions as controllable and themselves as responsible for the outcomes of those interactions. That locus of control may prove valuable in helping to comprehend the processes by which marriages are strengthened or weakened is as yet speculative. However, Doherty's chapter helps to bring the reader abreast of the current literature and also offers sug- gestions as to where further investigations are apt to be profitable. Because marriage is one of the few institutions that actively fosters a sense of intimacy between individuals, and intimacy, closeness, and social support are so important in helping individuals cope with dif- ficult life circumstances (Brown & Harris, 1976; Cobb, 1976), the cur- rent trends toward instability in marriage must be viewed with alarm. It is hoped that the use of constructs such as locus of control for in- terpreting the failures of couples to aid and facilitate each other may both encourage further research in this area and offer suggestions for the improvement of relationships. Part III: Locus of Control as a Moderator Variable In Part III, two chapters are presented that describe the manner in which locus of control can be used in interaction with other variables for predicting given outcomes. In Chapter 6, by Sandier, Reese, Spen- 4 : HERBERT M. LEFCOURT cer, and Harpin, locus of control is described as a variable that can be used to study the impacts of various procedures that differ in the degree to which they allow subjects freedom to maneuver on their own. Locus of control has been found to afford some prediction of outcomes achieved in response to particular educational and therapeutic ap- proaches, with internals preferring certain kinds of procedures and ex- ternals often favoring diametrically opposite procedures. As such, locus of control can be viewed as a moderator variable, which, in interaction with given treatment conditions, helps to produce particular results. In this chapter, Sandier and his associates attempt to review the findings implicating locus of control as a moderator of different treatments. In like fashion, in Chapter 7, I describe the literature concerned specifically with stress. In earlier articles (Lefcourt, 1973, 1980) I re- viewed the literature suggesting that locus of control could be used to help explain differential responses to aversive situations. In this chap- ter I explore how locus of control can be used to predict the responses of individuals to a number of common life stresses. As with specific treatment conditions, locus of control is found to act as a moderator variable; that is, as a predictor of how stress is likely to affect its vic- tims. In Chapters 6 and 7, examples are provided to show how situation and personality variables may be used conjointly, as has been recom- mended by innumerable advocates of interactionism (e.g., Endler & Magnusson, 1976). In addition, some evaluation of the effectiveness of locus of control for prediction of responses to stress, on the one hand, and to particular treatments, on the other, can be derived. As was the case in the first volume, it is the hope of both chapter authors and the editor that the material in this volume will help to bring readers up to date and in so doing will stimulate and encourage others to examine further the ramifications of beliefs regarding personal ef- fectiveness, causality, and the manner in which action and experience are connected. Though many writers prefer to attend to the distinctions between various beliefs and perceptions concerning control, causality, and efficacy, it is my hope that this latter emphasis on conceptual de- lineations will not detract from the larger issue that concerns the man- ner in which individuals perceive themselves interacting in their worlds. It is in the examination of our subjects* phenomenal worlds, wherein they perceive the connections between their own actions and characteristics and the events that befall them, that we will probably obtain our most fruitful results; and this phenomenal world will prob- ably prove to be neither as atomistic as attribution researchers would have us believe nor as segmented as those concerned with specific areas 1. INTRODUCTION : 5 of efficacy have suggested. Rather, in viewing attributions, perceived efficacy, and so on as parts or indicators of one's view of oneself and one's world, perhaps we will be able to derive more useful statements about human endeavors. References Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. Social origins of depression: A study of psychiatric disorder in women. London: Tavistock, 1978. Cobb, S. Presidential Address: Social support as a moderator of life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine, 1976, 38, 300-314. Endler, N. S., & Magnusson, D. Toward an interactional psychology of personality. Psy- chological Bulletin, 1976, 83, 956-974. Lefcourt, H. M. The function of the illusions of control and freedom. American Psy- chologist, 1973, 28, 417-425. Lefcourt, H. M. Locus of control and coping with life's events. In E. Staub (Ed.), Person- ality. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1980. Lefcourt, H. M. Locus of control: Current trends in theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1982. Naditch, M. P., & De Maio, T. Locus of control and competence. Journal of Personality, 1975, 43, 541-559. Reid, D. W., & Ziegler, M. The desired control measure and adjustment among the el- derly. In H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the locus of control construct (Vol. 1): Assessment Methods New York: Academic Press, 1981. Sroufe, L. A. The coherence of individual development. American Psychologist, 1979, 34, 834-841. Suomi, S. J. Contingency, perception and social development. In L. R. Sherrod & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), In/ant social cognition: Empirical and theoretical considerations. Hills- dale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1980. Watson, J. S. Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In E. B. Thomas (Ed.), The origins of social responsiveness. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1979. Worell, L., & Tumilty, T. N. The measurement of locus of control among alcoholics. In H. M. Lefcourt (Ed.), Research with the locus of control construct (Vol. 1): Assessment Methods New York: Academic Press, 1981.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.