ebook img

Development of the social brain PDF

251 Pages·2018·2.327 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Development of the social brain

Table of Contents Cover Preface List of Contributors CHAPTER 1: The Evolution and Ontogeny of Deep Social Mind and the Social Brain INTRODUCTION PRIMATE MACHIAVELLIAN INTELLIGENCE AND THE SOCIAL BRAIN FROM MACHIAVELLIAN INTELLIGENCE TO THE CULTURAL INTELLIGENCE HYPOTHESIS THE EVOLUTION OF DEEP SOCIAL MIND THE ONTOGENY OF DEEP SOCIAL MIND: THE LIFE HISTORY MATRIX THE ONTOGENETIC DEVELOPMENT AND EVOLUTIONARY FOUNDATIONS OF DEEP SOCIAL MIND AND ITS SOCIAL BRAIN CONCLUDING REMARKS REFERENCES PART I: Animal Models of Social Brain Function CHAPTER 2: Neurobiology of Infant Sensitive Period for Attachment and Its Reinstatement Through Maternal Social Buffering INTRODUCTION NEUROBEHAVIORAL ASSESSMENT OF LEARNED MATERNAL CUES DURING THE ATTACHMENT SENSITIVE PERIOD UNCOVERING THE EFFECTS OF EARLY-LIFE ADVERSITY CONCLUDING REMARKS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS REFERENCES CHAPTER 3: Marmoset Monkey Vocal Communication: Common Developmental Trajectories With Humans and Possible Mechanisms INTRODUCTION THE MARMOSET MONKEY MODEL SYSTEM BABBLING AND PERINATAL INFLUENCES ON VOCAL OUTPUT DEVELOPMENT OF VOCAL TURN-TAKING TURN-TAKING AS THE DEVELOPMENTAL SYSTEM UPON WHICH INFANT VOCALIZATIONS ARE LEARNED THE AUTONOMIC NERVOUS SYSTEM AS THE ENGINE FOR VOCAL DEVELOPMENT EVOLUTIONARY ORIGINS CONCLUSIONS ACKNOWLEDGMENTS REFERENCES PART II: Higher-Order Human Social Brain Function CHAPTER 4: The Social Brain in Adolescence and Adulthood: Lessons in Mindreading INTRODUCTION: WHAT AM I THINKING? READING MINDS AT ONE'S FOURTH BIRTHDAY PARTY: THE COGNITIVE FOUNDATIONS OF MENTALIZING A PRIMER FOR THE NEURAL FOUNDATIONS OF THEORY OF MIND WHAT THE DIFFICULTIES OF ADULTS CAN TELL US ABOUT THEORY OF MIND REASONING READING MINDS LIKE BREATHING AIR: “AUTOMATIC” PERSPECTIVE TAKING BUILDING A THEORY OF MIND: FUNCTIONAL AND NEURAL CHANGES THROUGH CHILDHOOD AND ADOLESCENCE CONCLUSION REFERENCES CHAPTER 5: Developmental Social Neuroscience of Morality INTRODUCTION DEFINITIONAL ISSUES AND THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES PERCEPTION AND SENSITIVITY TO INTERPERSONAL HARM NEURODEVELOPMENTAL CHANGES IN THIRD-PARTY PERCEPTION OF INTERPERSONAL HARM NEUROLOGICAL LESIONS THAT IMPAIR MORAL COGNITION AND BEHAVIOR ATYPICAL FUNCTIONAL AND ANATOMICAL CONNECTIVITY WHAT WE HAVE LEARNED WHERE SHOULD DEVELOPMENTAL NEUROSCIENCE BE HEADING? REFERENCES NOTE PART III: Summary and Future Directions CHAPTER 6: Development of the Social Brain: From Mechanisms to Principles INTRODUCTION MECHANISTIC FEATURES OF NEURAL DEVELOPMENT THE SOCIAL ENVIRONMENT: PERMISSIVE, INSTRUCTIVE, ENABLING, AND/OR BUFFERING? CAUSALITY: PARTIAL CORRELATION VERSUS TEMPORAL ORDER WHAT ARE THE PROCESSES? INSIGHTS FROM THE VARIED NATURE OF MENTALIZING DOMAIN SPECIFICITY REVISITED FROM MECHANISMS TO PRINCIPLES ACKNOWLEDGMENTS REFERENCES Author Index Subject Index End User License Agreement List of Tables Chapter 1 Table 1.1 Articles indexed in Web of Knowledge according to key words in title or in topic field. List of Illustrations Chapter 1 Figure 1.1 Group size and encephalization (here, executive brain ratio = volume of cortex over rest of brain) in primates. Figure 1.2 Social learning and encephalization in primates. Frequency of social learning in the survey of Reader and Laland (2002) is plotted against executive brain ratio (see text for further explanation). Labels refer to three species with complex cultures discussed extensively in the text. Figure 1.3 Deep Social Mind. Principal classes of social cognition (in bold capitals) in hunter-gatherer bands and inferred reinforcing relationships between them, with causal link indicated by directional arrows (after Whiten & Erdal, 2012). Note that such relationships cannot be exhaustively illustrated in a single legible figure; those shown are indicative only. For explanation and discussion see text. Chapter 2 Figure 2.1 This schematic represents pups' transitions in attachment learning with odor- 0.5mA shock conditioning. Pups younger than PN10 have robust attachment learning during a sensitive period due to low CORT levels. This learning circuit requires low levels of CORT and involves maternal behavior stimulation of the locus coeruleus to release norepinephrine into the olfactory bulb to induce the neural changes required for pup learning. Older pups readily learn amygdala-dependent fear because of CORT's action on the amygdala to permit learning-induced plasticity, although maternal presence through social buffering lowering of CORT enables the reinstatement of the sensitive period (Moriceau, Wilson, Levine, & Sullivan, 2006; Upton & Sullivan 2010). Figure 2.2 This figure summarizes how the HPA axis, social buffering, and its impact on amygdala-dependent fear changes during development. In the youngest pups, during the sensitive period for attachment, the stress hyporesponsive period (SHRP) means pups have low CORT even when receiving stimuli such as shock and adult-like social buffering does not occur. This age range is associated with attachment learning with a wide range of stimuli, including milk, tactile stimulation, or pain from shock or an abusive mother. The maternal odor activates the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) and the prefrontal cortex (PFC). With maturation, pups enter the transitional sensitive period and amygdala-dependent fear learning occurs. However, maternal odor socially buffers pups, and entirely blocks CORT release and amygdala-dependent fear. Finally, as pups approach weaning and independence, the system becomes more adult-like with amygdala-dependent fear and social buffering that does not block fear learning. While social buffering at this age only blocks CORT release by half, additional blockade of CORT to more fully block CORT still does not reinstate attachment learning. This suggests a fundamental change in the ability of social buffering to alter pups' neurobehavioral function (Moriceau et al., 2006; Upton & Sullivan, 2010). Chapter 3 Figure 3.1 Infant marmoset vocalizations undergo dramatic acoustic changes. (A) Vocalizations from one infant. (B) Twitters and trills change usage whereas cries, phee- cries, and subharmonic-phees transition to phee calls. Figure 3.2 Babbling sequences and their similarity among twins. (A) Transition diagrams visualizing vocal sequences from two subjects at different postnatal time points. Each node in the diagram corresponds to a type of call, and the arrows correspond to the transitions between call types. The five most frequently produced call types are: phee (Ph), twitter (Tw), trill (Tr), cry (Cry), and phee-cry (P-C). Node size is proportional to the fraction of the call types, and edge size is proportional to the transition probability between calls. Thin dashed arrows are where transitions dropped below 5% occurrences. (B) Transition diagrams of vocal sequences from the first postnatal week for three sets of twins. Each twin set is arranged in the vertical order with the highlighted most frequent four-call transitions plotted on the right. (C) Comparison of JSDRs in three relationship categories: twins (= 5), nontwin siblings (n = 12), and nonsiblings (n = 28), p = 3.8e-5, ANOVA. Figure 3.3 Transition from cry to phee is influenced by contingent parental calls. (A) Weighted average entropy of infant calls produced before adult call onset and after adult call offset. The shaded regions indicate the respective 95% confidence intervals. (B) Correlations between the transition day and the proportion of contingent (left) and noncontingent (right) parental responses, respectively. Figure 3.4 Vocal-production learning by infant marmoset monkeys. (A) Twin infants received either high-contingency playbacks (100%) or low contingency playbacks (10%). Spectrograms depict when such playbacks were delivered relative to the infant vocalizations. (B) Wiener entropy (in decibels) changes over postnatal days for high and low contingency infants. (C) Dominant frequency (in kilohertz) changes over postnatal days for high and low contingency infants. Shaded regions indicate 1 standard error intervals. Figure 3.5 Physiological mechanisms of vocal development in marmoset monkeys. Figure shows a schematic illustrating spontaneous vocal production as a function of ANS oscillation and the threshold to vocalize. The continuously produced vocalizations by very young infant marmosets are driven by the natural rhythmic activity of respiration whose power is modulated by the slower, 0.1 Hz rhythm of the ANS. This consequently changes the quality of the vocalizations so that they fluctuate between high (cry) and low (phee) levels of entropy. Chapter 5 Figure 5.1 Converging evidence from social neuroscience and neurology demonstrates that brain regions underpinning moral reasoning are widely distributed and share computational resources with circuits controlling other capacities such as emotional saliency, mental state understanding, valuation of rewards, and decision-making. These regions include the posterior temporal cortex (pSTS) near the temporoparietal junction, amygdala, insula, ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC). Importantly, both empathic concern and moral decision-making require involvement of the vmPFC, a region that bridges conceptual and affective processes, necessary to guide moral behavior and decision-making. Human neuroimaging and primate electrophysiology studies show that the vmPFC tracks the personal subjective value of a wide range of stimuli during active decision-making and even in the absence of choice. Early damage to this region leads to impaired moral judgments and decision-making, a lack of concern for others, and failure to learn from repeated mistakes, despite normal intellect and explicit knowledge of the consequences of one's decisions. Chapter 6 Figure 6.1 Trying to understand a microprocessor by lesioning every single one of its transistors. The map of transistors (right) shows which locations, when lesioned, prevented the execution of one of three video games or their combination (inset: Donkey Kong, Space Invaders, or Pitfall). Yet these mere mappings produced no understanding of how the microprocessor works at all, illustrating that the functions implemented by individual transistors are difficult to map onto the overall function of the chip in playing video games. Figure 6.2 Levels of abstraction in a developmental social neuroscience. Inspired by David Marr's well-known scheme (Marr, 1982), and variously applied already to social cognition (Mitchell, 2006), the figure makes the point that we ultimately need to understand the adaptive problem in the service of which a particular mechanism was selected through evolution. Mediating between this broad driving force of evolution's design, and the causal mechanisms we can trace in the brain, are process-level computations or algorithms (although they need not be written as an equation or script) that serve to summarize how neurobiological data achieve the functions that they do. Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology Development of the Social Brain Volume 39 Edited by Jed T. Elison Maria D. Sera Copyright © 2018 by John Wiley & Sons, Inc. All rights reserved. Published by John Wiley & Sons, Inc., Hoboken, New Jersey. Published simultaneously in Canada. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, scanning, or otherwise, except as permitted under Section 107 or 108 of the 1976 United States Copyright Act, without either the prior written permission of the publisher, or authorization through payment of the appropriate per-copy fee to the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750- 8400, fax 978-646-8600, or on the Web at www.copyright.com. Requests to the publisher for permission should be addressed to the Permissions Department, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 111 River Street, Hoboken, NJ 07030, 201-748-6011, fax 201-748-6008, or online at www.wiley.com/go/permissions. Limit of Liability/Disclaimer of Warranty: While the publisher and author have used their best efforts in preparing this book, they make no representations or warranties with respect to the accuracy or completeness of the contents of this book and specifically disclaim any implied warranties of merchantability or fitness for a particular purpose. No warranty may be created or extended by sales representatives or written sales materials. The advice and strategies contained herein may not be suitable for your situation. You should consult with a professional where appropriate. Neither the publisher nor author shall be liable for any loss of profit or any other commercial damages, including but not limited to special, incidental, consequential, or other damages. Readers should be aware that Internet Web sites offered as citations and/or sources for further information may have changed or disappeared between the time this was written and when it is read. This publication is designed to provide accurate and authoritative information in regard to the subject matter covered. It is sold with the understanding that the publisher is not engaged in rendering professional services. If legal, accounting, medical, psychological or any other expert assistance is required, the services of a competent professional should be sought. For general information on our other products and services, please contact our Customer Care Department within the U.S. at 800-956-7739, outside the U.S. at 317-572-3986, or fax 317-572-4002. Wiley publishes in a variety of print and electronic formats and by print-on-demand. Some material included with standard print versions of this book may not be included in e-books or in print-on-demand. If this book refers to media such as a CD or DVD that is not included in the version you purchased, you may download this material at http://booksupport.wiley.com. For more information about Wiley products, visit www.wiley.com. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data is available ISBN 9781119461722 (Hardcover) ISBN 9781119461753 (ePDF) ISBN 9781119461739 (ePub) Cover Design: Wiley FIRST EDITION Preface The origin for what would become the Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, formally established in a 1966 meeting and subsequent volume the following year, can potentially be traced to an event in December 1955 organized by Dale Harris, director of the Institute of Child Development at the time. Among the participants at this meeting, and contributors to the volume published in 1957 as The Concept of Development: An Issue in the Study of Human Behavior, was T.C. Schneirla. Schnierla's theoretical and empirical contributions were rather disruptive at the time, and in some respect remain so, challenging popular notions of instincts and simplistic conceptualizations of maturational processes, among others. More relevant to our concerns, Schneirla's work consistently considered developmental change across ontogenetic and phylogenetic frames of reference. He also attempted to explicate a precise and parsimonious description of development, hence the invitation from Professor Harris. The concept of development would remain the organizing theme for the Minnesota Symposia meetings/volumes until the first topical meeting was held in 1977 on language development. This would become the 12th volume, published in 1979. How did the concept of development fill 11 volumes from 1967 to 1978? In his contribution to the eighth volume in 1974, Irv Gottesman paraphrased Paul Meehl's paraphrase of Albert Einstein: “The trouble with The Concept of Development – the annual organizing theme of these volumes – is that it is too difficult for developmental psychologists, and further, that it is too difficult for developmental biologists.” In partial homage to Schneirla and Gottesman, we opted to embrace the complexity of ontogenetic and phylogenetic development in the current volume. Ninety years after the founding of the Institute of Child Development, 70 years after Harris's meeting, nearly 40 years after Nicolas Humphrey's The Social Function of Intellect, 30 years since Michael Gazzaniga's The Social Brain: Discovering Networks of the Mind, and 25 years after Leslie Brothers published The Social Brain: A Project for Integrating Primate Behavior and Neurophysiology in a New Domain, we organized the 39th Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology around the topic of the Development of the Social Brain. I had preferred the title Phylogeny and Ontogeny of the Social Brain, but as we attempt to impress on our trainees, concise writing generally represents the best course of action (although the previous sentence may betray my career stage). This topic attempts a synthesis across two distinguishable lines of research: the phylogenetic line, which focuses on identifying the factors that could possibly account for the disproportionate expansion of the primate neocortex, and a second line of research focused on characterizing the conditions by which specific neural circuits become dedicated to processing social information across ontogeny. To this end, the primary objectives of the two-day symposium held in October of 2015 and this, the subsequent volume were fourfold: 1. To delineate the prerequisites for the existence of neural circuitry dedicated to processing

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.