ebook img

Control Motivation and Social Cognition PDF

352 Pages·1993·5.631 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Control Motivation and Social Cognition

Control Motivation and Social Cognition Gifford Weary Faith Gleicher Kerry L. Marsh Editors Control Motivation and Social Cognition With 23 Illustrations Springer-Verlag New York Berlin Heidelberg London Paris Tokyo Hong Kong Barcelona Budapest GIFFORD WEARY, Ph.D. Department of Psychology, Ohio State University, 142 Townsend Hall, 1885 Neil Avenue Mall, Columbus, OH 43210, USA FAITH GLEICHER, Ph.D. Department of Psychology, University of California at Santa Barbara, Santa Barbara, CA 93106, USA KERRY L. MARSH, Ph.D. Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN 46556, USA Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Control motivation and social cognition / Gifford Weary, Faith Gleicher, Kerry L. Marsh, editors. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references. ISBN-13: 978-1-4613-8311-6 e-ISBN-13: 978-1-4613-8309-3 DOl: 10.1007/978-1-4613-8309-3 1. Control (Psychology) 2. Motivation (Psychology) 3. Locus of control. 4. Social perception. I. Weary, Gifford, 1951- II. Gleicher, Faith. III. Marsh, Kerry L. BF611.C66 1993 153.8'5 - dc20 92-42359 Printed on acid-free paper. © 1993 Springer-Verlag New York, Inc. Softcover reprint ofthe hardcover 1s t edition 1993 All rights reserved. This work may not be translated or copied in whole or in part without the written permission of the publisher (Springer-Verlag New York, Inc., 175 Fifth Avenue, New York, NY 10010, USA), except for brief excerpts in connection with reviews or scholarly analysis. Use in connection with any form of information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software, or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed is forbidden. The use of general descriptive names, trade names, trademarks, etc., in this publication, even if the former are not especially identified, is not to be taken as a sign that such names, as understood by the Trade Marks and Merchandise Marks Act, may accordingly be used freely by anyone. Production coordinated by Chernow Editorial Services, Inc. and managed by Christin R. Ciresi; manufacturing supervised by Jacqui Ashri. Typeset by Best-Set Typesetter Ltd., Hong Kong. Printed and bound by Edwards Brothers, Inc., Ann Arbor, MI, USA. 987 6 5 4 3 2 1 To Our Husbands David A., David B., and Bill W. Preface Over the past two decades theorists and researchers have given increasing attention to the effects, both beneficial and harmful, of various control related motivations and beliefs. People's notions of how much personal control they have or desire to have over important events in their lives have been used to explain a host of performance and adaptational outcomes, including motivational and performance deficits associated with learned helplessness (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978) and depression (Abramson, Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989), adaptation to aging (Baltes & Baltes, 1986; Rodin, 1986), cardiovascular disease (Matthews, 1982), cancer (Sklar & Anisman, 1979), increased reports of physical symptoms (Pennebaker, 1982), enhanced learning (Savage, Perlmutter, & Monty, 1979), achievement-related behaviors (Dweck & Licht, 1980; Ryckman, 1979), and post abortion adjustment (Mueller & Major, 1989). The notion that control motivation plays a fundamental role in a variety of basic, social psychological processes also has a long historical tradition. A number of theorists (Heider, 1958; Jones & Davis, 1965; Kelley, 1967), for example, have suggested that causal inferences arise from a desire to render the social world predictable and controllable. Similarly, control has been implicated as an important mediator of cognitive dissonance (Wicklund & Brehm, 1976) and attitude phenomena (Brehm & Brehm, 1981; Kiesler, Collins, & Miller, 1969). Despite the apparent centrality of control motivation to a variety of social psychological phenomena, until recently there has been relatively little research explicitly concerned with the effects of control motivation on the cognitive processes underlying such phenomena (cf., Pittman & D' Agostino, 1985). This is no doubt due, in part, to the reign of "cog nitivism" and information-processing models of social behavior during the last decade. However, such a singular focus on "cold" cognition now seems to have given way to an increased interest in the interrelations of motivation and cognition-the so-called "warm look." As evidence of this new "Zeitgeist," we are seeing volumes such as the Handbook of Motivation and Cognition (Higgins & Sorrentino, 1990; Sorrentino & vii viii Preface Higgins, 1986) that include chapters focusing on many new and exciting research programs in the "warm look" tradition. In this book, we seek to present the only volume to date that highlights work on control motivation and social cognition. While there are other volumes (e.g., Higgins & Sorrentino, 1990) that examine a variety of motivational bases of social-cognitive processes, we believe that a volume devoted solely to work on control motivation and cognition is needed. Why? First, control has been viewed as a fundamental psychological need, and there is an enormous body of literature concerned with psy chological control. Second, there now exists a growing body of research concerned with the social-cognitive antecedents, correlates, and con sequences of a desire for control. Third, we believe that the two literatures referred to above have developed, in many instances, in parallel; much could be gained by a cross-fertilization of work on psychological control and on control-motivated social cognition. We believe that a systematic review of the literature on psychological control will offer new theoretical and methodological avenues for our understanding of control-motivated social cognition, and vice versa. In the current book, we attempted to bring together a broad collection of contributors whose research is at the forefront of the psychology of control and scholars whose work represents the many individual strands of research bridging control motivation and social cognition. The book is divided into four major sections. The first section is intended to present a discussion of the major theoretical perspectives on control. In his chapter on control and its loss, Jack W. Brehm (Chapter 1) sets the stage by defining what we mean by perceived control, various types of control beliefs that have been identified in the literature, and types of control motivation, no small task given the diversity of current definitions for all of these constructs. Brehm then turns to a discussion of a number of the currently prominent theories of control motivation and provides an overview of the strengths and short comings of these theories, as well as the evidence available with respect to them. In this context, particular attention is given by Brehm to reac tance theory; a basic premise of this chapter is that consideration of the reactance literature will do much to illustrate the types of variables and alternative interpretations that must be considered by any theory of psychological control. Each of the authors in Section II emphasizes the importance of perceived control for social and individual functioning. They agree that perceptions of control allow individuals to construct plans of future action and con tribute to their perceptions of well-being and satisfaction. The unique contributions of the chapters in this section, then, lie in their analyses of the determinants of control perceptions and, in the case of Anderson and Deuser's chapter, in its assessment of the significance of control motivation in attribution. One distinction that emerges among the approaches taken Preface ix in this section concerns the relationship of perceptions of response outcome contingencies and perceptions of control. Perceived behavioral contingency traditionally has been emphasized by learned helplessness theorists, and is also the focus of Lauren B. Alloy, Caroline M. Clements, and Linda J. Koenig's chapter (Chapter 2). These authors review a wide variety of research bearing on situational and personal determinants of perceptions of contingency and suggest that perceptions of control are likely to be multiply determined through the effects of both cognitive and motivational mechanisms on perceptions of contingency. They suggested self-esteem maintenance, impression management, and a commitment to goal achievement as motivational determinants; and focus on the effects of associative learning, expectancies, and self-focused attention as potential cognitive determinants. Miroslaw Kofta (Chapter 5), on the other hand, does not consider perceptions of response-outcome contingencies as central to perceptions of control. Rather, he proposes an "informational model of learned helplessness" that emphasizes the significance of individuals' ability to cognitively construct models of their environments. According to the informational model, people normally actively incorporate information from their environments in order to understand causal relations and construct a cognitive frame to guide their future behavior. Uncontrollable situations, with their lack of consistent informational feedback, prevent the individual from forming reasonable hypotheses about the nature of the situation. Kofta reviews evidence in support of his informational perspective and speculates provocatively about the implications of informational helplessness for depression, performance in school, creativity, and personality growth. A second area of difference among the chapters is in their domain of focus (i.e., laboratory or naturalistic settings). Whereas Kofta and Alloy and her colleagues focus primarily on laboratory studies in which individuals are temporarily deprived of control, Suzanne C. Thompson's research (Chapter 3) examines control perceptions in naturally occurring situations of low control. Her work with cancer and stroke patients and with members of health maintenance organizations also emphasizes the notion of multiple determinism of control perceptions. In her chapter, Thompson focuses on how three interacting factors-realism, optimism, and flexibility-might influence control perceptions in low-control settings. Finally, Craig A. Anderson and William E. Deuser (Chapter 4) ex plore the significance of the control dimension in the formation of attri butions and in the effects of attributions on thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. According to their "attributional functionalism" approach, attributions of both self and others serve the primary function of allowing for prediction and control of future outcomes. For this reason, these authors predict (and find) that when people think about causes, they typically think in terms of controllability and locus, the two dimensions x Preface that provide the most information about future control. Further, these two dimensions contribute most importantly to the effects of attributions on related thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. One way to recognize the instrumentality of a psychological variable is when its relative absence is as informative (and interesting) as its presence. For researchers interested in social cognition, such is the instrumental function of control motivation. In Section III, the authors provide over lapping, yet distinctive, approaches to understanding how our processing of social informat;on is shaped by the extent to which our control needs are satisfied. Whether exploring individual differences in control motivation, or the implications of control deprivation, or postulating a concomitant need for uncertainty-reduction, a prevailing view is that the motivation to gain control is manifested in a processing style commonly characterized as vigilant, effortful, extensive, and complex. Where such processing is thought to be less extensive or less accurate is in those situations where information processing may be relatively uninformative about control, when extensive processing will further threaten control, or when processing is skewed toward maintaining perceived control. Beyond this, the chapters in this section provide different perspectives on how to conceptualize and engage control motives. Control needs are construed by some as originating primarily from within the individual, either because of a generalized predisposition to desire control, or because of chronic prior exposure to threatened control, or rather as a reflection of a more general need to learn about one's capabilities. Other authors emphasize that situational or interactional forces can induce control needs, or threaten perceived control. That control motives can be engaged by the situation is seen in the two chapters (6 and 7) which address the role of control motivation in the areas of interpersonal influence and power. In Thane S. Pittman's chapter (6), he suggests that events that threaten control can result in temporary boosts in control motivation. As a result, control-deprived persons often process information in a more accurate and effortful manner. Pittman extends his previous work on control deprivation into the domain of attitude change and finds that control-deprived persons are more persuaded by the strength of the message content than by more superficial heuristic cues in a persuasive message. In the next chapter, Eric Depret and Susan T. Fiske (Chapter 7) provide a compelling case for an interactional under standing of control motivation, suggesting that social power (control over others' outcomes), rather than residing in the individual, is better con ceptualized as a structural characteristic of relationships. They suggest that when one's outcomes are asymmetrically dependent on the other, this power imbalance can result in a feeling of control deprivation. More over, such a loss of social power results in increased attention to the other person, and increased dispositional attributions about the powerful other. Preface xi The remaining three chapters in this section illustrate how individual differences in control needs impact the way people seek out, attend to, process, and behave in response to control-relevant information in a variety of domains. Jerry M. Burger (Chapter 8) examines the individual difference variable of desire for control. His research demonstrates that persons high in desire for control are particularly attentive to control information, and show attributional patterns consistent with a heightened desire to see events as under their control. Finally, he suggests that people high in desire for control may be more susceptible to gambling and depression as a result of motivated responsivity to control information. Michael J. Strube and John H. Yost (Chapter 9) present their self-appraisal model, an outgrowth and extension of earlier research on type A behavior and reactivity to control loss. Their dynamic model of self-seeking suggests that control over the environment is essential for accurate self-appraisal; moreover, persons high in desire for self-appraisal are especially motivated to reduce uncertainty about their abilities. For example, they show more accurate control perceptions when tasks are diagnostic about their abilities. Finally, Gifford Weary, Kerry L. Marsh, Faith Gleicher, and John A. Edwards (Chapter 10) present a model of how individual dif ferences in depression impact causal uncertainty, and consequently, information processing in those social tasks that lack clear outcome criteria. Their research suggests that mild to moderately depressed individuals show increased sensitivity and seeking of social informa tion in simple impression formation or social comparison tasks, and reveal more extensive, complex, and effortful modes of processing such information. In the final section, Richard M. Sorrentino presents a classification of each of the chapters in terms of their relative positions along three dimensions. The first dimension has to do with whether the theory presented is primarily motivational, primarily cognitive, or both. The second dimension is a rationallnonrational dimension that asks whether the chapter focuses on the rational, thoughtful processing of social information, or nonrational, nonconscious cognitive processes. The third dimension asks whether the theory concerns individual differences in control motivation and social cognition. This classification scheme and the presentation of several important issues addressed in the author's own research program on uncertainty orientation prove extremely useful in highlighting points of convergence and divergence of, as well as con ceptual lacunae common to the various chapters in this volume. This volume is intended for a graduate and professional-level audience. We have tried to emphasize ideas and approaches that we believe will have durability and that will influence in a substantial way the future of work on control-motivated social cognition. We believe the volume will be appropriate as a supplementary text in basic, graduate-level courses in social psychology or as the sole text for advanced seminars. Further, we xii Preface believe that the book will be useful not only to social psychologists, but also to scholars in clinical and personality psychology. Finally, we wish to express our sincere thanks to many people. Foremost, we must thank the contributors of this volume without whose enthusiasm, hard work, and adherence to oftentimes difficult schedules this book would not have been possible. Second, we would like to thank our publisher, Springer-Verlag, whose support was invaluable. Third, we would like to thank Shannon Riley for her help with the preparation of parts of this manuscript. A final thanks goes to our husbands whose patience and support was essential during the planning stages and through out the entire project. GIFFORD WEARY FAITH GLEICHER KERRY L. MARSH

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.