ebook img

ERIC EJ941322: College-Level Instruction: Derived Relations and Programmed Instruction PDF

2011·0.13 MB·English
by  ERIC
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview ERIC EJ941322: College-Level Instruction: Derived Relations and Programmed Instruction

JOURNALOFAPPLIEDBEHAVIORANALYSIS 2011, 44, 413–416 NUMBER2 (SUMMER2011) COLLEGE-LEVEL INSTRUCTION: DERIVED RELATIONS AND PROGRAMMED INSTRUCTION DANIEL M. FIENUP QUEENSCOLLEGEANDTHEGRADUATECENTEROFTHECITY UNIVERSITYOFNEWYORK JEFFERY HAMELIN AND KIMBERLY REYES-GIORDANO THEGRADUATECENTEROFTHECITYUNIVERSITYOF NEWYORK AND TERRY S. FALCOMATA UNIVERSITYOFTEXASATAUSTINANDTHEMEADOWS CENTERFORPREVENTINGEDUCATIONALRISK Recent research has demonstrated the effectiveness of programmed instruction that integrates derivedrelationstoteachcollege-levelacademicmaterial.Thismethodhasbeendemonstratedto be effective and economical in the teaching of complex mathematics and biology concepts. Althoughthisapproachmayhavepotentialapplicationswithotherdomainsofcollegelearning, more studies are needed to evaluate important technological variables. Studies that employ programmed instructionare discussed inrelation tofuturedirections for research. Keywords: college students, derived relations, programmed instruction _______________________________________________________________________________ Recent advances in college-level instruction and stimulus types might consist of class name have included programming for derived rela- (i.e., mammal and reptile), skin type (i.e., furry tions. A derived relation is an outcome that and scaly), birth method (i.e., live and egg), and results from directly teaching the interconnec- thermophysiology (i.e., warm-blooded and cold- tedness of stimuli. The most important type of blooded). Direct teaching of class names with derived relation involves relating two or more each stimulus type would likely result in the stimuli that have never been directly paired but emergence of relations between the stimuli share an association with a common stimulus without directly teaching those relations. These (Critchfield & Fienup, 2008). Programming applications are economical in that individuals for derived relations is accomplished by orga- demonstrate adaptive behavior in excess of that nizing stimuli according to classes and types of which was directly taught (Fienup & Critch- stimuli (e.g., see Figure 1 of Fields et al., 2009) field, 2010; Stromer, MacKay, & Stoddard, and determining what is likely to emerge, given 1992; Toussaint & Tiger, 2010). therelationsdirectlytaught.Forexample,when Both relational frame theory (RFT, Hayes, teaching about different classes in biology, Barnes-Holmes,&Roche,2001;Ninnessetal., classes might consist of mammal and reptile, 2009) and stimulus equivalence (Fields et al., 2009) paradigms have accounted for derived Address correspondence to Daniel M. Fienup, Depart- relations. From an application standpoint, both mentofPsychology,QueensCollege,65-30KissenaBlvd., paradigms are inherently interested in classes of Flushing, New York 11367 (e-mail: daniel.fienup@ physically different stimuli that are functionally qc.cuny.edu). doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-413 related. Instructional applications from both 413 414 DANIEL M. FIENUP et al. paradigms involve teaching the minimal num- of48relations(i.e.,fourtimesasmanyrelations ber of relations between members of a class so learned as taught). The authors also found that that all relations between stimuli emerge in an students were able to generalize responding to efficient manner. novel graphs. Additional examples were provided by College-Level Instructional Research on Fienup and Critchfield (2010) and Critchfield Derived Relations and Fienup (2010) in teaching concepts of Typically, derived relations research has inferential statistics and hypothesis decision involved instruction on how various represen- making.Inthefirstlesson,studentsweretaught tations of stimuli are positively related. The two statistical categories: one that involved majority of these applications have focused on statistically significant stimuli and one that statistical and mathematical concepts. For involvestatisticallynonsignificantstimuli.Next, example, Ninness et al. (2006, 2009) pro- students were taught to match hypothesis- and grammed for derived relations when teaching results-basedstatementstostatementsaboutthe college students trigonometry and algebraic null hypothesis (reject or fail to reject) and concepts. The instructional package included scientific hypothesis (consistent or inconsis- verbal rules and match-to-sample training. The tent). Last, students were taught to incorporate stimuli involved standard equations (y 5 6 2 statistical information with hypotheses and !2x24),factoredequations(y5!2[x+4]+ results to make accurate decisions about 6),andgraphicalrepresentations.First,students hypotheses. In total, students were directly were taught rules about how the stimuli were taught 40 relations and demonstrated up to related. Next, students received computerized 144 relations (i.e., 4.7 times as many relations match-to-sample training of the respective learned as taught). In addition to these stimuli. Following instruction, students consis- demonstrations of the application of stimulus tentlydemonstratednovelformsofbehavior,or equivalence to math instruction, similar appli- derived relations, such as matching graphs with cations have also been demonstrated with standard equations. In addition, they general- brain–behavior concepts (e.g., Fienup, Covey, ized responding to novel types of functions. & Critchfield, 2010) and disability categoriza- Recently,Ninnessetal.(2009)incorporatedthe tion (Walker, Rehfeldt, & Ninness, 2010). construction of graphs. This approach allowed students to complete complex reciprocal trigo- Future Directions nometric relations. Fields et al. (2009) taught college students Several areas are in need of future research. conceptsofstatisticalinteractions.Studentswere For example, a number of new content areas taughtfouruniquecategoriesofinteractions(no could benefit from instruction that programs interaction,divergent,synergistic,crossover)and for derived relations. Areas such as geography, four different representations of the category the taxonomy of biological organisms, psychi- (graph, a description of the interaction repre- atric disorder categorization, and language all sented in the graph, name of interaction, and involvemultiplerepresentationsofconceptually definition of interaction). Using only match-to- relatedstimuli.Someoftheseareashavealready sample training, the experimenter taught the been investigated with young learners (e.g., students three relations. Following teaching, geography,LeBlanc,Miguel,Cummings,Gold- students generalized responding to novel stimu- smith,&Carr,2003;foreignlanguage,Joyce& lus relations, including matching graphs and Joyce, 1993) and could be extended to include definitions. Instruction involving 12 relations more complex topics that are taught in college (threerelationsbyfourclasses)resultedinatotal curricula. BRIEF REVIEW 415 A significant practical challenge involves the It is important to determine which of these technology for promoting derived relations. inputs are necessary and sufficient when Other areas within applied behavior analysis designing efficient instruction. It is not enough have agreed-upon technology (e.g., functional to claim an instructional package is efficient by analysis, Hanley, Iwata, & McCord, 2003), but illustrating that the stimulus relations output there is no consensus in the literature on what exceeds the input. The actual teaching portions experiences a college student requires to derive of these computerized programs are likely relations reliably. The lack of consensus on how efficient. What has been given less attention is to program for derived relations can be a the intensive nature of the testing batteries. deterrent to new experimenters and instructors Measuring the emergence of derived relations is whowanttoprogramforderivedrelations.Areas typically done across many trials (e.g., Fienup of divergence in the literature include the et al., 2010, involved a minimum of 448 test sequence of teaching, the sequence of testing trials). Programmed instruction is a package, and training, the content of testing, the mastery and research is needed to determine how to criterion,andthetypeandfrequencyoffeedback. measure the emergence of derived relations This issue can be illustrated by comparing accurately while maintaining an efficient pro- and contrasting research. Ninness et al. (2006) gram of instruction. combinedmatch-to-sample trainingwithverbal College-level instruction that programs for rules about how the stimuli were related, derived relations has shown promise for future whereas Fields et al. (2009) used match-to- applications. This type of instruction, whether sample training alone. Both used linear-series using an RFT or stimulus equivalence para- training (e.g., ARB, BRC), but Ninness et al. digm, can be applied to a variety of academic preceded and followed this training with mixed contents. However, more research is needed to tests of all relations. Fields et al., on the other refine the technology of programming for hand, interspersed additional tests before, derived relations in instructional settings, espe- during, and after training to measure the cially if the goal is wider acceptance, use, and gradual emergence of derived relations. application of derived relations. The field has no agreed-upon approach for even seemingly simple parameters such as type REFERENCES and frequency of feedback. Fields et al. (2009) Critchfield, T. S., & Fienup, D. M. (2008). Stimulus providedvisualcorrectivefeedbackoneachtrial equivalence. In S. F. Davis & W. F. Buskist (Eds.), and then faded feedback across trials. These 21st century psychology (pp. 360–372). Thousand Oaks,CA:Sage. authors evaluated mastery based on blocks of Critchfield, T. S., & Fienup, D. M. (2010). Using trials, meaning that after the passage of a set stimulus equivalence technology to teach about number of trials, the computer program statistical inference in a group setting. Journal of evaluated whether a criterion had been met Applied BehaviorAnalysis,43,437–462. Fields, L., Travis, R., Yadlovker, E. D., Roy, D., Aguiar- since the last evaluation. More recently, Fienup Rocha, L., & Sturmey, P. (2009). Equivalence class et al. (2010) incorporated a rolling mastery formation: A method for teaching statistical interac- criterion. Participants were required to respond tions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 575–593. correctly on a certain number of consecutive Fienup,D.M.,Covey,D.P.,&Critchfield,T.S.(2010). trials. These authors also provided auditory Teaching brain–behavior relations economically with feedback and visual feedback that allowed stimulus equivalence technology. Journal of Applied participants to view how many trials were on Behavior Analysis,43,19–33. Fienup, D. M., & Critchfield, T. S. (2010). Efficiently a test or how many consecutive trials on which establishing concepts of inferential statistics and they had responded correctly. hypothesis decision making through contextually 416 DANIEL M. FIENUP et al. controlled equivalence classes. Journal of Applied Ninness,C.,Dixon,M.,Barnes-Holmes,D.,Rehfeldt,R. BehaviorAnalysis,43,437–462. A., Rumph, R., McCuller, G., et al. (2009). Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). Constructing and deriving reciprocal trigonometric Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. relations: A functional analytic approach. Journal of JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysis,36,147–185. AppliedBehaviorAnalysis,42,191–208. Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (Eds.). Stromer, R., Mackay, H. A., & Stoddard, L. T. (1992). (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian Classroom applications of stimulus equivalence account of human language and cognition. New York: technology. Journal of Behavioral Education, 2, KluwerAcademic/Plenum. 225–256. Joyce, B. G., & Joyce, J. H. (1993). Using stimulus Toussaint, K. A., & Tiger, J. H. (2010). Teaching early equivalenceprocedurestoteachrelationshipsbetween braille literacy skills within a stimulus equivalence EnglishandSpanishwords.Education&Treatmentof paradigm to children with degenerative visual im- Children,16,48–65. pairments. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, LeBlanc, L. A., Miguel, C. F., Cummings, A. R., 181–194. Goldsmith, T. R., & Carr, J. E. (2003). The effects Walker, B. D., Rehfeldt, R. A., & Ninness, C. (2010). of three stimulus-equivalence testing conditions on Using the stimulus equivalence paradigm to teach emergent US geography relations of children diag- course material in an undergraduate rehabilitation nosed with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 18, course. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 279–289. 615–633. Ninness, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rumph, R., McCuller, G., Ford, A. M., Payne, R., et al. (2006). Transfor- Received August 30,2010 mations of mathematical and stimulus functions. Final acceptance October21,2010 JournalofAppliedBehavior Analysis,39,299–321. Action Editor,Dorothea Lerman

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.