ebook img

Young-Adult Male Rats' Vulnerability to Chronic Mild Stress Is Reflected by Anxious-Like instead of PDF

13 Pages·2016·1.5 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Young-Adult Male Rats' Vulnerability to Chronic Mild Stress Is Reflected by Anxious-Like instead of

Hindawi Publishing Corporation Neuroscience Journal Volume 2016, Article ID 5317242, 12 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/5317242 Research Article Young-Adult Male Rats’ Vulnerability to Chronic Mild Stress Is Reflected by Anxious-Like instead of Depressive-Like Behaviors Herrera-PérezJoséJaime,1Benítez-CoronelVenus,1Jiménez-RubioGraciela,1 Hernández-HernándezOliviaTania,2andMartínez-MotaLucía1 1LaboratoriodeFarmacolog´ıaConductual,Direccio´ndeInvestigacionesenNeurociencias, InstitutoNacionaldePsiquiatr´ıaRamo´ndelaFuenteMun˜iz,CalzadaMe´xico-Xochimilco101,ColoniaSanLorenzoHuipulco, Delegacio´nTlalpan,14370CiudaddeMe´xico,DF,Mexico 2ConsejoNacionaldeCienciayTecnolog´ıa,InstitutoNacionaldePsiquiatr´ıaRamo´ndelaFuenteMun˜iz, CalzadaMe´xico-Xochimilco101,ColoniaSanLorenzoHuipulco,Delegacio´nTlalpan,14370CiudaddeMe´xico,DF,Mexico CorrespondenceshouldbeaddressedtoMart´ınez-MotaLuc´ıa;[email protected] Received11April2016;Accepted1June2016 AcademicEditor:NashatAbumaria Copyright©2016Herrera-Pe´rezJose´Jaimeetal. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons AttributionLicense,whichpermitsunrestricteduse,distribution,andreproductioninanymedium,providedtheoriginalworkis properlycited. Inapreviousstudy,wefoundthatchronicmildstress(CMS)paradigmdidnotinduceanhedoniainyoung-adultmaleratsbutit reducedtheirbodyweightgain.Thesecontrastingresultsencouragedustoexploreotherindicatorsofanimal’svulnerabilitytostress suchasanxious-likebehaviors,sincestressisanetiologicfactoralsoforanxiety.Thus,inthisstudy,weevaluatedthevulnerability oftheseanimalstoCMSusingbehavioraltestsofdepressionoranxietyandmeasuringserumcorticosterone.MaleWistarrats wereexposedtofourweeksofCMS;theanimals’bodyweightandsucrosepreference(indicatorofanhedonia)wereassessedafter threeweeks,and,afterthefourthweek,someanimalswereevaluatedinabehavioralbattery(elevatedplusmaze,defensiveburying behavior,andforcedswimmingtests);meanwhile,otherswereusedtomeasureserumcorticosterone.WefoundthatCMS(1) didnotaffectsucrosepreference,immobilitybehaviorintheforcedswimmingtest,orserumcorticosterone;(2)decreasedbody weightgain;and(3)increasedtherat’sentriesintoclosedarmsoftheplusmazeandthecumulativeburyingbehavior.Thesedata indicatethatyoungmalerats’vulnerabilitytoCMSisreflectedaspoorbodyweightgainandanxious-likeinsteadofdepressive-like behaviors. 1.Introduction response, which in turn would cause depressive or anxious events[3,4]. AccordingtoSeyle[1,2],stressisdefinedasthenonspecific Depression is a mood disorder characterized by anhe- responseofthebodytoanydemand.Thisresponseisinduced donia (incapacity to experience pleasure), feelings of sad- by any new situation (chemical, physical, environmental, ness and guilt, depressed mood, hopelessness, and suicidal emotional, and psychosocial stressor) which disturbs the thoughts[5];meanwhile,anxietyisdistinguishedbyexcessive homeostasisandinducesageneraladaptiveresponseaimed andinappropriateworrying(i.e.,persistentandnotrestricted at restoring the initial level of stability (adaptation). All the to particular circumstances), restlessness, difficulty concen- manifestationsoftheadaptiveresponsearebeneficialtothe trating, irritability, muscle tension, and disturbed sleep [5]. organismwhenlimitedintime,butwhenthedurationofthe To study these pathologies, researchers have used animal stressisexcessiveitcontributestothedevelopmentofpatho- models to simulate a symptom of the human disease [6]; logicalconditions[1,2].Thus,thediathesis-stresshypothesis most of these models are based on rodent exposure to a ofaffectivedisordersstatesthatchronicstresscouldinduce stressor.Chronicmildstress(CMS)isananimalmodelthat physiologicalalterationsthatpromoteanexaggeratedstress simulates anhedonia; in this paradigm, constant exposure 2 NeuroscienceJournal of the animal to several stressors of moderate intensity 2.2.2. Baseline Sucrose Consumption. The baseline sucrose duringaperiodofseveralweeksleadstoareductionofthe consumption was determined two days after the training consumption of a palatable sucrose solution, indicating an periodatthebeginningofthedarkphase(at10:00a.m.).For anhedonic state [7]. It has been established that this model thispurpose,theratswerewaterandfooddeprivedfor20h hashighvalidity(face,predictive,andconstructvalidity)that andthereafterpresentedwithtwobottlesduringaperiodof makesitausefultooltostudytheneurobiologyofdepression onehour:onecontainingsucrosesolution(1%)andtheother [8]. In agreement with the diathesis-stress hypothesis of tap water. The objective of the food and water deprivation affective disorders, several studies have found that rodent’s periodistostimulatetheliquidconsumptionintheanimals exposure to CMS can induce several behavioral alterations inordertohaveagooddynamicrangetoevaluatedifferences (differentfromanhedonia)thatmimichumansymptomsof in fluid intake [16]. Fluid (sucrose solution or tap water) depression [8–10] and anxiety [11, 12]. Recently, using this consumption was calculated by weighing the bottles before andafterexposuretotheanimals.Forbaselinebodyweight model,wefoundthatahighproportionofyoung-adultmale measurements, animals were weighed immediately before rats did not develop anhedonia when exposed to a CMS waterandfooddeprivation. paradigm,suggestingthattheseanimalsareresilienttothis kind of stress [13]. Contrary to this, we also found that the CMS reduced the rats’ body weight gain as compared to 2.2.3.ExperimentalDesignforBehavioralStudy. Afterdeter- unstressedrats[13].Thesedataindicatethatthechronicstress minationofbaselinesucroseconsumption,young-adultmale scheduleaffectsthephysiologyoftheanimal,butthisisnot ratswererandomlyassignedtoacontrolorstressgroup.The enoughtoinduceananhedonicstate.Theseresultsencourage malesinthecontrolgroup(𝑛 = 13)weremaintainedduring finding other indicators to evaluate the vulnerability of a period of four weeks without stress, but under similar young-adult rats to CMS; this could be done by scanning handling and storage conditions to the stressed animals. a broader spectrum of behaviors or phenotypes potentially The stress group (𝑛 = 13) was exposed, during a period impaired or induced by this kind of stress and by mea- of four weeks, to several stressors: white noise (∼90dB), suringtheactivityofhypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal(HPA) overcrowding (2-3 animals per cage), continuous lighting, axis. soiledcage(250mLwaterspilledintobedding),stroboscopic ∘ Onthesebases,wehypothesizethattheyoungratsthat light (300flashes/min), 45 cage tilt along the vertical axis, did not develop anhedonia when exposed to CMS present andwaterdeprivation.Thestressorschedulefollowedinthis other behavioral alterations that suggest depression and/or studywaspreviouslyused[13,17,18]anditisshowninTable1. anxiety.Thus,theobjectiveofthisstudywastoevaluatethe To determine anhedonia development, we used a within- vulnerability of young-adult rats to CMS using behavioral subject design since the effect of stress on hedonic state is tests that detect depressive- or anxious-like behaviors and progressive and the kinetics of the changes in fluid intake measuringcorticosteroneserumlevels. are importantto determine theestablishment ofanhedonia in the animals. Thus, during the first three weeks of CMS, sucroseandwaterintakewasdeterminedweekly,asindicated 2.MaterialsandMethods below. At the fourth week (day 27), the 20h of water and food deprivation indicated in Table1 was omitted and at 2.1. Animals. Young (3-month-old) male Wistar rats were this time the CMS was finished. Afterwards, a behavioral obtained from the vivarium of the Instituto Nacional de tests battery was performed to evaluate stress vulnerability Psiquiatr´ıaRamo´ndelaFuenteMun˜iz (INPRFM).Animals wereindividuallyhousedincagesmeasuring27×16×23cm beyond anhedonia; we used a between-subject design in order to avoid ceiling effects or reduction of exploration andmaintainedonaninverted12hdark-lightcyclecondition originatedbyrepeatedanimals’exposuretothetests.Then, (lights off at 10:00a.m.), under controlled temperature and onday28,thecontrolandstressedgroupswereexposedto humidity. The animals had free access to water and food, the elevated plus maze test and, immediately after, to the except for the periods required by the CMS procedure. defensiveburyingbehaviortest.Ondays29and30,animals Animal management was done according to the general were evaluated in the forced swimming test (pretest and principles of laboratory animal care [14]. All experimental test,resp.,at2:00p.m.).Severalstudiesindicatethatanimal’s procedureswereperformedinaccordancewiththeMexican performanceinthesebehavioraltestsissensibletoprevious officialnormforanimalcareandhandling[15]andapproved stressexposure,includingCMS[19–22];thus,theycouldbe by the Ethical Committee of the INPRFM. All efforts were good indicators of stress vulnerability in the nonanhedonic made to minimize the number of animals used and their rats. The tests are described below and the time flow of suffering. experimentalmanipulationsisindicatedinFigure1. 2.2.ChronicMildStressModel 2.3. Sucrose Preference Test and Body Weight Measurement. 2.2.1. Sucrose Consumption Training. Animals were allowed Incontrolandstressedanimals,thesucrosesolutionandtap to adapt to the taste of a palatable sucrose solution (1%) waterconsumptionweredeterminedweeklyasindicatedin for two weeks. During this period, a bottle containing the baseline sucrose consumption section. These measure- sucrose solution was presented to the rats daily for one ments (for the control and stressed animals) were done at hour. 10:00a.m.,aftera20hperiodofwaterandfooddeprivation. NeuroscienceJournal 3 Table1:Chronicmildstressschedule. Time(hours) FirstWed. Thr. Fri. Sat. Sun. Mon. Tue. EverysubsequentWed. 7:00-8:00 SC/CL WD O/CL FD/WD 8:00-9:00 SC/CL WD CT/CL FD/WD 9:00-10:00 CL WD CT/CL FD/WD 10:00-11:00 Baseline SL WN CT/CL SPT 11:00-12:00 O SL WN CT/CL 12:00-13:00 O SL WN SL CT 13:00-14:00 WN O SL WN SL CT WN 14:00-15:00 WN O SL SL FD/WD WN 15:00-16:00 WN O/CL SL FD/WD WN 16:00-17:00 SC/CL WD SL FD/WD 17:00–7:00 SC/CL WD O/CL FD/WD WN:whitenoise(∼90dB);O:overcrowding(2-3ratspercage);CL:continuouslighting;SC:soiledcage(250mLwaterspilledintobedding);SL:stroboscopic ∘ light(300flashes/min);WD:waterdeprivation;CT:cagetilt(45);FD:fooddeprivation;SPT:sucrosepreferencetest. Elevated plus maze and defensive burying Sucrose consumption Control or CMS training Time −14 0 21 28 29 30 (days) Baseline Determination Pretest Test sucrose of anhedonia Forced swimming intake test Figure1:Timelineforexperimentalmanipulations. Rat’sbodyweightwasmeasuredweekly,immediatelybefore periodof10minforposterioranalysis.Themazewascleaned the 20h of water and food deprivation. To avoid changes aftereachtesting.Thenumberofentriesandthetimespent determined by baseline differences, individual sucrose and intheclosedoropenarmsweremeasured.Anincrementof water consumption were expressed as relative sucrose or rat’s anxiety-like state induced by CMS would be indicated water intake; this parameter was calculated by dividing the byincreasingthetimespentorthenumberofentriesinthe sucroseorwaterconsumptionatanytimebytherespective closedarms[24]. baselineingestion;thisparameterhasstatisticalsensibilityto reductionsinsucrosesolutionintake[18].Sucrosepreference 2.5. Defensive Burying Behavior Test. The burying behavior was calculated weekly by dividing the sucrose intake by test was used to assess experimental anxiety and it is based total fluid (sucrose plus water) consumption and the result ontherat’sinnatebehaviortoburyaversivestimuli[25].The was expressed as percentage. Finally, rat’s body weight was expressedasapercentagerelativetobaselinebodyweightin test was performed in a cage measuring 27 × 16 × 23cm ordertodiscardeffectsofbaselinedifferences. withanelectrifiedprobe(7cmlong,2cmabovethebedding The anhedonic state generated by the exposure to CMS material) placed in one of the cage walls. When the rat schedule would be indicated by a reduction in the weekly touched the electrified probe with its snout or forepaws, it sucroseconsumptionorpreferenceafterthreeweeksofstress received a 0.3mA shock. The experiments were performed [7,13,18]. under dim red light. The rats were exposed to this cage during a period of 10min, and the test was videotaped for 2.4. Elevated Plus Maze Test. The elevated plus maze is an posterioranalysis.Thenumberofprodshocks,theburying animal model of anxiety based on rodents’ aversion for behaviorlatency(timebetweenthefirstshockandthedisplay openspaces[23].Thetestsettingconsistedofaplus-shaped of burying behavior), and the cumulative burying behavior apparatus(armssize:50×10cm)placed50cmabovethefloor (total amount of time spent spraying the bedding material withtwoopenandtwoclosedarms(enclosedby40cmhigh towardsandontopoftheshockprobe)weremeasured.An walls).Theexperimentswereperformedunderdimredlight. anxiogenic effect of CMS would be indicated by a decrease Theratswereplacedattheintersectionofthefourarmsofthe of the burying behavior latency and/or an increase of the mazefacinganopenarm.Thetestwasvideotapedduringa cumulativeburyingbehavior. 4 NeuroscienceJournal 2.6.ForcedSwimmingTest. Theforcedswimmingtest(FST) test; and (e) corticosterone serum levels. Statistical analysis is a commonly used animal model of depression that was carried out using the Sigma Plot software, version 11. evaluates behavioral despair. In this study, we used the A value of 𝑝 < 0.05 was considered statistically signifi- modified FST [26]. Swimming sessions were conducted by cant. placing the rat in a glass cylinder (46cm tall × 20cm in ∘ diameter) containing water (30cm depth) at 23–25 C. The 3.Results FST consisted of two swimming sessions 24h apart. In the first session (pretest), rats were allowed to swim for 15min. 3.1.AnhedoniaDevelopmentintheCMS. Figure2showsthe Twenty-four hours later, animals were subjected to a 5min sucrose intake (a), the tap water consumption (b), and the swimmingsession(test).Attheendofeachsession,ratswere sucrosepreference(c)ofratsexposedtoCMS(𝑛=13,black removed from the jar, dried with paper towel, and placed circles)ormaintainedwithoutstress(𝑛 = 13,whitecircles). in a cage for 15min before returning them to the home The two-way RM ANOVA indicated that sucrose solution cages.Thetestwasvideotapedandatime-samplingtechnique intakewasnotaffectedbystress(𝐹1,24 = 2.537,𝑝 = 0.124), was used to score, every 5s, the presence of immobility timeofexposure(𝐹3,72 =2.175,𝑝=0.098),ortheinteraction behavior(floatingwithoutstrugglingandmakingonlythose ofbothfactors(𝐹3,72 =0.975,𝑝=0.409).Inasimilarway,tap movements necessary to keep the head above the water). waterconsumptiondidnotshowsignificantdifferences(RM Behavioral despair would be indicated by high levels of ANOVA,stress:𝐹1,24 = 0.172,𝑝 = 0.682;timeofexposure: immobility. 𝐹3,72 = 2.151,𝑝 = 0.101;andinteraction:𝐹3,72 = 1.248,𝑝 = 0.299).Regardingsucrosepreference,thestatisticsindicated 2.7. Corticosterone Serum Measurements. To evaluate the differences determined by time (𝐹3,72 = 3.011, 𝑝 = 0.036) effectofCMSonHPAactivity,wemeasuredcorticosterone butnotbystress(𝐹1,24 =1.385,𝑝=0.251)ortheinteraction serumlevels.Ithasbeendemonstratedthatrats’exposureto of both factors (𝐹3,72 = 0.584, 𝑝 = 0.627); the main effect oftimeisduetoincreaseofpreferencealongtheexperiment behavioral tests such as the FST could increase their HPA (Figure2(c)). axis activity [27]; this effect could veil the impact of the CMS protocol on the function of this axis. Thus, in the current study, the corticosterone levels were measured in 3.2. Relative Body Weight. Relative body weight of rats two independent groups of rats that were experimentally exposedtoCMS(𝑛=13,blackcircles)ormaintainedwithout manipulated as indicated above (see Figure1), except that stress (𝑛 = 13, white circles) is shown in Figure3. Data theywerenotexposedtotheelevatedplusmaze,thedefensive analysisindicatesasignificanteffectofstress(𝐹1,24 =37.919, behavior, or the forced swimming tests. During a period 𝑝 < 0.001),timeofexposure(𝐹3,72 =6.762,𝑝 < 0.001),and of four weeks, one of these groups was exposed to CMS theinteractionofbothfactors(𝐹3,72=10.213,𝑝<0.001).Post (𝑛 = 7);meanwhile,theotherwaskeptoncontrolcondition hocanalysisindicatedthatthebodyweightofcontrolratsat (𝑛 = 6); according to the sucrose preference, after three week 1, 2, or 3 is higher than their basal value (𝑝 < 0.001 weeksofCMS,noneofthestressedratsdevelopedanhedonia inallcases);meanwhile,inthestressedgroup,onlythebody (data not shown). On day 28, the animals of both groups weightatweek1waslowerthantheirbasallevel(𝑝=0.041). were sacrificed by decapitation and their trunk blood was Tukeytestalsoevidencedthenotionthatbodyweightofthe collectedincoldtubes.Thebloodwascentrifuged(4000rpm stressedgroupwaslowerthantheweightinthecontrolgroup for25minat4∘C)toobtainserumsamplesthatwerestored atweeks1,2,and3(𝑝<0.001inallcases). at −4∘C until analysis. Corticosterone concentrations were measured by radioimmunoassay using a commercial kit 3.3. Anxiety-Like Behavior in the Elevated Plus Maze. The (Siemens Rat Corticosterone Coat-A-Count Kit, TKRC1); performance of stressed (𝑛 = 13, gray bars) or unstressed 125 this procedure used antibody-coated tubes in which I- (𝑛 = 13,blackbars)maleratsontheelevatedplusmazetest labelledratcorticosteronecompetedwiththefreehormone is shown in Figure4. The time spent in the open or closed in the sample for antibody sites. Corticosterone (ng/mL) arm (Figure4(a)) did not present significant differences totalquantitywasdeterminedusingacalibrationcurve.The determinedbyCMS(𝑝 = 0.606,𝑝 = 0.445,resp.).Similarly, interassay and intra-assay variabilities were 4.8 and 4.0%, thenumberofentriesintotheopenarm(Figure4(b))didnot respectively. showsuchdifference(𝑝 = 0.667);incontrast,thestatistical analysisofthenumberofentriesintotheclosedarmindicated that the stressed group showed more entrances than the 2.8.Statistics. Tapwaterorsucrosesolutionintake,sucrose unstressedgroup(𝑝=0.010). preference, and relativebody weight were analyzed by two- way repeated measures analysis of variance (RM ANOVA) including the factors stress exposure and time, followed 3.4.DefensiveBuryingBehavior. Animalsthatdidnotreceive by Tukey as the post hoc test. Student’s 𝑡-test was used to a shock (2 stressed and 4 control rats) throughout the test analyze (a) the time spent and the number of entrances were omitted from the analysis; there was no difference in the open or closed arm of the elevated plus maze; (b) betweentheproportionsofstressedandcontrolanimalsthat the number of shocks, the burying behavior latency, or did not receive the shock (𝑝 = 0.645, Fisher exact test). the cumulative burying behavior in the defensive burying Figure5 shows the number of shocks (a), burying behavior behavior test; (c) the immobility in the forced swimming latency (b), and cumulative burying behavior (c) of rats NeuroscienceJournal 5 2.0 2.0 e k a nt ke olution i 1.5 ater inta 1.5 ose s 1.0 ap w 1.0 ucr ve t e s 0.5 ati 0.5 ativ Rel Rel 0.0 0.0 Baseline 1 2 3 Baseline 1 2 3 Time (weeks) Time (weeks) Control Control Stressed Stressed (a) (b) 100 %) e ( c n e er 80 ef pr n o uti 60 ol e s os cr Su 40 Baseline 1 2 3 Time (weeks) Control Stressed (c) Figure2:CMSfailedtoinduceanhedoniaintheanimals.Thesucrosesolution(a)andtapwaterintake(b)aswellasthesucrosesolution preference(c)ofratexposedtoCMS(blackcircles)ormaintainedwithoutstress(whitecircles)didnotchangethroughouttheexperiment. Fluidintakeofsucrosesolutionandtapwaterwasexpressedasrelativetobasalconsumptioninordertoavoidbaselinedifferences.Dataare expressedasmean±SEM. previouslyexposed(𝑛 = 11,blackbars)ornonexposed(𝑛 = 4.Discussion 9,graybars)toCMSandevaluatedinthedefensiveburying behavior test. Data analysis did not indicate differences 4.1. CMS as a Paradigm of Depressive-Like State. CMS determined by stress in the shock number or the burying paradigm is a highly validated animal model that uses latency (𝑝 = 0.419 and 𝑝 = 0.818, resp.); in contrast, this stressors of moderate intensity (to give a realistic analogue analysis showed that the cumulative burying behavior was ofthestressorfoundineverydaylife)togenerateanhedonia higherinthestressedgroupcomparedtotheunstressedone inrodents[7,8];thisstateisindicatedbyareductionofthe (𝑝=0.035). intake of a sucrose solution, which is found to be palatable for most rats. The sensitivity of this parameter to detect anhedoniaincreaseswhentheanimalsaredeprivedoffood 3.5. Behavioral Despair in the FST. Figure6 illustrates the and water for several hours (14 to 20) before the sucrose immobilitybehaviorintheFSTofratspreviouslyexposedto solution intake test; this deprivation reduces the within- 4weeksofCMS(𝑛 = 13,blackbar)orthosewhowerekept group variability, resulting in a wider difference between unstressed(𝑛=13,graybar).Statisticalanalysisofthesedata stressed and control animals with respect to nondeprived indicatednosignificantdifferencesbetweenthecontroland subjects [16]. In our laboratory, the mild stressors used in stressedgroup(𝑝=0.971). the CMS schedule have been effective to induce anhedonia in male rats, showing age differences, insomuch that high 3.6.CorticosteroneSerumLevels. Corticosteroneserumlev- andsmallpercentagesofmiddle-agedandyoungratsexpress els were 242.21 ±45.56ng/mL (𝑛 = 6) for control rats and thiscoresignofdepression,respectively[13].Inthepresent 197.35 ± 17.14ng/mL (𝑛 = 7) for the animals exposed to study, we found that young-adult male rats did not reduce CMS;statisticalanalysisdidnotshowsignificantdifferences the sucrose consumption or sucrose preference when they (𝑝=0.349,𝑡-test). were exposed to the CMS paradigm during a period of 6 NeuroscienceJournal 110 stressschedules,developedanhedonia[7,29,30].However, therearealsodescriptionsthatsomeanimalsareresistantto 105 ∗∗∗ ∗∗∗ ∗∗∗ aparticularCMSschedule[31,32];althoughthisproportion %) isrelativelylower,itsuggeststhatintrastraindifferencesare ht ( important regarding stress resilience. According to these eig 100 ### authors, the difference in vulnerability to CMS could be y w ### determinedbyvariationsinfactorssuchashippocampalneu- d ∗ o rogenesisorcellularplasticitypathways[31,32].Alternatively, b 95 ### ve it could be argued that 3 weeks of CMS is not adequate elati or enough to induce anhedonia in the animals; however, a R 90 similarstressprotocolwasshowntobeeffectivetoinducethis stateinyoung-adultSpragueDawleyrats[33];furthermore, in our laboratory, this scheme of stressors has effectively 85 Baseline 1 2 3 inducedanhedoniainmiddle-aged[13,18]andyoung-adult Time (weeks) [17] Wistar male rats. Additionally, in our experience, the exposureofnonanhedonicyoung-adultmaleratstoalonger Control periodofCMS(7weeks)didnotchangethethresholdofrats Stressed toexhibitahedonicstate(unpublisheddata). Figure 3: The rats’ growing rate is reduced by CMS exposure. Incontrastwithresultsofanhedonia,herewefoundthat Compared to unstressed rats (white circles), the relative body the body weight gain of the rats was reduced by the CMS weights of rats exposed to CMS (black circles) are lower at the paradigm.Thesedataareinlinewithapreviousstudymade threeweeksofstressexposure.Valuesareexpressedaspercentageof on Sprague Dawley and Long Evans rats where CMS did baselinetoavoideffectsdeterminedbybaselinedifferences.Dataare expressedasmean±SEM.Tukeytest:∗𝑝 < 0.05and∗∗∗𝑝 < 0.001 notinduceanhedoniabutreducedrats’bodyweight[34].It versusbaseline;###𝑝<0.001versuscontrolgroup. has been proposed that body weight changes indicate male rats’ vulnerability to the stress since they reflect the overall impactofachronicstressfulsituation[35,36].Thissignmay beinterpretedasamanifestationofadepressive-likestatein threeweeks,whichagreeswithourpreviousstudy[13].The ratssincereductionofbodyweightisconsideredasymptom lack of difference in the anhedonic state between stressed of the human disorder, mainly in comorbidity with anxiety and nonstressed rats cannot be explained by differences disorders[5]. in conditions of housing or handling, since they were the Restriction of body weight in rats exposed to chronic same for both groups along the experiment (except for the stressors is a physiologic correlate evoked by changes in a stressors), including the periods of deprivation of food and central level. Stress response is mediated by a main secre- waterbeforethesucroseconsumptiontest. tagogue, the 41-aminoacid peptide corticotrophin releasing Inthesameline,presentresultscouldsuggestvariations factor (CRF), which is synthesized in neurons of hypotha- in the spectrum of physiological or behavioral responses lamic or extrahypothalamic nuclei (i.e., the paraventricular in rodents after application of chronic stressors. Besides nucleusandcentralamygdala,resp.).CFRinteractswithtwo anhedonia,CMSparadigmgeneratesotheroutcomesrelated main receptors (CRF1 and CRF2), which are differentially todepression(increasedimmobilityintheFST,potentiation expressedindistinctbrainstructuresandperipheraltissues of learned helplessness, decreased male sexual behavior, [37]. A huge number of studies demonstrate that central and decreased REM sleep latency) and other stress-related administration of CRF replicates signs of depression and disorders such as anxiety (for review, see [10]). In the same anxiety in rodents, such as reduced exploration, increased way,severalreportsindicatethatestablishmentofhighlevels despair, decreased appetite, disrupted sleep, and reduced of anhedonia by means of a CMS schedule could show gainofbodyweight[38–42].Furthermore,experimentswith variationsbyfactorsas(1)intrastrainvariation;(2)epigenetic drugsthatselectivelyantagonizetheCRF1receptorssuggest or genetic aspects; (3) the laboratory environment; and (4) that these play an important role in the initiation of events adverseorstimulatingearlylifeevents[28].Forthesimilitude that lead to reduction of body weight in rodents without with the multifactorial etiology of human depression, these stress exposure (i.e., poor food intake) [43] as well as in observationsfurthersupportthehypotheticalvalidityofthe downregulationofbodyweightofanimalsunderCMS[44]. CMSmodel. ResultsshownheresuggestthatthevulnerabilitytoCMSof someyoung-adult malesseems to be expressed by different 4.2.Depressive-LikeBehaviorsInducedbyCMS. Inthisstudy, signs associated with the depressive spectrum and is not we found that young-adult male rats did not reduce the limitedtoanhedonia. sucrose consumption or sucrose preference when exposed Given the contrasting results described above and con- to the CMS paradigm during a period of three weeks and sidering that CMS is able to generate behavioral despair couldsuggestthatyoungmaleratsareresilienttothiskindof in the FST (for review, see [10]), another animal model of stress.TheresilienceofyounganimalstotheCMSparadigm depression, we decided to evaluate whether this behavior foundinthecurrentstudycontrastswiththereportsshowing would better indicate the vulnerability of young rats to the that rats from the same age and strain, exposed to similar CMSparadigm.ResultsfromtheFSTindicatedthatchronic NeuroscienceJournal 7 500 12 ∗ 10 econds) 340000 of entries 68 me (s 200 mber 4 Ti u 100 N 2 0 0 Open arm Closed arm Open arm Closed arm Control Control Stressed Stressed (a) (b) Figure4:EffectofCMSonmalerat’sperformanceintheelevatedplusmaze.TheCMSparadigmdidnotaffectthetimespentintheopenor closedarms(a),butitincreasedthenumberofentriesintotheclosedarms(b),suggestingananxiogeniceffectofCMS.Dataareexpressed asmean±SEM.Student’s𝑡-test:∗𝑝<0.05versuscontrolgroup. 6 300 5 250 mber 4 onds) 200 ock nu 3 me (sec 150 Sh 2 Ti 100 1 50 0 0 Control Stressed Control Stressed (a) (b) 120 100 ∗ ds) 80 n o ec 60 e (s m Ti 40 20 0 Control Stressed (c) Figure5:EffectofCMSontheperformanceofmaleratsinthedefensiveburyingbehaviortest.CMSdidnotaffectthenumberofshocks(a) orburyingbehaviorlatency(b);however,itincreasedthecumulativeburyingbehavior(c),suggestingananxiogeniceffectofCMS.Dataare expressedasmean±SEM.Student’s𝑡-test:∗𝑝<0.05versuscontrolgroup. stress did not increase the behavioral despair, suggesting currentstudyisinagreementwithotherreportswhereCMS again that the young-male rats are resilient to CMS. This didnotincreasebehavioraldespairofyoung-adultratsinthe result contrasts with studies that describe an increase of FST [45–47]. A putative explanation for the inconsistency immobility behavior of young animals in the FST after of result among the effect of CMS on FST could be related previousexposuretoaCMSschedule[19,20];however,the to variations in the intensity, unpredictability, or duration 8 NeuroscienceJournal 60 test, these animals presented higher cumulative burying behaviorwithrespecttothenonstressedanimals;thisresult 50 indicates that CMS induces an increased aversion for the noxiousstimulusinthemalerats. n mi These data together indicate that CMS induced an 40 5unts/ aannxhieedtyo-nliiake(satsateinindiycoautendg-baydutlhteainrimsuaclsrothseatpdridefneroetndceev;elsoepe o 30 of c Figure2(c)).Theseresultsareinlinewithpreviousreports[11, er 12]whereCMSincreasedanxious-likebehaviorsinrodents mb 20 evaluated in several tests. The anxiogenic effect of CMS on u N young male rats indicates that they are not resilient to this 10 kindofstressor,aspreviouslyreported[13];instead,itappears that young animals’ vulnerability to CMS is not reflected as the core symptoms of depression. The anxiogenic-like 0 Control Stressed effects of CMS found in the current study contrast with the study of Kompagne et al. [19] who found ambiguous Figure6:EffectofCMSonthebehavioraldespairintheFST.There were no significant differences between the control and stressed effectsofthisparadigmonanxious-likebehaviorsinWistar group in the immobility behavior. Data are expressed as mean ± rats(CMSinducedsocialavoidanceandincreasedgrooming SEM. but acted as if it was anxiolytic in the elevated plus maze). Interestingly, in the study referred to, a clear effect of CMS ondepressive-likebehaviorswasobserved.Inline,Karsonet al.[49]foundthatWistaryoung-adultmaleratsexposedto of stressors used in the stress regime; also, the rat strain theCMSparadigmexhibiteddepressive-likebehaviorwhen used could be a factor of variability. In the current study, evaluatedinthesucrosepreferencetestandtheFST;however, immobilitylevelspresentedbythemaleratscouldcontribute theanimalsdidnotdevelopbehavioralalterationsrelatedto to the lack of effect of CMS on FST; as shown in Figure6, anxiety in the elevated plus maze test. Compared with the control male rats presented very high levels of immobility currentCMSschedule,Karson’sprotocolincludedperiodsof (∼50countsfromatotalof60);thischaracteristicimportantly restraint (4h), level shaking (10min), and nip tail (10min); reducesthedynamicrangetoobserveputativestress-related meanwhile,itdidnotincludestroboscopiclight,whitenoise, magnification of the depressive-like behavior (an increase ordeprivationoffoodorwater.Thus,allthesedatasuggest of immobility levels) in the animals; thus, it is difficult to thatCMSinduceseitheranxietyordepressive-likebehaviors detecttheimpactofCMSonbehavioraldespair.Thiseffect inyoungmalerats;thisdifferentialeffectcouldbedetermined could be related to the large period (around 2 months) of bytheintensityordurationofthestressorsemployedinthe animal isolation required by the CMS schedule, since this CMS schedule. This idea is supported by a clinical report condition alters the animals’ performance in the FST [48]; suggestingthatthenatureofthestressorisimportantinthe thishypothesisremainstobeproved. developmentofdepressionoranxiety[50]. Italsomightbethoughtthattheanimalsrequiredmore Itisknownthatthestressorandtheabilitytoeffectively than three weeks of CMS exposure to develop high levels deal with it contribute to the development of anxious and of behavioral despair in the FST; however, the exposure of depressivesymptoms[3,4].Thisrelationshipconducestothe 3 to 4 weeks of CMS has been shown to be enough to coexistenceofanxietyanddepressionsymptomatologyinthe effectivelyincreaseimmobilityintheFST[19,20].Thus,the same patients; this fact has led authors to propose a con- discrepancies in the expression of depressive-like behaviors tinuum model, from anxiety syndromes to mild, moderate, maybedeterminedbydifferencesinstressorsintensityand and severe depression. However, an alternative hypothesis animals’abilitytocopewiththem. that needs to be explored is whether the comorbidity of Since anxiety is another psychiatric disease where the anxiety and depression symptomatology is due to linked stressfuleventsplayanimportantetiologicrole,wedecidedto neurobiological abnormalities that are nevertheless distinct evaluatetheimpactofthiskindofstressonrodent’sanxious- intermsofwhethertheymediatetheanxietyorthedepressive likebehaviors. symptoms[51].Inthisrespect,thecurrentstudysuggeststhat CMS paradigm could be a useful tool to selectively study 4.3. Anxious-Like Behaviors Induced by CMS. In this study, neurobiologicalbasisofanxietyanddepression.Thus,itwill weevaluatedanxious-likebehaviorsusingtwotestssensitive be interesting to study different CMS schedules varied in to previous stress exposure: elevated plus maze [21] and duration,intensity,ornumberofstressorsperdayinorderto defensive burying behavior test [22]. In the elevated plus differentially generate anxious- or depressive-like behaviors mazetest,wefoundthatCMSonlyincreasedthenumberof inyoungmalerats. entries to the closed arms, suggesting that stressed animals have an increased preference for the closed spaces which 4.4.CorticosteroneSerumLevels. Inthecurrentstudy,wedid is considered an anxious-like behavior [23, 24]. In line, notfinddifferencesbetweencorticosteronelevelsofstressed althoughthestressedanimaldidnotavoidtheelectrode(no andcontrolrats,suggestinghabituationoftheanimalstothe differences in the shock number) in the defensive burying stressors employed in our CMS schedule. This result is in NeuroscienceJournal 9 agreementwithpreviousdatafoundinourlaboratorywhere tochronicstress[43].Interestingly,someoftheseeffectsare seven weeks of CMS induced a reduction of corticosterone independentofadrenals,sincecorticosteronelevelswerenot serumlevels(345.98±47.73ng/mL,unstressedgroup,versus directly corelated to increases or decreases in CRF [42, 43, 185.41 ± 56.85ng/mL, stressed group, 𝑛 = 8, 𝑝 < 0.01, 68].Accordingly,someauthorsproposethatthisdissociation unpublished data); however, the failure of CMS to increase isduetotheactionsofcorticosteroneondifferentneuronal corticosterone levels in young adults contrasts with reports populations: on PVN neurons for blunting the HPA axis indicating that chronic stress induces elevated levels of activity(viarestrictionofCRFproduction)andonneurons glucocorticoids[52,53].Thisdiscrepancymaybeexplained ofamygdalaforstimulatingCRFexpressionandinductionof byvarianceinintensityofstressorssinceinthosereportsthe anxiety-likebehaviors.Resultspresentedhereseemtobein stressorsweremoresevere(e.g.,restraint)thantheonesused linewiththesecondoption;however,furtherexperimentsare inthecurrentstudy;insupportofthisidea,oneoftherules neededtotestthishypothesis. ofhabituationthatappeartogovernadaptationtoachronic Present data together indicate that the relationship stressorstatesthat“theprogressivereductionoftheresponses between stress and affective disorders is not as simple as to a repeated stimulus is negatively related to its intensity” it appears; thus, to elucidate the impairments induced by [54]. chronicstressoncentralnervoussystemthatcouldgenerate Contrastingwiththecurrentresults,therearealsoseveral thesedisorders,anevaluationofthedifferentcomponentsof reportsinwhichyoung-adultratsexposed toseveralweeks HPAaxisandtheireffectonlimbicstructuresisneeded. ofCMSshowedelevatedcorticosteronelevelscomparedwith theunstressedanimals[34,55–57];however,itisimportant 5.Conclusions to highlight the notion that in these studies there was a significantproportionofanhedonicrats(∼70%accordingto The CMS schedule used in this study induced behavioral [58]),asindicatedbythereductioninsucroseintake,which alterationsthatsuggestanxietybutnotthecoresymptomof is different from the one found in the current study, where depression, anhedonia, in young-adult males. These results no animal developed anhedonia. Thus, such dissimilarities indicatethatnonanhedonicyoungmaleratsarenotresilient maybe,atleastinpart,explainedbydifferencesinbehavioral to this kind of stressor, but rather the alterations are not phenotypesinceinthecurrentstudytheanimalsexpressed expressedinthesphereofmotivationalbehaviors.Thisstudy anxious-likeinsteadofdepressive-likebehaviors. highlights the need to use several physiologic measures In the current study, the animals exposed to CMS (suchasbodyweight)andbehavioraltests,whenevaluating displayed an anxious-like phenotype which was not related animal’svulnerabilitytostress.Thisstudyalsosuggeststhat to hyperactivity of HPA axis; this result contradicts the CMScouldbeananimalmodeltostudytheneurobiologyof diathesis-stresshypothesisforanxietyanddepression;how- depressionandanxiety. ever, in some cases, especially after chronic stress, low corticosteronelevelsarethefeatureofHPAaxisdysfunction CompetingInterests [54,59]thatcouldberelatedtoaffectivedisorders,asinthe The authors declare that there are no competing interests caseofatypicaldepressioninhumans[59,60].Furthermore, regardingthepublicationofthispaper. thecorticosteroneprofilefoundinourstressedanimalsisin agreementwitharecentstudy[61]thatdescribesareduction incorticosteronelevelsofyoungratsafterchronicstress(21 Acknowledgments days of social isolation); despite this, the animals displayed anxious-anddepressive-likephenotypesintheelevatedplus TheauthorswishtothankbachelorBeatrizCruz-Lo´pezfor mazeandtheFST,respectively;thesebehavioralresultswere technicalassistanceinexperiments.Thisworkwassupported relatedtoalterationsinbrainplasticityinthestressedanimals by Instituto Nacional de Psiquiatr´ıa Ramo´n de la Fuente (asmeasuredbydeficiencyinsomecelladhesionmolecules), Mun˜iz(NC093370.2). which are characteristic of depressive/anxious states [62]. Thus, it appears that hyperactivity of HPA axis is not the References unique factor related to the etiology of affective disorders; it is becoming clear that hypoactivity of the HPA axis may [1] H.Seyle,“Stressandthegeneraladaptationsyndrome,”British resultfromchronicstressexposureandthatthiseffectmay MedicalJournal,vol.1,pp.1383–1392,1950. beimportantinstress-relateddisorders[63,64]. [2] H. Selye, “Forty years of stress research: principal remaining problemsandmisconceptions,”CanadianMedicalAssociation Anadditionalinterpretationfortheoccurrenceofbehav- Journal,vol.115,no.1,pp.53–56,1976. iors related to affective and/or anxiety disorders takes into [3] C. Heim and C. B. Nemeroff, “The impact of early adverse account the action of CRF on limbic brain structures, such experiencesonbrainsystemsinvolvedinthepathophysiology asamygdaloidnuclei.Thus,studiesshowthatadministration ofanxietyandaffectivedisorders,”BiologicalPsychiatry,vol.46, ofthispeptidedirectlyintobasolateralamygdala[39,65]or no.11,pp.1509–1522,1999. itsoverexpressionincentralamygdala[66]inducedanxious- [4] K.S.Kendler,L.M.Thornton,andC.O.Gardner,“Stressfullife likebehaviorsinrodents.Inturn,treatmentwithantagonists eventsandpreviousepisodesintheetiologyofmajordepression ofCRFreceptorsisabletoreduceanxiety-likebehaviorsand in women: an evaluation of the ‘kindling’ hypothesis,” The expressionofCRFmRNAinthecentralnucleusofamygdala American Journal of Psychiatry, vol. 157, no. 8, pp. 1243–1251, [67]andrestorethegainofbodymassinanimalssubmitted 2000. 10 NeuroscienceJournal [5] American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical EuropeanJournalofPharmacology,vol.463,no.1–3,pp.163–175, ManualforMentalDisorders,AmericanPsychiatryPress,Wash- 2003. ington,DC,USA,4thedition,2000. [22] S.F.DeBoerandJ.M.Koolhaas,“Defensiveburyinginrodents: [6] M.SarterandJ.P.Bruno,“Animalmodelsinbiologicalpsychi- ethology, neurobiology and psychopharmacology,” European atry,”inBiologicalPsychiatry,H.Dhaenen,J.A.denBoer,andP. JournalofPharmacology,vol.463,no.1–3,pp.145–161,2003. Willner,Eds.,pp.1–8,JohnWiley&Sons,Chichester,UK,2002. [23] S. Pellow, P. Chopin, S. E. File, and M. Briley, “Validation of [7] P.Willner,A.Towell,D.Sampson,S.Sophokleous,andR.Mus- open:closedarmentriesinanelevatedplus-mazeasameasure cat,“Reductionofsucrosepreferencebychronicunpredictable ofanxietyintherat,”JournalofNeuroscienceMethods,vol.14, mild stress, and its restoration by a tricyclic antidepressant,” no.3,pp.149–167,1985. Psychopharmacology,vol.93,no.3,pp.358–364,1987. [24] S.Hogg,“Areviewofthevalidityandvariabilityoftheelevated [8] P.Willner,“Validity,reliabilityandutilityofthechronicmild plus-mazeasananimalmodelofanxiety,”PharmacologyBio- stress model of depression: a 10-year review and evaluation,” chemistryandBehavior,vol.54,no.1,pp.21–30,1996. Psychopharmacology,vol.134,no.4,pp.319–329,1997. [25] J.P.J.PinelandD.Treit,“Buryingasadefensiveresponsein [9] P.S.D’Aquila,P.Brain,andP.Willner,“Effectsofchronicmild rats,”JournalofComparativeandPhysiologicalPsychology,vol. stressonperformanceinbehaviouraltestsrelevanttoanxiety 92,no.4,pp.708–712,1978. anddepression,”Physiology&Behavior,vol.56,no.5,pp.861– [26] M.J.Detke,J.Johnson,andI.Lucki,“Acuteandchronicantide- 867,1994. pressantdrugtreatmentintheratforcedswimmingtestmodel [10] P. Willner, “Chronic mild stress (CMS) revisited: consistency ofdepression,”ExperimentalandClinicalPsychopharmacology, andbehavioural-neurobiologicalconcordanceintheeffectsof vol.5,no.2,pp.107–112,1997. CMS,”Neuropsychobiology,vol.52,no.2,pp.90–110,2005. [27] A. Armario, A. Gavalda`, and J. Mart´ı, “Comparison of the behaviouralandendocrineresponsetoforcedswimmingstress [11] A.R.Salomons,T.Kortleve,N.R.Reinders,S.Kirchhoff,S.S. infiveinbredstrainsofrats,”Psychoneuroendocrinology,vol.20, Arndt,andF.Ohl,“Susceptibilityofapotentialanimalmodel no.8,pp.879–890,1995. for pathological anxiety to chronic mild stress,” Behavioural BrainResearch,vol.209,no.2,pp.241–248,2010. [28] W.Theilmann,A.Kleimann,M.Rheinetal.,“Behavioraldiffer- encesofmaleWistarratsfromdifferentvendorsinvulnerability [12] N. Boufleur, C. T. D. Antoniazzi, C. S. Pase et al., “Neonatal andresiliencetochronicmildstressarereflectedinepigenetic handling prevents anxiety-like symptoms in rats exposed to regulationandexpressionofp11,”BrainResearch,vol.1642,pp. chronic mild stress: behavioral and oxidative parameters,” 505–515,2016. Stress,vol.16,no.3,pp.321–330,2013. [29] M. N. Jayatissa, C. Bisgaard, A. Tingstro¨m, M. Papp, and O. [13] J.J.Herrera-Pe´rez,L.Mart´ınez-Mota,andA.Ferna´ndez-Guasti, Wiborg,“Hippocampalcytogenesiscorrelatestoescitalopram- “Agingincreasesthesusceptibilitytodevelopanhedoniainmale mediatedrecoveryinachronicmildstressratmodelofdepres- rats,” Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological sion,”Neuropsychopharmacology,vol.31,no.11,pp.2395–2404, Psychiatry,vol.32,no.8,pp.1798–1803,2008. 2006. [14] National Research Council, Committee for the Update of the [30] S.A.Montgomery,H.Loft,C.Sa´nchez,E.H.Reines,andM. GuidefortheCareandUseofLaboratoryAnimals:Guideforthe Papp,“Escitalopram(S-enantiomerofcitalopram):clinicaleffi- CareandUseofLaboratoryAnimals,NationalAcademiesPress, cacyandonsetofactionpredictedfromaratmodel,”Pharma- Washington,DC,USA,8thedition,2011. cologyandToxicology,vol.88,no.5,pp.282–286,2001. [15] DiarioOficialdelaFederacio´n,NormaOficialMexicana-NOM- [31] A. Bergstro¨m, M. N. Jayatissa, T. Thykjær, and O. Wiborg, 062-ZOO-1999. Especificaciones Te´cnicas para la Produccio´n, “Molecularpathwaysassociatedwithstressresilienceanddrug CuidadoyUsodelosAnimalesdeLaboratorio,2001. resistanceinthechronicmildstressratmodelofdepression—a [16] R.MuscatandP.Willner,“Suppressionofsucrosedrinkingby geneexpressionstudy,”JournalofMolecularNeuroscience,vol. chronicmildunpredictablestress:amethodologicalanalysis,” 33,no.2,pp.201–215,2007. NeuroscienceandBiobehavioralReviews,vol.16,no.4,pp.507– [32] C. F. Bisgaard, M. N. Jayatissa, J. J. Enghild, C. Sanche´z, R. 517,1992. Artemychyn, and O. Wiborg, “Proteomicinvestigation of the [17] J.J.Herrera-Pe´rez,L.Mart´ınez-Mota,andA.Ferna´ndez-Guasti, ventralrathippocampuslinksDRP-2toescitalopramtreatment “Agingimpairstheantidepressant-likeresponsetocitalopram resistance and SNAP to stress resilience in the chronic mild inmalerats,”EuropeanJournalofPharmacology,vol.633,no. stressmodelofdepression,”JournalofMolecularNeuroscience, 1–3,pp.39–43,2010. vol.32,no.2,pp.132–144,2007. [18] J. J. Herrera-Pe´rez, L. Mart´ınez-Mota, R. Chavira, and A. [33] A.J.Grippo,T.G.Beltz,andA.K.Johnson,“Behavioraland Ferna´ndez-Guasti, “Testosterone prevents but not reverses cardiovascular changes in the chronic mild stress model of anhedoniainmiddle-agedmalesandlacksaneffectonstress depression,”PhysiologyandBehavior,vol.78,no.4-5,pp.703– vulnerabilityinyoungadults,”HormonesandBehavior,vol.61, 710,2003. no.4,pp.623–630,2012. [34] A.T.M.Konkle,S.L.Baker,A.C.Kentner,L.S.-M.Barbagallo, [19] H.Kompagne,G.Ba´rdos,G.Sze´na´si,I.Gacsa´lyi,L.G.Ha´rsing, Z.Merali,andC.Bielajew,“Evaluationoftheeffectsofchronic andG.Le´vay,“Chronicmildstressgeneratescleardepressive mildstressorsonhedonicandphysiologicalresponses:sexand but ambiguous anxiety-like behaviour in rats,” Behavioural straincompared,”BrainResearch,vol.992,no.2,pp.227–238, BrainResearch,vol.193,no.2,pp.311–314,2008. 2003. [20] C.Bielajew,A.T.M.Konkle,A.C.Kentneretal.,“Strainand [35] A. Armario, “The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis: what genderspecificeffectsintheforcedswimtest:effectsofprevious canittellusaboutstressors?”CNS&NeurologicalDisorders— stressexposure,”Stress,vol.6,no.4,pp.269–280,2003. DrugTargets,vol.5,no.5,pp.485–501,2006. [21] S.M.KorteandS.F.DeBoer,“Arobustanimalmodelofstate [36] A.Armario,J.Marti,andM.Gil,“Theserumglucoseresponse anxiety:fear-potentiatedbehaviourintheelevatedplus-maze,” to acute stress is sensitive to the intensity of the stressor and

Description:
weight gain; and (3) increased the rat's entries into closed arms of the plus maze and the cumulative burying behavior. These data makes it a useful tool to study the neurobiology of depression. [8]. In agreement with “Aging increases the susceptibility to develop anhedonia in male rats,” Pro
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.