ebook img

Yearbook of Morphology 1992 PDF

295 Pages·8.008 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Yearbook of Morphology 1992

YEARBOOK OF MORPHOLOGY 1992 Yearbook of Morphology Editors: Geert Booij Jaap van Marle Consulting Editors: Stephen Anderson (Baltimore) Mark Aronoff (Stony Brook, N.Y.) Laurie Bauer (Wellington) Rudie Botha (Stellenbosch) Joan Bybee (Albuquerque, New Mexico) Wolfgang Dressler (Wien) Jack Hoeksema (Groningen) Rochelle Lieber (Durham, N.H.) Peter Matthews (Cambridge, U.K.) Franz Rainer (Salzburg) Thomas Roeper (Amherst, MA) Sergio Scalise (Bologna) Henk Schultink (Utrecht) Jindrich Toman (Ann Arbor, MI) Wolfgang Wurzel (Berlin) Editorial address: Editors, Yearhook of Morphology Vakgroep Taalkunde, Vrije Universiteit De Boelelaan 1 lOS 1081 HV Amsterdam, The Netherlands YEARBOOK OF MORPHOLOGY 1992 Edited by GEERT BOOIJ General Linguistics, Free University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands ] AAP VAN MARLE P.l. Meertens Institute of the Royal Netherlands Academy ofA rts and Sciences ... " SPRINGER-SCIENCE+BUSINESS MEDIA, B.V. ISSN 0922-3495 ISBN 978-90-481-4197-5 ISBN 978-94-017-3710-4 (eBook) DOI 10.1007/978-94-017-3710-4 Printed on acid-free paper All Rights Reserved © 1993 by Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht Originally published by Kluwer Academic Publishers in 1993 Softcover reprint of the hardcover I st edition 1993 No part of the material protected by this copyright notice may be reproduced or utilized in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording or by any information storage and retrieval system, without written permission from the copyright owner. Table of contents HARALD CLAHSEN and MONIKA ROTHWEILER / Inflectional rules in children's grammars: evidence from German participles 1 AIDAN DOYLE / Suffixal Rivalry: a case study in Irish nominalisa- tions 35 AD NEE LEMAN and JOLEEN SCHIPPER / Verbal prefixation in Dutch: thematic evidence for conversion 57 JOEL A. NEVIS and BRIAN D. JOSEPH / Wackernagel affixes: evidence from Balto-Slavic 93 FRANZ RAINER / Head-operations in Spanish morphology 113 GREGORY T. STUMP / Position classes and morphological theory 129 Discussion HARALD BAA YEN / On frequency, transparency and productivity 181 Review articles ANDREW CARSTAIRS-McCARTHY / Morphology without word internal constituents: a review of Stephen R. Anderson's A- Morphous Morphology 209 RICHARD SPROAT / Morphological non-separation revisited: a review of R. Lieber's Deconstructing Morphology 235 Book reviews HARALD BAA YEN / Hartmut Gunther (ed.), Experimentelle Studien zur deutschen Flexionsmorphologie 259 LAURIE BAUER / Thomas Becker, Analogie und morphologische Theorie 264 v VI Table of contents JOACHIM MUG DAN / John T. Jensen, Morphology: Word Structure in Generative Grammar 268 ANDREW SPENCER / Mark Aronoff (ed.), Morphology Now 282 Book notices LAURIE BAUER / Elizabeth Closs Traugott and Bernd Heine (cds.), Approaches to Grammaticalization 285 GEERT BOO!] / J. G. H. Combrink, Afrikaanse moifologie 286 FRANZ RAINER / Maria G. Lo Duca, Creativita e regole. Studio sllll'acquisizione della moifologia derivativa dell'italiano 287 GEERT BOO!] / D. Corbin (ed.), La formation des mots; structures et intelJm~tations 290 PUBLICATIONS RECEIVED 290 NOTES TO CONTRIBUTORS 293 Inflectional rules in children's grammars: evidence from German participles* HARALD CLAHSEN AND MONIKA ROTHWEILER 1. INTRODUCTION In recent studies on the acquisition of inflectional morphology in English much debate has centred around the question whether children's overregu larization errors, such as goed instead of went result from a morphological rule or from a connectionist network such as the one suggested by Rumelhart and McClelland (1986). Pinker and Prince (1992) and Marcus et al. (1992) have argued that the -ed affix has the status of a symbolic rule in the children's grammars which is qualitatively distinct from an associative memory for irregulars. However, researchers such as Marchman and Bates (1991) and MacWhinney and Leinbach (1992) have disputed the existence of such symbolic rules and suggested that overregularizations are effects of associa tive networks: In English, the -ed affix has much higher type frequencies than the irregular past tense forms, and is therefore preferred in children's inflec tional errors. The system of German past participle inflection provides an interesting test case to this controversy, because in contrast to past tense marking in English, the default participle affix (= -t) does not vastly outnumber the irregular one (= -n). Thus, study of German participle affixes allows us to tease apart the role of (type) frequency in the acquisition of inflectional systems. In this way, the present paper sheds light on an important aspect of the general debate between symbolic and connectionist approaches to language. We will present acquisition data from two sources demonstrating that German-speaking children develop a regular default participle affix which is qualitatively different from the irregular affix. The first source are spon taneous speech samples from 3 language-unimpaired children studied longi tudinally over more than two years. The second source are spontaneous speech samples from 19 language-impaired German-speaking children studied longitudinally over a period of one year. Our most important finding is that only the participle affix -t is overregularized by the children, whereas the -n affix is not overextended. A parallel (asymmetrical) distribution can be observed in the stem errors. In all the data, there are no participles in which an irregular stem pattern has been extended to a weak verb. Rather, the only kind of stem error we found are regular stems replacing irregular ones, e.g. *gebind instead of gebunden 'bounded', *gewinn instead of gewonnen 'won'. These two findings hold for the language-impaired children as well as for the normal controls. We will conclude that children's grammars involve a qualita- Geert Rooij and Jaap vall Marie (eds.), Yearbook of Morphology 1')92,1-34. © 1993 Kluwer Academic Publishers. 2 Harald Clahsen and Monika Rothweiler tive distinction between regular and irregular inflection which is not reduc ible to frequency differences. 2. PREVIOUS ACQUISITION STUDIES The acqUlsltlon of participles has not been systematically investigated in previous studies on the acquisition of German. There are only a few observa tions made by Mills (1985: 168f.) based on data from the early diary studies on German. Mills observed that the prefix ge- is 'often' left out before the age of 3;0, and she quotes some examples of children's participles indicating that around age 3;0 there is 'frequent' overgeneralization of the weak form of participles, i.e. present tense stems and the suffix -to However, Mills does not mention any figures as to how frequently such errors occur and how past participles develop in German child language. Most of the empirical evidence and theoretical approaches on the acquisi tion of inflection come from the study of English. In the following, we will therefore briefly summarize some findings from these studies. However, we cannot directly compare the acquisition of past participles in English and German, because in English the past participle is used much less frequently than it is in German. This has to do with a difference in tense marking between the two languages: in German, events in the past are expressed through the so-called present perfect (consisting of an auxiliary plus past participle), whereas English uses simple past tense in such cases. The closest equivalent to German past participle development is a comparison with the acquisition of English past tense formation. Traditional models of the acquisition of English past tense formation posit that the child creates an inflectional rule, add -ed, to generate regular past tense forms (e.g. walk - walked), while irregular past tense forms are stored in a list (d. Menyuk 1963, Ervin & Miller 1963, Cazden 1968). For example, Berko (1958) showed that five- and six-year-old children con sistently added the appropriate regular past tense affix -ed to new words they had never heard before. This ability was taken to indicate that children construct inflectional rules. As opposed to the traditional model, Rumelhart and McClelland (1986) conducted a connectionist simulation of the acquisition of the English past tense system. In their computer model, Rumelhart and McClelland tried to mimic aspects of the child's acquisition of the English past tense system. After brief training of 10 verbs and their past tense forms, their model was able to overgeneralize, i.e. to create appropriate past tense forms for words that were not part of the training set. Rumelhart and McClelland's model contains a pattern associator that directly takes a simplified quasi-phono logical representation (= 'Wickelfeatures') of the stem as input and computes a corresponding phonological representation of the past tense form as output. Correlations among pairs of features in the stem and the past are Participles in German Child Language 3 stored, and based on the strength of the association between the stem features and various output features in the training set, the model generalizes to new verbs. The challenge of this model is that the entire past tense system including its acquisition is represented in a unitary associative architecture without any recourse to morphological rules of inflection. In empirical studies, it has been observed, for example, that the child's earliest past tense forms are mostly correct and that overgeneralizations (go ed, etc.) occur later in development, before the correct adult system is acquired; this is referred to as the U-shaped curve of the development of inflection (d. e.g. Slobin 1973). In order to explain the U-shaped development of inflection and the occurrence of overgeneralizations, Rumelhart and McClelland assumed that before the first overgeneralizations are used, the child shows 'explosive' vocabulary growth, which would result in a sudden acquisition of a large number of new regular verbs. In their computer model, this has the desired results, because as a consequence of the vocabulary growth, the features defining the -ed ending are strengthened by the pattern associator, and in this way overgener alizations are created. Later, when the discrepancies between the correct irregulars that continue to appear in the input and the overgeneralized forms will be registered, the crucial irregular features will be strengthened and the overgeneralizations will vanish over time. In this way, Rumelhart and McClelland successfully duplicated the U-curve of the development of English past tense inflection. Strikingly, this was possible within a model that had no explicit representation of words, rules, and no distinction between regular and irregular inflection. However, Pinker and Prince (1988) and Marcus et al. (1992) pointed out several deficiencies of Rumelhart and McClelland's model. For example, the pattern associator can learn arbitrary input/output mappings, even those that are linguistically impossible ones, e.g. string reversals. Since there is no morphological representation separate from the phonological content, the overall similarity between stems and past tense forms appears to be acci dental. Marcus et al. (1992) examined Rumelhart and McClelland's assump tions about vocabulary development in children. They found no support for the hypothesis that overgeneralization errors are triggered by a sudden increase in the number of regular verbs: regular verb tokens remain roughly constant over time, and the proportion of regular verb types increases, but does not correlate positively with the children's tendency to overregularize. Marcus et al. conclude that something endogenous must cause the child to overregularize, and not a change in their environments or their vocabulary. Pinker and Prince (1992) and Marcus et al. (1992) developed an alterna tive model of past tense acquisition in English in which they suggested that children (like adults) have two qualitatively different psychological mecha nisms for inflection: regular inflection which is based on symbolic rules, and irregular inflection which is based on associative processes of stored items. Support for this distinction comes from studies on the acquisition and 4 Harald Clahsen and Monika Rothweiler processing of the English past tense system. First, Marcus et al. (1992) found that of the various types of potential errors, only one type is productive in children's past tense forms: over-applications of the regular past tense affix -ed to irregular stems (*go-ed, etc.). In contrast to that, over-applications of irregular patterns are extremely rare (Xu and Pinker 1992). Second, Pinker and his colleagues found frequency effects for irregulars, but not for regulars. For example, Marcus et al. observed that children overregularize more often for verbs which are rare in the input. Third, Marcus et al. observed effects of similarity in the distribution of overregularizations: children make fewer over regularization errors for verbs that fall into families with more numerous and higher-frequency members (see also Bybee & Siobin 1982). These results were taken to indicate that irregular inflection is based on memory-based retrieval processes and that regular inflection is based on symbolic rules. The major controversy between these approaches is whether the regular/ irregular distinction is considered to be real and fundamental in children's grammars or whether it is treated as a secondary effect of peripheral factors, e.g. frequency differences in the input. Cross-linguistic comparisons may contribute to decide this issue. Consider, for example, that in past tense errors, English-speaking children preferably overregularize with the -ed suffix. In English the default status of the -ed suffix is confounded with its high frequency: regular forms clearly outnumber irregular ones.' In fact, Bybee (1991: 86f.) recently argued that in the acquisition of inflection in general, there is no cutoff point between regular and irregular inflection, and that the observed differences are due to type frequency, i.e. the number of distinct lexical items involved. Thus, according to Bybee, English-speaking children overregularize with the -ed suffix, because they have heard it used with so many different English verbs, and they rarely over-apply irregular patterns, simply because irregular verbs are rare in the input. This view is opposed to Marcus et al.'s model of the acquisition of inflection. Evidence from languages with different vocabulary statistics than English is necessary to tease apart the qualitative and quantitative differences between regular and irregular systems. The acquisition data on German provide such evidence. 3. PARTICIPLES IN GERMAN In traditional grammars three patterns of participle inflection are distin guished: weak inflection, strong inflection and mixed inflection. In (1), rhyme-scheme like abbreviations are used to illustrate the various root/stem preterite-participle patterns. Weak inflection involves -t affixation without stem/root changes (la); strong inflection involves -n affixation plus root! stem changes (1 b, I d, 1e ) and mixed inflection -t affixation plus stem/root changes (IcV Similar to English, ablaut and other changes of the verbal root/stem occur in both simple past tense forms and in past participles. Only weak verbs never involve root/stem changes, cf. (la). There are about 160

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.