CognitiveDevelopment27 (2012) 227–239 ContentslistsavailableatSciVerseScienceDirect Cognitive Development Who knows what’s good to eat? Infants fail to match the food preferences of antisocial others J.KileyHamlina,∗,KarenWynnb aDepartmentofPsychology,TheUniversityofBritishColumbia,2136WestMall,Vancouver,BCV6T1Z4,Canada bDepartment of Psychology, Yale University ,2 Hillho useAve,Ne wHa ven,C T065 11,UnitedS tat es a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Keywords: Humansgathermostoftheirknowledgeabouttheworld,includ- Socialevaluation ingobjec tivelyt ruefa cts and specificcult uralno rm s,byob serving Social learning/imitation and beingtaug htb yoth ers. Somein dividual sarew ort hyteach- Negat ivity bias ersa ndobj ectsof imi tation,h aving knowledgeo fcu lturalpr actices andpositiveintentionstoinform.Othersarebetterignoredbecause theyareignorant,becausetheymeanusharm,orsimplybecause wedonotwishtobe“likethem.”Thisstudyexamineswhether 16-month-oldsaresensitivetothepro-orantisocialbehaviorofa sourcethatdemonstratespreferencefortwonovelfoods.Infants took the emotional reactions displayed by novel and previously prosocial sources, but not antisocial sources, into account when deciding what to eat. These results suggest that others’ social behaviorinfluencesinfants’likelihoodtomatchtheirpreferences, illustratingtheinfluenceofsocialevaluationonsociallearning. © 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Most of what we know comes from others. By observing and interacting with others socially, wegainawealthofinformationrequiredforsuccessfulexistence,fromthelanguageandcultural practicesofoursocialgrouptotheworkingsofaparticularpieceofcomplexmachinery.Humans, even in infancy, appear uniquely adapted to using others as sources of information, through such mechanismsasthereproductionofnovelbehaviors(Bandura,1977;Barr,Dowden,&Hayne,1996; Meltzoff,2007;Tomasello,1999;Tomasello,Kruger,&Ratner,1993),theuseofemotionalreactionsto appraiseunfamiliarobjectsandevents(Campos&Stenberg,1981;Feinman,1982;Klinnert,Campos, Sorce,Emde,&Svejda,1983),andsensitivitytodirectedpedagogicalinteractions(Csibra&Gergely, 2006;Gergely&Csibra,2005,2006).Usingtheseandothersociallearningmechanisms,infantsrapidly ∗ Correspondingauthor.Tel.:+16048222297;fax:+16048226923. E-mailaddress:[email protected](J.K.Hamlin). 0885-2014/$–seefrontmatter© 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.05.005 228 J.K.Hamlin,K.Wynn/CognitiveDevelopment27 (2012) 227–239 acquireinformationandskillsthatwouldbedifficultorimpossibletolearnviafirstpersontrial-and- error. Despitethepowerfulinfluenceofsocialsourcesinhumandevelopment,researchoverthepast twodecadessuggeststhatchildrendonottakeinalltheinformationasourceprovides.Instead,they selectivelyincorporatesomepiecesofinformationandignoreothers.Inimitativeinteractionsearlyin thesecondyearoflife,infantsdonotsimplyreproduceeveryactionamodelperformsbutseemingly incorporatetheirownanalysisoftheintentionalandcausalstructureofactionintotheirimitative acts.Forexample,youngtoddlersfailtoimitatebehaviorsmarkedasaccidental(Carpenter,Akhtar, &Tomasello,1998)andreproduceonlythosepiecesofactionsthatarecausallyrelatedtoamodel’s overarchinggoal,leavingoutunnecessaryorsubordinatecomponents(Brugger,Lariviere,Mumme, & Bushnell, 2007; Carpenter, Call, & Tomasello, 2005; Gergely, Bekkering, & Király, 2002; Nielsen, 2006;butseeHorner&Whiten,2005;Lyons,Young,&Keil,2007,forevidenceoffaithfulimitation inchildren).Additionally,infantssometimesgo“belowthesurface”ofobservedbehaviors,effecting end-statesofgoal-directedactionsthatwereneveractuallycompletedandthereforemusthavebeen inferred(Bellagamba&Tomasello,1999;Johnson,Booth,&O’Hearn,2001;Legerstee&Markova,2008; Meltzoff,1995).Theseresultssuggestprivilegedrolesforthementalandcausalstructuresdrivinga source’sbehavior,overthephysicalnatureoftheactionsthemselves,asinfantsgatherinformation fromsocialothers. Inadditiontoselectingwhattolearnfromothers,itisalsoimportanttoselectfromwhomtolearn. Someindividualshaveinformationusefultoinfants,whereasothersmaynot(Feinman,1982).There areavarietyofnon-mutually-exclusivedimensionsonwhichonemightdiscriminatethevalueof informationfrompotentialsources.Perhapsthemostbasicisaccuracy:whetherasource’sinforma- tionisobjectivelytrueorfalse.Infantsinthesecondyeararesensitivetoothers’knowledgestates (Liszkowski,Carpenter,&Tomasello,2008;Onishi&Baillargeon,2005;Tomasello&Haberl,2003), and can identify (and sometimes attempt to correct) false statements (e.g., when a speaker labels acupa“dog;”Koenig&Echols,2003;Pea,1982).Agrowingbodyofresearchshowingimpressive selectivityinpreschoolchildren(Birch&Bloom,2002;Birch,Vauthier,&Bloom,2008;Corriveau& Harris,2009a;Jaswal&Neely,2007;Koenig&Harris,2005;Rakoczy,Warneken,&Tomasello,2009; Sabbagh&Baldwin,2001)suggeststhatsensitivitytoepistemicstatesplaysaroleinhowinfants useothersassourcesofnewinformation.Forexample,16-month-oldsfailtolearnnewwordsfrom previouslyinaccuratelabelers(Koenig&Woodward,2010),14-month-oldsarelesslikelytofollow thegazeofsomeonewhowaspreviouslyunreliable(e.g.,whoexpressedexcitementwhilelooking intoanemptycontainer;Chow,Poulin-Dubois,&Lewis,2008),and14-month-oldstendnottoimitate thenovelactionsofthosewhoincompetentlyperformedfamiliaracts(e.g.,putshoesontheirhands; Zmyj,Buttelmann,Carpenter,&Daum,2010).Theseresultssuggestthatevenininfancy,childrenare sensitivetopastaccuracyanddonotlearnfrominaccuratesources. However,pastaccuracyisnotalwaysdirectlyavailableinalearningsituation.Therefore,itwould be beneficial to be sensitive to a source’s potential for accuracy, or, in other words, that source’s general expertise (Henrich & Gil-White, 2001; Lutz & Keil, 2002). Expertise may be evident from one’sconfidenceand/orfamiliaritywithasituation.Themoreconfidenceorfamiliarityanindividual displays,themorelikelyalearnershouldbetoselectthatindividualtolearnfrom.Inanoveltoy situation,infantsasyoungas12monthsselectivelyreferenceanexperimenterknowntobefamiliar withalaboratoryratherthantheirmother(unfamiliarwiththelab),presumablyduetoanassumption thattheexperimenter’sfamiliaritymakeshermoreknowledgeable(Stenberg,2003,2009;Walden& Kim,2005). Further,somesourcesshouldbeselectedoverothersduenottotheirobjectiveaccuracyorexper- tise,butbecauseofthepotentialrelevanceoftheirinformationtothelearner.Thingslikelinguistic forms,ritualbehaviors,andfoodandobjectusemaydifferwidelyacrossculturesandgroupsand thereforeshouldonlybeacquiredfromsourceswhoknowtheformsrelevanttoaparticularlearner. Determiningwhohasculturallyrelevantinformationisnotnecessarilyeasy,butcouldbefacilitated byselectivelyattendingtosourceswhoarefamiliar,similartothelearner,orknowntobewithinthe learner’ssocialgroup.Infants’tendencytosociallyreferencehighlyfamiliarindividuals(likemothers) versusunfamiliarindividuals(likeresearchers)isambiguous.Somestudieshavefoundthatinfants shownopreferenceforcaregiversoverexperimenters(Devouche,2004;Klinnert,Emde,Butterfield, J.K.Hamlin,K.Wynn/CognitiveDevelopment27 (2012) 227–239 229 & Campos, 1986; McCabe & Uzgiris, 1983; Meltzoff & Moore, 1992), others show a preference for experimenters(Stenberg,2003,2009;Walden&Kim,2005),andstillothershaveshownacaregiver advantage(Zarbatany&Lamb,1985).Despitethisambiguity,preschoolersdoselectivelylearnfrom familiaroverunfamiliarteachers(Corriveau&Harris,2009b),infantsselectivelychooseobjectsand foodsprovidedbyindividualswhospeaktheirnativelanguage(Kinzler,Corriveau,&Harris,2011; Kinzler, Dupoux, & Spelke, 2007; Shutts, Kinzler, McKee, & Spelke, 2009), and 14–18-month-olds morecloselyimitatetheactionsofmodelsthatarenearertotheminage,evenaweekaftertheinitial modeling(Ryalls,Gul,&Ryalls,2000).Together,theseresultssuggestthatfamiliarity,similarity,and groupme mbershi ppla y arolei ninfan ts’source select ionand informa tion retention.1 Alsopotentiallyrelevantiswhetherasourceisliked.Likingisoftenrelatedtofamiliarityandsim- ilaritybutcanalsoexistindependently.Selectivityforpositivelyand/ornegativelyevaluatedsources mightserveavarietyofinter-relatedfunctions.First,ifadislikedsourceinturndislikesthelearner, orisanantisocialindividualingeneral,heorshemaymeanthelearnerharm.Ifso,thesource’sinfor- mationmaybeintentionallymisleadingandshouldbeignored(Mascaro&Sperber,2009;Vanderbilt, Liu,&Heyman,2011).Second,evenifadislikedsource’sinformationisobjectivelytrue,alearnermay stillchoosetoignoreittoavoid“beinglike”or“affirmingasharedstatewith”thesource.Achieving asenseofsharedlikenesshasbeentheorizedtobeasocialfunctionofsociallearningmechanisms, inparticularimitation(Hobson&Lee,1999;Lakin&Chartrand,2003;Meltzoff&Moore,1995;Nadel, Guérini,Pezé,&Rivet,1999;Over&Carpenter,2009;Over&Carpenter,2012;Uzgiris,1981).Ifso, individuals should avoid accepting information from those they have negatively evaluated. In one seriesofstudies(Nielsen,2006;Nielsen,Simcock,&Jenkins,2008),infantsinthesecondyearoflife morecloselyimitatetheactionsofavailable,“sociallyengaged”individualsthansocially-unavailable, “aloof” individuals, potentially indicating a role for social evaluation in imitative learning. Shutts, Kinzler,etal.(2009)foundthatinfants’tendencytomatchthefoodpreferencesofnativeoverforeign languagespeakerswasenhancedwhenthenativelanguagespeakerdisplayedpositiveaffect(pre- sumablylikedbytheinfants)andtheforeignlanguagespeakerdisplayednegativeaffect(presumably disliked).Theseresultssuggestaroleforliking/dislikinginsourceselectivityininfancy;however,they donotprovideadirecttestofthishypothesis. Thepresentstudydirectlyexaminesthelinkbetweensocialevaluationandthetendencytogain anduseinformationfromsocialsourcesininfancy.LikeShutts,Kinzler,etal.(2009),wedidthisin thedomainoffood,askingwhetherinfants’tendencytoeatthefoodsasourcelikesoverthefoodsthe samesourcedislikesisinfluencedbythesource’spreviousthird-partyprosocialorantisocialbehavior. Sixteen-month-oldinfantsobservedasinglepuppetsourcetastetwosimilar,novelfoods;thesource expressedlikingforonefoodanddislikingfortheother.Infantswerethengiventheopportunitytoeat thefoodsthemselves.Weobservedtheextenttowhichinfantsmatchedthesource’spreference.Prior tothefooddemonstrationphase,allinfantswatchedthesamepuppetshow,inwhichapuppettried butfailedtoopenaboxcontainingatoy.Theunsuccessfulpuppetwasthenhelpedinhisgoaltoopen theboxbyasecond“prosocial”characterandhinderedinhisgoalbyathird“antisocial”character. Thus,intheProsocialSourcecondition,thesourceoffoodliking/dislikinginformationhadpreviously helped a third-party to obtain a desired object; in the Antisocial Source condition, the source had hinderedthethird-party.Finally,inathird,NovelSourcecondition,althoughinfantsbeganbywatching aprosocial/antisocialpuppetshow,thesourcewhosubsequentlyexpresseditsfoodpreferencewas notapartoftheshowandwasthereforeunfamiliartotheinfants. Throughoutthefirsttwoyearsoflife,infantspositivelyevaluatetheprosocialandnegativelyeval- uatetheantisocialcharactersinthisexactscenario.Theseevaluationsareevidentininfants’affiliative behavior(e.g.,reaching;Hamlin&Wynn,2011),intheirsocialresponses(e.g.,whotheygivetreatsto andtaketreatsawayfrom;Hamlin,Wynn,Bloom,&Mahajan,2011),andintheirjudgmentsofothers whodirectpositiveandnegativebehaviorstotheprosocialandantisocialcharacters(Hamlinetal., 1 Whiletoourknowledgetherearenoadditionalstudiesreportingselectivelearningfromsimilarindividualsininfancy, alargebodyofresearchsuggeststhatpreschoolchildrengaininformationfromsimilaroverdissimilarothersalongmultiple dimensions,preferringthesameobjects,foods,andactivitiesasthosewhoaresimilartotheminage(ShuttsBanaji,&Spelke, 2010;c.f.VanderBorght&Jaswal,2009),andwhoaretheirsamegender(Bradbard&Endsley,1983;Bradbard,Martin,Endsley, &Halverson,1986;Martin,Eisenbud,&Rose,1995;Martin&Little,1990;Shuttsetal.,2010). 230 J.K.Hamlin,K.Wynn/CognitiveDevelopment27 (2012) 227–239 2011).Thisbreadthofsocialresponses,aswellastheuseofpuppetsinmanyotherstudiesofyoung children’ssocialjudgmentandinteraction(Olson&Spelke,2008;Rakoczyetal.,2009;Vaish,Missana, &Tomasello,2011),suggeststhatinfantstreatthesepuppetsassocialotherswhoareappropriate tointeractwith,toevaluate,and(potentially)touseasasourceofinformation.Hereweexamine thisfinalsuggestion,byaskingwhetherasource’spreviousprosocialorantisocialbehaviorinfluences infants’tendencytomatchthatsource’sfoodpreference. 1. Method 1.1. Participants Previousstudiesonselectivelearningininfancyhaveinvolvedinfantsinthefirsthalfofthesecond yearoflife(Chowetal.,2008;Koenig&Woodward,2010;Shutts,Condry,etal.,2009;Shutts,Kinzler, etal.,2009;Zmyjetal.,2010).Asourprocedurerequiredgraspingsmallitemsoutofsmallbowlsand eatingfingerfoods,wechosetostudy16-month-olds.Forty-eightinfantswererandomlyassignedto threeconditions:ProsocialSource(sevenboys;meanage16months,3days,range15months,15days to16months,25days),NovelSource(eightboys;meanage16months,0days,range15months,14 daysto16months,16days),andAntisocialSource(sevenboys;meanage15months,26days,range15 months,12daysto16months,17days).Nineadditionalinfantswereexcludedduetoparentalinter- ference(3),proceduralerror(2),orrefusaltoparticipate(4).Demographicinformationwascollected fromonethirdofparticipants;infantswere71%Caucasian/Non-Hispanic,11%Caucasian/Hispanic,6% Black/Non-Hispanic,6%NativeAmerican/Hispanic,and6%NativeAmerican/Non-Hispanic. 1.2. Stimuliandprocedure 1.2.1. Eatingwarm-up Infantssatonaparent’slapintheofficewaitingroom.Anexperimenterholdingawhitebowl containingyellowFruityCheerioskneeledinfrontoftheinfant,said“Look!”andateaCheerio,saying “Mmm!”Shethenofferedthebowltotheinfant,encouragingthechildtoeatuntiltheinfantwillingly tastedCheeriostwice. 1.2.2. Prosocial/antisocialpuppetshows(viewableonlineat www.yale.edu/infantlab/socialevaluationpreferencematching) Regardlessofeventualsourcecondition,allinfantswatchedthesameprosocial/antisocialpuppet show.Infantssatintheparent’slapbeforeatablewithacurtainatitsfarend(165cmfromtheinfants) thatcouldbeloweredtooccludetheshow.Puppetswereapproximately10in.high.Theprotagonist puppetwasalwaysabrowndog,theprosocialandantisocialpuppetswere(counterbalancedacross infants in each condition) a grey and an orange cat (Prosocial/Antisocial Source conditions) and a black&whiteandagreycat(NovelSourcecondition).Parentswereinstructedtositquietlywiththeir infantsandnotattempttoinfluencetheminanyway. PuppetshowswereidenticaltothoseinExperiment1byHamlinandWynn(2011).Duringall trials,thecurtainraisedtorevealastagesurroundedonthesidesandbackbyblackcurtains;theback curtainoccludedapuppeteer.Theprosocialandantisocialpuppetssatattherearcornersofthestage, andaclearboxcontainingarattlerestedinthemiddle.Theprotagonistenteredfromthebackofthe stage,movedtowardonesideofthebox,andleaneditsheaddowntolookthroughthesideofthe boxtwice.Itthenattemptedtoliftthelidoftheboxfourtimes,liftingslightlyanddroppingthelid twice,andthenliftingslightlyandloweringthelidtwice.Duringthefifthattempt,thepuppetonthe oppositesideofthestagefromtheprotagonist(whoenteredonalternatingsidesoftheboxduring eachtrial)intervened(seeFig.1). Duringprosocialevents,theprosocialpuppetrantothefrontofthebox,grabbedthecornerofthe lid,andopenedittogetherwiththeprotagonist.Theprotagonistdovedowninsidethebox,grabbing therattle.Theprosocialpuppetthenranoff-stage,andtheprotagonistliftedtherattlefromthebox. Allactionceased. J.K.Hamlin,K.Wynn/CognitiveDevelopment27 (2012) 227–239 231 Fig.1. Stimulipresentedtoinfants:rows1and2representtheprosocialandantisocialpuppetshows,inwhichadogpuppet triedbutfailedtoopenaclearboxcontainingarattle.Onprosocialevents(row1)theprosocialpuppethelpedopentheboxand thedogobtainedtherattle.Onantisocialevents(row2)theantisocialpuppetjumpedontheboxandthedogdidnotobtainthe rattle.Row3representsthefoodpreferencepresentationbyasourcewhowaspreviouslyeitherprosocial,novel,orantisocial. Duringantisocialevents,theantisocialpuppetrantothefrontoftheboxandjumpedsideways ontothelid,slammingitshut.Theprotagonistdovedownnexttothebox.Theantisocialpuppetthen ranoff-stage,andtheprotagonistsatupnexttothebox.Allactionceased. Bothprosocialandantisocialeventslastedapproximately15s.Anindependentcoderrecorded infants’lookstothedisplaythroughaholeinthecurtainfromthetimeactionceaseduntiltheinfant lookedawayfortwoconsecutiveseconds,oruntil30selapsed,asinpreviousstudies(Hamlin,Wynn, &Bloom,2007;Hamlin,Wynn,&Bloom,2010;Hamlinetal.,2011).Infantswerefamiliarizedtothree prosocialandthreeantisocialeventspresentedinalternation,forsixtotalevents.Thefollowingwere counterbalancedacrossinfantswithineachcondition:colorofprosocial/antisocialpuppets(black& whiteorgreyfortheNovelSourcecondition,andgreyororangefortheProsocial/AntisocialSource conditions),sideofprosocialpuppetduringevents(rightorleftsideofstage),andorderofprosocial events(firstorsecond). 1.2.3. Preferencedisplay(viewableonlineat www.yale.edu/infantlab/socialevaluationpreferencematching;seeFig.1) Infantssatontheparent’slapatatable,approximately105cmfromtheexperimenterwhohad performedtheeatingwarm-up.(Thisexperimenterhadnotobservedtheprosocial/antisocialpuppet show and was therefore blind to the identity of the source puppet in the Prosocial and Antisocial Sourceconditions.)Parentswereaskedtoclosetheireyes.Theexperimenterplacedtwoclearbowls onthetableinfrontofher,approximately30cmapart,onecontainingredFruityCheeriosandone containingpurpleFruityCheerios. Theexperimenterbroughtoutanorangecatpuppet(theProsocial,Novel,orAntisocialSource)on herrighthandandspoketoit,saying“HiKitty!”andmakingitwavetotheinfant.Shethenaskedthe 232 J.K.Hamlin,K.Wynn/CognitiveDevelopment27 (2012) 227–239 sourcepuppet:“Doyouwanttotrysome?”andmadeitnod“yes”towardtheinfant.Theexperimenter thenmovedthesourcepuppettowardoneofthefoodbowls,‘tasting’theCheeriosinitbyleaning intoandoutofthebowlandmakingeatingsounds.Aftertasting,thesourcepuppetexpressedeither apositive(“Mmmm!Yum!Ilikethat!”inahigh-pitched,positivevoice)ornegative(“Blech!Yuck!I don’tlikethat!”inalow-pitched,negativevoice)opinionoftheCheeriosinside.Afterexpressingits preference,thesourcepuppetmovedbacktowardtheexperimenter,whoasked:“Doyouwanttotry theotherone?”andmadeitnod“yes”towardtheinfant.Thesourcethentastedfromthesecondbowl andexpressedtheoppositeopinion. EachinfantsawthesourcetasteboththeredandpurpleCheerios,expressinglikingforoneand dislikingfortheother.Afterthetastepreferencesweremodeled,theexperimentersaid“ThanksKitty! Byebye!”andthesourcepuppetwavedtotheinfantandleft. Becausetheywereaskedtoclosetheireyesforthepreferencedisplay,parentsknewneitherthe identityofthesourcepuppetnorwhichfooditpreferred.Theexperimenterwasblindtotheprevious behaviorofthesourcepuppetintheProsocialandAntisocialSourceconditions.Thefollowingwere counterbalancedacrossinfants:locationofredCheerios,whetherredCheerioswerelikedordisliked, andwhetherlikingordislikingwasexpressedfirst. 1.2.4. Foodchoice Theexperimentermadeeyecontactwiththeinfant,pushedthetwofoodbowlswithinhisorher reach,andsaid,“Doyouwanttotrysome?”Shethenwaiteduntiltheinfanthadmadefourconsecutive foodchoices.Theexperimenterusedavarietyofwaystoencourageinfantstoeat.Encouragement waspermissibleaslongasaninfantwasnotcurrentlyholdinganyCheerios,norhadhisorherhands ineitherbowl,toavoidpromptingtheinfanttoeatfromaparticularbowl.Iftheinfantwasdistracted, theexperimentershookbothbowlsorknockedthemtogetherlightlyandrepeatedtheinvitationto trysome.IftheinfantseemedreluctanttotryanyCheerios,theexperimenterencouragedeatingby saying,“It’sOK,youcantrysome,”or“Youcaneatsomeifyouwant.”Althoughparentswereinstructed beforethestudynottotalktotheirinfantsduringthetask,ifinfantswereveryreluctanttoeat,the experimenterpromptedparentstotelltheinfant,“It’sOK.”Eatingandencouragingcontinueduntil infantshadmadefourclearfoodchoices. 1.3. Coding 1.3.1. Attentionfollowingpuppetshows Infants’attentionwascodedbyanonlinecoderfollowingeachpuppetshow(fromthetimeaction ceased, as described above). We calculated how long infants watched following prosocial puppet eventsandfollowingantisocialpuppeteventstoexaminewhetherthisinfluencedinfants’laterfood choices.Asecondindependentcodercodedarandom25%ofinfants’attentionineachcondition.The inter-ratercorrelationswere.995,.98and.97fortheProsocial,Novel,andAntisocialSourceconditions, respectively. 1.3.2. Attentioncodingtofoodpreferencedisplay To examine the effect of infants’ attention to the sources on eating behaviors, an independent coder(blindtocondition)codedeachinfant’sattentiontothefoodpreferencedisplayfromvideotape. Becausepreferencewasdisplayedliveandtherewerethereforesmalldifferencesinthetotallengthof eachpresentation,wealsocodedthetotaldisplaylengthforeachinfantanddeterminedtheproportion ofthetotaldisplaytimethateachinfantattendedto.Asecondindependentcodercodedarandom 25%ofinfants’attentionineachcondition.Theinter-ratercorrelationcoefficientwas.77. 1.3.3. Foodchoicecoding The experimenter coded infants’ choices as the first four times an infant grasped one or more Cheeriosfromabowlandatethemwithoutinterruption.“Interruptions”werecodedasanybehavior J.K.Hamlin,K.Wynn/CognitiveDevelopment27 (2012) 227–239 233 thatcouldhaveledtoaninfantnotknowingwhichbowlaCheeriohadcomefrom2andtookavariety offorms,includingdroppingCheeriosontoclothingorthetableandlaterfindingandeatingthem, picking up multiple different-colored Cheerios with both hands and moving them back and forth betweentheirhands.Theexperimenterdeterminedwhetherachoicehadbeeninterrupted.Eating continueduntiltheinfantmadefournon-interruptedchoices.Eachinfantreceivedascoreindicating thenumberoftrials(offour)inwhichtheyatethefoodthepuppetmodelhadliked. Anindependentcoderblindtothemodel’sidentityrecodedarandomlychosen25%ofinfants’ choicesandagreedwiththeoriginalexperimenteron100%oftrials. 2. Results 2.1. Attentiontopuppetevents Collapsedacrosscondition,infantslookedlongeratprosocialpuppetshowevents(mean=32.58s of total looking over three trials, SEM=3.35) than antisocial puppet show events (mean=26.25s, SE M=2 .69); F(1 ,47)= 10.08 , p<.0 05, (cid:2) 2 =.1 8. Th is tendenc y did not di ffer ac ross co n dition s, p F(2,45)=.75,p=.48,(cid:2)2=.03,nordidinfants’relativeattentiontoprosocialversusantisocialevents p (adding this as a covariate in the choice analyses) influence their food choices across condition, F(1,46)= .004 ,p =. 95,(cid:2)2=.0 0, orw ithinan yconditio nalone(a llps> .28). p 2.2. Attentiontothepreferencedisplay InfantsintheProsocialSourceconditionlookedtothepresentationanaverageof95%ofthetime (SEM=1%);infantsintheNovelSourceconditionlookedanaverageof95%ofthetime(SEM=1%),and infant s inth eAntis oc ialS ource conditio nlooked anaver age of96%o ft het im e(S EM= 1%).3 Th ism ea- suredidnotdifferacrossconditionaccordingtoaunivariateanalysisofvariance(ANOVA),F(2,40)=.32, p=.7 3,(cid:2) 2= .02), andwh enadded asacova ria te didnotsi gnifican tly influenc einfants’ foodch o ices p acrosscondition,F(1,39)=.76,p=.40,(cid:2)2=.02,orwithinanycondition(ps>.35). p 2.3. Foodchoice Infants’foodchoicesbyconditionarerepresentedinFigs.2and3.AunivariateANOVAinclud- ingcondition,sexofinfant,orderofprosocialactionduringfamiliarization,coloroflikedCheerios, and order of liking event as between-subject factors, and including age as a covariate revealed only aneff ect ofcon dition on infants’foodcho ices,F(2, 45)= 6.094,p< .05, (cid:2)2 = .484,andn osignif- p icantinteractions.Plannedcontrastsusingone-samplet-testsrevealedthatinfantsintheProsocial Sourceconditionsignificantlyatethesource’spreferredoverdispreferredfood,mean(SEM)=3.0(.27); t(15)=3.65,p<.005,asdidinfantsintheNovelSourcecondition,mean(SEM)=2.88(.26),t(15)=3.42, p<.005. In contrast, infants who saw the antisocial puppet’s food preference ate the two foods eq u ally, me an(SEM) =1.69(.2 5);t( 15)= −1.2 3,p=.24.T hissignifi cant tendencyto ma tcht heso urce’s preference in the Prosocial and Novel Source conditions but not in the Antisocial Source condi- tionwasevidentinnon-parametricKolmogorov–Smirnovone-sampletests,Dmax(Prosocial)=.437, p<.01,Dmax(Novel)=.375,p<.05,Dmax(Antisocial)=.187,p>.05,andinindividualinfants’patternsof response.Thatis,12of16infantschosetheProsocialSource’spreferredfoodmorethanhalfthetime (onthreeorfouroffourchoices,chance=31.5%,binomialtest,p<.001),11of16infantsintheNovel Sourceconditiondidso(binomialtest,p<.005),andonlytwoof16infantsintheAntisocialSource 2 Whileourtwofoodsweredistinguishablebybothcolorandlocation,itisunclearwhichofthesecuesinfantsusedto distinguishthem.Recentworksuggeststhatyounginfantsmaynotyetcategorizefoodtypesasadultsdo(i.e.,uniquelyby substance;Birch,1980;Brody&Stoneman,1981;c.f.VanderBorght&Jaswal,2009;Hendy&Raudenbush,2000;Jaswal& Neely,2007;Shutts,Condry,Santos,&Spelke,2009). 3 Du etoeq uipmen terror, videoda ta werelo stfromtwoinfantsintheprosocialandthreeinfantsintheantisocialcondition; thisanalysisthereforeincludesdatafromtheremaining14and13infantsinthosetwoconditions,respectively. 234 J.K.Hamlin,K.Wynn/CognitiveDevelopment27 (2012) 227–239 Fig.2. Results:numberoftimes(offour)infantsatelikedversusdislikedfoodfollowingpreferencemodelingbyProsocial, Novel,orAntisocialSource. conditiondidso.Furtheranalysesrevealedthatinfantsweresignificantlymorelikelytomatchthe preferenceoftheProsocialSourcethantheAntisocialSource,t(30)=3.52,p<.005;Fisher’sexacttest p<.001.TheywerealsomorelikelytomatchthepreferenceoftheNovelSourcethantheAntisocial Source,t(30)=3.29,p<.005,Fisher’sexacttest,p<.005,butequallylikelytomatchthepreferenceof theProsocialandNovelSource,t(30)=.333,p=.71,Fisher’sexacttest,p=.99. 3. Discussion Resultssuggestthatinfantsgaininformationfromcertainindividualsandignoreinformationfrom others.Whenpresentedwiththefoodpreferencesofapuppetwhohadpreviouslybeenprosocial towardathirdparty,infantschosethefoodthepuppethadexpressedlikingoverthefooditexpressed dislikingfor.Incontrast,whenapreviouslyantisocialpuppetpresenteditspreferences,infantsdid not appear to take this information into account in deciding what to eat, choosing both the liked anddislikedfoodsequally.ComparingtheseresultswiththoseintheNovelSourceconditionsug- geststhatthiseffectisdrivenbyarelativefailuretomatchthefoodpreferencesofantisocialothers: infantswereequallylikelytomatchthefoodpreferencesofanunknownandaprosocialsource.Taken together,theseresultssupporttheclaimthatone’sevaluationofapotentialsourceofinformationmay contributetoselectivelearningininfancy. Fig.3. Results:numberofinfantswhomatchedthesource’spreferencezero,one,two,threeorfouroffourtimesfollowing preferencemodelingbyProsocial,Novel,orAntisocialSource. J.K.Hamlin,K.Wynn/CognitiveDevelopment27 (2012) 227–239 235 Note,however,thatthecurrentstudydidnotdirectlytestwhetherinfantspositivelyevaluated theprosocialcharacterandnegativelyevaluatedtheantisocialcharacter.Wechosenottoexamine infants’ preferences in order to avoid influencing their subsequent interactions with the puppets. Instead,infants’evaluationswereinferredfrompreviousstudies–allusingtheboxscenarioutilized here–whichhavedemonstratedthat(1)infantsprefertheprosocialtotheantisocialcharacter(Hamlin &Wynn,2011),(2)infantspreferthosewhohelptheprosocialcharacterandthosewhohinderthe antisocialcharacter(Hamlinetal.,2011),and(3)toddlersthemselvesgivetreatstotheprosocialchar- acterandtaketreatsawayfromtheantisocialcharacter(Hamlinetal.,2011).Together,theseresults stronglysuggestsocialevaluationistakingplaceinthepresentstudy;however,weacknowledgethat inferredevaluationisapotentiallimitationoftheresults. Importantly,theobservedeffectscannotbeaccountedforbyattentionaldifferences:infantswere aslikelytoattendtothepreferencesofProsocial,Antisocial,andNovelSources,andtheirattention followingprosocialandantisocialeventsdidnotinfluencetheirfoodchoices.Itisadditionallydifficult toexplainthispatternofresultsasstemmingfrompurelyassociativemechanisms.Becauseallinfants sawbothprosocialandantisocialpuppetevents,wecanbesurethattheydidnot,forexample,simply feellesssocialafterviewingantisocialeventsandsubsequentlyfailtotakeinanysource’sinformation. Inaddition,becauseallsourcesexpressedbothpositiveandnegativefoodpreferences,infantscould nothavesimplytaggedfooditemswithpositiveversusnegativeemotionalinformationwithoutalso consideringthesourceofthatinformation,norcouldtheyhavemerelyrespondedtothepresenceof positiveversusnegativesourceswithoutconsideringthespecificpreferenceinformation.Finally,asall infantswereprovidedwithonlyonepotentialsourceofinformation,theycouldnothavebasedtheir responsesonwhetheronesourcewasrelativelypreferabletoanothersource,butonlyonwhether theonlysourceavailablehadinformationworthusing. Thereareseveralwaystointerprettheseresults.Onepossibilityisthatinfantsconsideredasource’s emotionalinformationtoreflectitsindividualpreference.Previousresearchhasshownthatby13 monthsofage,infantsexpectsomekindsofinformation(e.g.,language)tobesharedacrossindividuals butappreciatethatotherkindsofinformation(e.g.,objectpreferences)maydifferfromoneindividual toanother(Buresh&Wooward,2007;Repacholi&Gopnik,1997).Ifinfantsinterpretedthepuppet’s expressions to reflect personal attitudes about the foods, our results may reflect infants’ desire to affiliatewithand/or“belike”theNovelandProsocialSources,butnottheantisocialsource.Whilethe puppetmodelitselfwasnotpresentduringinfants’eatingtest(andwasthereforeperhapsunavailable foraffiliation),thepuppeteerexperimenterwaspresent,whichmayhaveexertedasocialinfluence oninfants’choice.Researchwithchildrenandadultssuggeststhatimitativebehaviorisbothcause andconsequenceofadrivetoaffiliate(Chartrand&Bargh,1999;Lakin&Chartrand,2003;Over& Carpenter,2009;Over&Carpenter,2012;Uzgiris,1981).Ourresultssuggestthatthislinkbetween imitation/emulationandlikingmaybepresentininfancy. Alternatively,infantsmayhavetakenthesources’expressedpreferenceinformationtobeabout theactualstateoftheworld(i.e.,thataparticularfoodisgoodandanotherfoodisbad).Ifso,infants’ selectivitymayreflectlowerlevelsoftrustintheaccuracyofinformationprovidedbyanAntisocial Source,similarlyto howolderchildrenbelievethata previouslyinaccuratelabelermay also have incorrectknowledgeofotherobjectnames(Birch&Bloom,2002;Sabbagh&Baldwin,2001).Onthis explanation,wewouldexpectinfantstoavoidtheProsocialorNovelSource’sdislikedCheeriocolor innewsituationsinwhichneitherpuppetnorpuppeteerispresent. Finally,thisstudyislimitedinthatthepuppets’behaviorwastheonlyinformationinfantshad abouttheirpotentialvalueasknowledgesources.Intherealworld,learnersoftenknowmuchmore inadditiontoasource’simmediatebehavior,suchastheirpriorbehavioralhistory,skillsindifferent areas,andgroupmemberships,Wemayalsoknowsomethingaboutthetargetofone’sbehaviors, suchaswhetherthetargetsthemselvesaregoodorbad,orwhetherornottheyareinourgroup. Infants’evaluationsofhelpfulandunhelpfulcharactersareknowntodifferdependingonthepast actsofthetargetofthosecharacters’behaviors(Hamlinetal.,2011);perhapsinfants’judgmentsof theAntisocialSourcemighthavedifferedhadtheyknownmoreaboutthesourceitself(forinstance,if itwereanin-groupmember)ormoreaboutthetargetoftheAntisocialSource’sbehavior(forinstance, ifthetargetwerebadoranout-groupmember).Furtherstudyshouldattempttoteaseapartthese possibilitiestoshedlightontheroleofaffiliationandlikinginsociallearning. 236 J.K.Hamlin,K.Wynn/CognitiveDevelopment27 (2012) 227–239 Interestingly,infantsinthepresentstudieswereequallylikelytomatchthefoodpreferencesof theProsocialandNovelSources,relativetotheirreluctancetomatchtheAntisocialSource,whom theyneithermatchednormismatched.Whileourresultsmayrepresentaceilingeffectonpreference matchingoftheprosocialandneutralsources,andfurtherstudycouldshowthatprosocialsources areprivilegedoverneutralones,infantsinboththeProsocialandNovelSourceconditionsdidsome- timeseatthefoodstheirsourcehaddisliked(onaverageonceoffourtimes),makingthispossibility somewhatlessplausible.Ourresultssuggestthatthebaselineresponsemaybetoacceptothers’infor- mationunlesstheypresentsomereasonnotto(inthiscase,beingantisocial).Thisresponsepattern wouldbeadaptive(morelearningmaybebetter)andisreminiscentofthenegativitybias,inwhich negative(relativetopositiveorneutral)individualsandactionsmaybeparticularlysalient,influen- tial,andmemorabletoadultandchildobservers(Abelson&Kanouse,1966;Aloise,1993;Kanouse &Hanson,1972;Knobe,2003a;Leslie,Knobe,&Cohen,2006).Theseresultssupportclaimsthatthe negativitybiasisearly-developing(Hamlinetal.,2010;Vaish,Grossman,&Woodward,2008),andis alsohighlyconsistentwithrecentreportsofa“pitchforkeffect”inpreschoolers’selectivelearning: Beingpreviouslyinaccuratehasarelativelygreaterinfluenceonchildren’stendencytogaininforma- tionfromasourcethandoesbeingpreviouslyaccurate(Corriveau,Meints,&Harris,2009;Koenig& Jaswal,2011). Overall,theseresultsindicatethatinfantsareselectivenotonlyinwhatkindsofinformationthey gainfromsocialsources,butalsoregardingwhichsourcestheyreceiveinformationfrom.Whilewe havenotyetaccountedforthespecificmechanismleadingtoinfants’responses(accuracy,familiar- ity/similarity,liking),ourfindingsaddtothesmallbutexpandingliteratureonselectivesourcelearning andimitationininfancy(Chowetal.,2008;Koenig&Woodward,2010;Nielsen,2006;Nielsenetal., 2008;Ryallsetal.,2000;Shutts,Condry,etal.,2009;Shutts,Kinzler,etal.,2009;Zarbatany&Lamb, 1985;Zmyjetal.,2010),aswellastorecentevidencethatinfantsevaluateothersbasedontheirpro- andanti-socialbehaviors(Dunfield&Kuhlmeier,2010;Geraci&Surian,2011;Hamlin&Wynn,2011; Hamlinetal.,2007;Hamlinetal.,2010;Schmidt&Sommerville,2011),andcallsforfurthereluci- dation.Ifsimplyseeinganindividualbehaveantisociallytowardanunknownthird-partydecreases infants’tendencynotonlytointeractwiththatindividual,butalsotousethatindividualasasource ofnewinformation,earlydevelopingmechanismsforsocialevaluationmay,overtime,havelasting effectsondevelopment. Acknowledgements WethankNehaMahajan,AnnieSpokes,andothermembersoftheInfantCognitionCenteratYale University,andtheparentsandinfantswhoparticipated. References Abelson,R.P.,&Kanouse,D.M.(1966).Subjectiveacceptanceofverbgeneralizations.InS.Feldman(Ed.),Cognitiveconsistency (pp.1 7 3–1 9 9).SanDi ego ,C A:Acad emicPress . Aloise,P .A.(1993). Trai tconfir mat ionanddi sconfirmation:Thedevelopmentofattributionbiases.JournalofExperimentalChild Psy ch olo gy,55, 177– 193. Bandura,A.(19 77) .Sociallearningtheory.NewYork:GeneralLearningPress. Barr,R.,D ow den,A .,&Ha yne,H. (1996). Deve lopme ntalcha ngesind eferredimitationby6-to24-month-oldinfants.Infant B eha viorandD ev elo pment, 19 ,159–17 1. Bellagamba, F.,& Tomasello,M .( 1999).Re-enactingintendedacts:Comparing12-and18-month-olds.InfantBehaviorand Developm en t, 22,277–282 . Birch,L.L.(1980). Effe ctsofpeermodels’foodchoicesandeatingbehaviorsonpreschoolers’foodpreferences.ChildDevelopment, 51 ,4 8 9–496. Birch,S .,Vauthier,S.,&Bloom,P.(2008).Three-andfour-year-oldsspontaneouslyuseothers’pastperformancetoguidetheir lea rn ing.Cogni tio n, 107(3), 10 18–103 4. Birch,S.A.J. ,&Bloom, P.(200 2).Preschoolersaresensitivetothespeaker’sknowledgewhenlearningpropernames.Child De ve lop m en t,73,43 4– 444. Bradbard,M.R.,& E ndsley,R.C.(1983).Theeffectsofsex-typedlabelingonpreschoolchildren’sinformation-seekingand retent ion .Se xR oles,9,24 7– 26 0. Bradbard,M.R .,M artin, C. L.,Endsley,R.C.,&Halverson,C.F.(1986).Influenceofsexstereotypesonchildren’sexplorationand memo ry: A compete nc e versuspe rfo rm a ncedistinc tio n. Develop mentalPs ych olo gy,22,481–4 86 .
Description: