ebook img

When Arousal Influences Ad Evaluation and Valence Does Not PDF

14 Pages·2001·0.24 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview When Arousal Influences Ad Evaluation and Valence Does Not

JOURNAL OF CONSUMER PSYCHOLOGY,11(1),43–55 Copyright © 2001, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. When Arousal Influences Ad Evaluation and Valence Does Not (and Vice Versa) Gerald Gorn Department of Marketing Hong Kong University of Science and Technology Michel Tuan Pham Graduate School of Business Columbia University Leo Yatming Sin Department of Business Administration Chinese University of Hong Kong Thisresearchexamines,across2studies,theinterplaybetweenthevalenceandarousalcompo- nentsofaffectivestatesandtheaffectivetoneofatargetad.Inthefirststudy,musicwasusedtoin- duceapleasantorunpleasantmood,whilecontrollingforarousal.Participantsweresubsequently exposedtoanadthateitherhadapositive-affectivetoneorwasambiguousinitsaffectivetone.As predicted,thevalenceoftheaffectivestatecoloredtheevaluationoftheadinamood-congruent direction,butthiscoloringeffectoccurredonlywhentheadhadanambiguous-affectivetone.In thesecondstudy,thetargetadhadaclearpositiveornegativeaffectivetone,andthevalenceand arousaldimensionsofthemoodstateweremanipulatedindependently.Aspredicted,thearousal dimension,butnotthevalencedimension,influencedadevaluation.Adevaluationsweremore polarizedinthedirectionofthead’saffectivetoneunderhigharousalthanunderlowarousal.This effectwasmorepronouncedforself-referentevaluations(e.g.,“Ilikethead”)thanforobject-ref- erentevaluations(e.g.,“Theadisgood”),favoringanattributionalexplanation—theexcitation transferhypothesis—overanattention-narrowingexplanation—thedynamiccomplexityhypoth- esis.Takentogether,theresultsofthe2studiesstresstheimportantcontingencyoftheaffective toneofthead,whenexaminingtheeffectsofthevalenceandarousaldimensionsofaperson’saf- fectivestateonadevaluation.Theresultsalsoprovideadditionalinsightsintohowandwhenaf- fectservesasinformationinjudgmentprocesses. Thesourcesofaffectcanbediverse.Inadexposuresettings, of this interplay, formalize its main contingencies, and ex- onesourceofaffectistheconsumer’spreexistingmoodstate plore underlying processes. atthetimeofexposure.Asecondsourceliesintheaffective Wesuggestthattoexaminethisinterplay,itisusefultode- toneof the aditself(e.g.,humorousor fear-inducing).1The composeconsumers’affectivestatesalongtheirtwoprimary interplaybetweentheconsumer’saffectivestateandtheaf- dimensions(Havlena&Holbrook,1986;Mehrabian&Rus- fective content of the adcan have intricate effects on judg- sell, 1974): valence (pleasant or unpleasant) and arousal ments.Thepurposeofthisresearchistodocumentthenature (highorlow).Weproposethateachofthesetwocomponents of consumers’ affective states interacts differently with the affectivetoneofatargetad.Priorresearchhassuggestedthat thevalencecomponenthastheeffectof“coloring”people’s RequestforreprintsshouldbesenttoMichelTuanPham,515UrisHall, judgmentsinamood-congruentdirection(e.g.,Forgas,1995; GraduateSchoolofBusiness,ColumbiaUniversity,3022Broadway,New Mayer,Gashke,Braverman,&Evans,1992).Ceterisparibus, York, NY 10027. E-mail: [email protected] thetargetadshouldbeevaluatedmorefavorablywhentheaf- 1Byaffectivetone(e.g.,humorous),wemeanthattheadhasthepotentialto evokespecificfeelingsrelatedtothetone(e.g.,feelingsofamusement=laughter). fective state is pleasant than when it is unpleasant (e.g., 44 GORN, PHAM, SIN Goldberg&Gorn,1987).However,wearguethatthiscolor- biguous.Forinstance,IsenandShalker(1982)observedthat ingeffectismorelikelytooccurwhentheadisambiguousin the effect of being in a pleasant mood when assessing the itsaffectivetonethanwhentheaditselfhasaclearlypositive pleasantnessofslidesoflocalsceneswasstrongerwhenthe ornegativeaffectivetone.Whentheaddoeshaveaclearaf- slides were affectively neutral than when the slides were fectivetone—andmoodvalencehaslessofaninfluence—it clearly pleasant or unpleasant. Similarly, Miniard, Bhatla, isthearousalcomponentthathasaneffectonadevaluation. andSirdeshmukh(1992)observedthattheeffectofbeingina Thiseffectistopolarizejudgmentsbyaccentuatingtheaffec- pleasant or unpleasant mood on participants’ rating of the tivetoneofthetarget ad.Adsthathavea positive-affective taste of a brand of peanut butter was stronger when the tone are likely to be evaluated more favorably under high brand’s actual taste was ambiguous than when it clearly arousalthanunderlowarousal,whereasadsthathaveanega- tastedgoodorbad.Severalmechanisms,whicharenotneces- tiveaffectivetonearelikelytobeevaluatedlessfavorablyun- sarilymutuallyexclusive,canexplainwhymoodshaveless der high arousal than under low arousal. In other words, ofaneffectwhenthetargetisaffectivelyunambiguous.First, dependingontheaffectivetoneofthead,thevalenceofaffec- thetarget’saffectivecontentmayactuallyerasethepreexist- tivestateshasaneffectwhenarousaldoesnot,andviceversa. ingmoodstate,precludinganymoodinfluenceonjudgment Thesepredictionsaretestedintwostudies:onefocusingon (Miniardetal.,1992).Second,whenthetargetisaffectively coloring and the other focusing on polarization. The pro- unambiguous,themoodstatewillloseitsheuristic(informa- cesses underlying coloring and polarization are discussed, tion) value in evaluative judgments (Clore et al., 1994). withtwodifferentexplanationsof polarizationexaminedin Third,judgmentsofaffectivelyunambiguoustargetsareless Study 2. likelytorequireasearchforadditionalinformation.Asare- sult,mood-congruentassociationswillhavelessofaninflu- ence on these judgments (Isen & Shalker, 1982). VALENCE, AROUSAL, AND Therolethataffectiveambiguityofthetargetplaysinmod- AD EVALUATION eratingthecoloringeffectofmoodvalencecanalsobeinter- preted in light of Forgas’s (1995) Affect Infusion Model Valence and Coloring (AIM).Accordingtothismodel,moodismostlikelyto“in- fuse”(i.e.,color)judgmentswhenpeopleuseagenerativepro- Aconsiderableamountofresearchhassuggestedthattheva- cessingstrategy—thatis,a heuristic or systematic searchfor lenceofpeople’saffectivestatesinfluencestheirjudgments additionalinputs—toconstructtheirjudgments.Whenthetar- in a mood-congruent direction (e.g., Clore, Schwarz, & getisaffectivelyunambiguous,agenerativeprocessingstrat- Conway,1994;Forgas,1995;Goldberg&Gorn,1987;Gorn, egy—hence, infusion or coloring—is less likelybecause the Goldberg,&Basu,1993;Mayeretal.,1992).Twoexplana- target’saffectivetoneprovidespotentcuesforthejudgments. tionshavebeenofferedforthiscoloringeffect.Oneexplana- Consistentwiththisreasoning,wehypothesizethefollowing: tionisthatmoodstates—pleasantorunpleasant—cue simi- larlyvalencedmaterialsinmemory,therebybiasingpeople’s H1: The coloringeffect of the valence of people’s perceptionsofthetargetatthetimeofevaluation(Isenetal., affectivestatewillbemorepronouncedforads 1978).Anotherexplanation,knownasaffect-as-information, thathaveanambiguous-affectivetonethanfor is that people often inspect their feelings when making ads that have a clear affective tone. evaluative judgments (Schwarz & Clore, 1988; see also Pham, 1998). During this process, pleasant or unpleasant Arousal and Polarization feelingsemanatingfromthemoodstateatthetimeofjudg- ment may be misattributed to the target (Schwarz & Clore, Asubstantialamountofresearch(seeReisenzein,1983)has 1983). Forgas(1995)suggestedthatthesetwoexplanations suggested that arousal experienced ata given point in time areinfactcomplementaryandapplytodifferentprocessing may accentuate subsequent affective and evaluative re- strategies. When the person uses a systematic processing sponses. Consumers’ responses to a given object—for in- strategy, mood will primarily influence judgments through stance,anad—maybemoreextremeorpolarizedifthecon- thecuingofmood-congruentthoughts.Whenthepersonuses sumershavebeenrecentlyaroused.Twoexplanations have a heuristic processing strategy, mood will influence judg- been offered for this effect. One invokes attributional pro- mentsthroughaffect-as-informationmechanisms.Still,both cesses, and the other invokes processing interferences. explanations suggest that the valence of people’s affective stateatthetimeofexposuretoanadwillcoloritsevaluation. Polarization as misattribution. BuildingonSchachter Research has suggested that this coloring effect may be andSinger’s(1962)work,Zillmann(1971)offeredtheexcita- contingentonanumberoffactors(Cloreetal.,1994;Martin, tiontransferhypothesis.Itiswellestablishedthatarousal,asa Abend,Sekides,&Green,1997;Pham,1998).Ofparticular generalized state of activation, tends to be nonspecific (e.g., interestisresearchshowingthatmood-congruentevaluations Schachter&Singer,1962).Moreover,arousaldoesnotdissipate maybemorepronouncedwhenthetargetsareaffectivelyam- immediately after the removal of the arousing condition: MOOD VALENCE, AROUSAL, AND AFFECTIVE TONE 45 Arousal decays relatively slowly (e.g., Cantor, Zillmann, & many ads have a positive affective tone (e.g., humorous, Bryant,1975).Asaresult,theresidualarousalexperiencedfol- warmcommercials),thepositivityofthistoneislikelytovary lowinganeventmaypolarizetheresponsetoasubsequenttarget (e.g., some commercials are warmer than others). Study 1 by intensifying the affect elicited by this target. For instance, teststhehypothesisthatthecoloringeffectofvalencewillbe White,Fishbein,andRutstein(1981)observedthatarousal,in- morepronouncedwhentheaffectivetoneofthetargetadis duced through eitherphysical exercise or emotional material, ambiguousthanwhenitisclearlypositive.Thepolarization subsequentlyincreased(decreased)maleparticipants’likingof effectshouldmanifestitselfinthereverseconditioninwhich an attractive (unattractive) female target. Apparently, partici- theaffectivetoneofthetargetadisunambiguous(asopposed pantsmisattributedtheresidualarousalleftbytheprioreventas toambiguous).InStudy2,wetestthehypothesisthatarousal partoftheirgenuineaffectiveresponsetothefemaletargetand willhaveapolarizationeffectwhenthead’saffectivetoneis thereforeexpressedmorepolarizedjudgments.Thisintensify- clearlypositiveornegative.Undersuchconditions,valence ingeffectofresidualarousalhasbeenreplicatedinmanystudies will have little influence. (forareview,seeReisenzein,1983). Polarization as reduced cognitive complexity. The STUDY 1 dynamic complexity hypothesis (Paulhus & Lim, 1994) statesthatarousalpolarizesevaluativejudgmentsbyreduc- Thepurposeofthisstudywastotestthepropositionthatthe ingthecomplexityofperceptionsaboutatarget.Thishypoth- coloring effect of consumers’ mood states on their evalua- esiselaboratesonthetheorythatbecausearousalreducespro- tionsofadvertisementswoulddependontheaffectiveambi- cessing capacity, aroused individuals tend to selectively guityofthead.Becausetheexperimentfocusedonthecolor- processimportantcuesattheexpenseoflessimportantones ing effect of affect states, participants’ mood was (Easterbrook, 1959). Because the primary dimensionof so- manipulatedtobe eitherpleasantor unpleasant, controlling cialperceptionisevaluative(Osgood,Suci,&Tannenbaum, forvariationinarousal.Followingthemoodinduction,par- 1957),selectiveprocessingfocusingonthisdimensionatthe ticipantsweresubsequentlyexposedtothetargetadinasup- expense of nonevaluative dimensions will polarize judg- posedly unrelated task. In one condition, the ad’s affective ments(Paulhus&Lim,1994).Inastudyofpeople’sevalua- tonewasclearlypositive.Intheothercondition,thead’saf- tionoffamousfiguresandsocialacquaintances,Paulhusand fecttonewasmoreambiguous.Itwaspredictedthattheva- Limfoundthatarousaldidresultinsimplerperceptionsand lence of participants’moodwouldcolortheirevaluationof thatthesesimplerperceptionsseemedtoleadtomorepolar- the target ad in the ambiguous-ad condition but not in the ized judgments. Consistent withthis hypothesis, itwas ob- clearly positive-ad condition. servedinan advertisingcontext that arousal could polarize brandevaluationby increasing people’sreliance on which- Method ever cues—good or bad—were perceived to be more diag- nostic (Pham, 1996). Participants and design. Participants consisted of Therefore, there is a strong theoretical reason tobelieve 80 business undergraduateswhoparticipatedin thestudy thatthearousalcomponentofconsumers’affectivestatesat to fulfill a course requirement. They were randomly as- thetimeofexposuretoanadvertisementshouldpolarizeits signedtooneoffourconditionsina2×2between-subjects evaluation. This effect should depend on the ad’s affective design.Thefirstfactormanipulatedtwolevelsofaffective tone.Ifthead’sprimaryaffectivetoneispositive,evaluations state: pleasant versus unpleasant. The second factor ma- shouldbemorefavorableasarousalincreases.Ifthead’saf- nipulatedtwolevelsoftheaffectivetoneofthead:positive fectivetoneisnegative,evaluationsshouldbelessfavorable versusambiguous. as arousal increases. In principle, thispolarization effect of arousal shouldbe independent of anycoloring effect of the Procedure. Theexperimentwasintroducedastwoun- valence component of the affective state. relatedstudies,conductedintwoseparatesmallrooms.The “first”studywaspurportedlyaboutmusicalappreciation.To H2: Whenadshaveaclearlypositiveornegativeaf- enhanceexperimentalcontrol,thestudywasconductedone fectivetone,higharousalwillpolarizeadeval- participant atatime. Participantswere seatedina comfort- uations in the direction of this tone. ablechairwhileoneofthetwoselectedmusicalpieces(see Pretest1next)wasplayedthroughahigh-qualitycassettere- Insummary,thetwomaincomponentsofconsumers’af- corder. Participants were instructed to listen carefully and fectivestatesatthetimeofexposuretoanadshouldhavedis- formimpressionsof,andopinionsabout,themusic.Afterlis- tincteffectsonitsevaluation.Thevalencecomponentshould teningtothemusicfor5min,participantswereaskedtoas- colortheevaluationoftheadinamood-congruentdirection, sess the music on a number of dimensions (e.g., fast–slow, whereasthearousalcomponentshouldpolarizeitsevaluation quiet–loud,andinteresting–uninteresting).Participantswere inthedirectionofthead’sprimaryaffectivetone.Although thendirectedtothe“second”study,whichwaspurportedlya 46 GORN, PHAM, SIN pilotadvertisingtestconductedbythemarketingdepartment smilingchild.Intheambiguous-affective-tonecondition,the fora private company. Participantswere exposed to one of picturewasamoredistantshotofamanandwomantakinga thetwotargetads(seePretest2next)for40sec.Theythen walk.Asidefromthepicture,thetwoadswereidenticalinev- evaluated the ad on four 7-point items: “I like (dislike) the eryrespect. Bothversions of the ad had a headline reading ad,”“Theadisgood(bad),”“Theadlooks(doesnot)profes- “YOUCANENJOYMOREFROMLIFE!”andashortcopy sionally made,” and “I feel positive (negative) towards the about the benefits of insurance with the company. ad.”Theseitems(a=.84)wereaveragedintoasinglemea- Thetwoversionsoftheadwereshowntoseparategroups sure of ad evaluation. ofparticipants(N=71).Theseparticipantswereaskedtoex- aminetheadandassesshowitmadethemfeel.Theaffective toneoftheadwasassessedbyfour7-pointsemanticdifferen- Pretest 1: Music selection. Becauseitisimportantto tialitems(a=.92)anchoredat“happy–sad,”“pleased–dis- separatethevalenceandarousaldimensionsofaffectivestates, pleased,” “delighted–distressed,” and “joyful–depressed.” we investigatedmanipulationsof valence that would control The arousal contentofthe adwasassessedby three7-point forthelevelofinducedarousal.Weelectedtorelyonmusicfor semantic differential items anchored at “stimulated–re- severalreasons.First,musichasbeenrepeatedlyshowntobe laxed,” “excited–calm,” and “aroused–unaroused.” As in- aneffectivemanipulationofvalenceofaffectivestates,bothin tended,theadwiththehappyfamilywasjudgedtobemore marketing studies (e.g., Alpert & Alpert, 1990; Kellaris & positiveintoneandelicitedmorepositivefeelings(M=5.09) Kent,1993;Miniardetal.,1992)andinpsychologicalstudies thandidtheadwiththecouple walking(M =4.40), t(69) = (e.g.,Eich&Metcalfe,1989).Second,comparedtomostso- 3.65,p<.01.Thetwoadswerenotdifferentinthefeelingsof cial manipulations of affective states—such as performance arousaltheyelicited(positivetonead:M=3.45andambigu- feedback,giftgiving,andinteractionwithaconfederate—mu- ous tone ad:M= 3.26),t< 1. sicisrelativelycontentfree.Finally,thereisanenormousvari- abilityofmusicavailableinthemarketplace.Thisfacilitated thesearchforpiecesthatwouldinducedifferentlevelsofva- Results lencewithoutinducingdifferentlevelsofarousal. Ourgoalinpretestingwastofind2 piecesofmusic that Manipulation checks. To check the perceived affec- wereequallyarousingbutonethatwasmorepleasingthanthe tivetoneoftheadinthemainexperiment,participants’rat- other. In a preliminary pretest (n = 86), 44 pieces of music ingsofthepleasantnessofthead(astheyagainlookedatit) wereevaluatedonpleasureandarousal.Participantswererun weresubmittedtoa2(adtone)×2(valence)analysisofvari- insmallgroupsof4to6people,witheachgroupevaluating6 ance(ANOVA).Asexpected, theANOVArevealedonly a piecesofmusic.Basedonthispreliminarypretest,2piecesof maineffectofadtone,F(1,79)=9.23,p<.01,showingthat musicwereselectedtobetestedinamoreformalpretestin theperceivedpleasantnessoftheadwassignificantlyhigher which 23 participants were run individuallyandlistened to inthe positive-adcondition(M = 4.88)thanintheambigu- only1pieceofmusic.Afterlisteningtoeachpiecefor5min, ous-ad condition (M = 4.30). Although the positive ad’s participantsratedhowpleasing and arousingthemusicwas pleasantness was clearly above the midpoint of the scale, on7-pointscales.Asexpected,thefirstpiece(“EineKleine t(39) =8.54,p<.001,theambiguousad’spleasantnesswas Nachtmusik:Allegro”byMozart)wasratedaspleasing(M= onlymarginallyabovethemidpointofthescale,t(39)=1.91, 5.70)andhighinarousal(M=5.70).Thesecondpiece(anIn- p=.06. Themaineffectof valenceandtheAd Tone× Va- dian classical piece by Pandit Dhimsen Joshi) was ratedas lence interaction did not approach significance,F’s < 1. muchlesspleasing(M=2.23)butstillhighinarousal(M= Theaffective-statemanipulationwascheckedwithanin- 5.61).Itwasconfirmedby t-teststhatthe2 piecesof music dependentsampleofparticipants(N=20)whowererunindi- weredifferentinhowpleasingtheywere,t(21)=11.71,p< vidually. Each participant listened for 5 min (the same .001, but not in arousal,t(21) = .21,p> .10. amountoftimeasinthemainexperiment)tooneofthetwo piecesofmusicusedinthemainexperiment.Afterlistening Pretest 2: Affective tone of the ad. Asecondpretest tothepiece,participantswereaskedtoreportontheiraffec- was conducted to identifytwo advertisements inwhich the tive state using the Affect Grid (Russell, Weiss, & content wouldvaryinterms of their affective tone. One ad Mendelsohn,1989),whichisagraphicalscalethatassesses shouldhaveaclearpositiveaffectivetone,whereastheother bothfeelingsofpleasureandarousalona9-pointscalerang- shouldhaveamoreambiguousaffectivetone.Awiderange ingfrom1(low)to9(high).Asexpected,thepleasantmusic ofadswereinitiallyscreenedbeforesettlingonthefollowing inducedhigherpleasure (M =7.10)thandidthe unpleasant approachfortheformalpretest.Adsforafictitiousinsurance music(M =3.0), t(18)=10.83,p< .001.However,thetwo companywereprofessionallyproduced.Acentralfeatureof piecesinducedsimilarlevelsofarousal(M =7.60and pleasant theadwasa picture,whichvariedacrossconditions.In the M = 7.10), t(18) = 1.41, p >.10. Therefore, the two unpleasant positive-affective-tone condition, the picture depicted a piecesinduceddifferentfeelingsofpleasantnessbutnotdif- happyfamily: a smilingfatherwith a smiling motheranda ferent levels of arousal. MOOD VALENCE, AROUSAL, AND AFFECTIVE TONE 47 Ad evaluation. Adevaluationsacrossconditionswere It is possible that mood-congruency effects would have analyzed in a series of planned contrasts (Rosenthal & beenobservedintheunambiguous-adconditionhadweuseda Rosnow,1985).Afirstcontrastshowedthatintheambigu- stronger mood manipulation. However, our manipulation (a ous-adcondition,participantsinapleasantaffectivestatein- 4.1difference on a 7-point scale; w2= .85)wasquite strong deedreportedmorefavorableevaluations(M=3.66)thandid compared to the mood manipulations used in other studies participantsinanunpleasantaffectivestate(M=3.16),t(76) where moodcongruencywas indeedobserved(e.g., Gorn et =1.65,p=.05,onetailed(w2=.02).Thiseffectreplicatesthe al.,1993;Schwarz&Clore,1983).3Thissuggeststhat,inreal standard mood-congruency effect. In contrast, in the posi- consumersettings,mood-congruencyeffectsonadevaluations tive-adcondition,thevalenceofparticipants’affectivestates maybelesspervasiveandpronouncedthanimpliedbyprevi- hadnoeffectsonadevaluations,t<1.Participantsinthiscon- ousresearch(e.g.,Goldberg&Gorn,1987). ditionreportedcomparableevaluationsregardlessofwhether Giventhatvalenceappearstocoloradevaluationonlywhen theywereinapleasant(M=3.64)orunpleasant(M=3.84)af- theadisaffectivelyambiguous,thequestionariseswhethercon- fective state. Consistent with the planned contrasts, which sumers’affectivestateshaveanyinfluenceatallonadevalua- were the focus of the analyses, a 2 (valence) × 2 (ad tone) tions when the ad is not affectively ambiguous (i.e., clearly ANOVA revealed a marginally significant Ad Tone × Va- positiveorclearlynegative).ThisissueisexaminedinStudy2. lenceinteraction,F(1,76)=2.66,p=.10(w2=.02).More- over, as expected, both the main effect of valence and the maineffectofadtonewerenotsignificant,F<1andF(1,76) STUDY 2 =2.29,p>.13,respectively,suggestingthatvalencedidnot coloradevaluationsindependentlyofthead’saffectivetone. Themainobjectiveof thisstudywastotesttheproposition Overall, the results are consistent withourhypothesis (H1) that when ad targets have a clear affective tone, it is the thatthecoloringeffectofvalencedependsonwhetherthetar- arousalcomponentofconsumers’affectivestates,andnotthe get ad has a clear affective tone.2 valencecomponent,thatwillaffecthowtheadwillbeevalu- ated. Participants whose affective state was manipulated in termsofbothvalenceandarousalweresubsequentlyexposed Discussion toatargetadthatwaseitherclearlypositive initsaffective tone or clearly negative. It was predicted that high arousal TheresultsareconsistentwithH1andwiththegeneralnotion wouldpolarizeadevaluationsinthedirectionofthead’saf- that ad evaluations may be influenced by an interplay be- fectivetone.Incontrast,assuggestedbytheresultsofStudy tweenthead’saffectivetoneandconsumers’affectivestates. 1,valencewouldhavelittleinfluenceonhowtheunambigu- Whenthetargetad’saffectivetonewasambiguous,adevalu- ously positive or negative ad would be evaluated. ationwasmorefavorableamongparticipantsinapleasantaf- Should a polarization effect of arousal be uncovered, a fectivestatethanamongparticipantsinanunpleasantaffec- secondobjectiveofthisstudywastoexplorealternativeex- tive states. Thiscoloringeffect of valence on adevaluation planationsofthiseffect.Accordingtotheexcitationtransfer replicatesthewell-establishedmoodcongruencyeffect.This hypothesis (e.g., Cantor et al., 1975; Zillmann, 1971), high coloringeffectdidnotoccur,however,whenthead’saffec- arousalshouldpolarizesubsequentadevaluationsifconsum- tive tone was clearly positive. Although mood-congruency ersbelievethatthecontext-inducedarousalreflectstheirgen- effects on evaluation have been considered to be pervasive uineaffectivereactionstotheaditself.Consumersinspecting androbust(e.g.,Mayeretal.,1992;Schwarz,1990),theyap- “how they feel” about anad(e.g., Pham, 1998; Schwarz & pear to be sensitive to the affective ambiguity of the target Clore, 1988) may infer from their arousal that they “feel (e.g.,Isen&Shalker,1982;Miniardetal.,1992).Thiscontin- strongly”abouttheadandhenceevaluateitmoreextremely. gencyisnoteworthygiventhatmanyadvertisementsarenot We examined this explanation by comparing responses to affectively ambiguous. In this study, affective ambigu- ad-evaluationitemsphrasedinself-referentialmanner(e.g., ity—andtherebythecoloringeffectofvalence—wasreduced “Ilikethead”and“Ireactfavorablytothead”)toresponsesto by the mere modification of an execution cue. items phrased in object-referent manner (e.g., “The ad is good” and “The ad is enjoyable”). It was reasoned that self-referential items should be particularly sensitive to a transferofexcitationbecausetheseitemsemphasizepartici- 2Alternatively,onecouldinterpretthedataasshowingthattheaffective toneoftheadhadastrongereffectonadevaluationsamongparticipantsinan unpleasantmoodthanamongparticipantsinapleasantmood.Thisinterpre- tationwouldbeconsistentwithpreviousargumentsthatnegativemoodin- creasesanalyticalprocessingandmessagescrutiny,whereaspositivemood 3InGorn,Goldberg,andBasu(1993),themoodmanipulationinduced decreasesit(e.g.,Schwarz,Bless,&Bohner,1991).Thisinterpretationdoes differencesof2.38(w2=.29)and3.16(w2=.22)onthe10-pointscalesthat notseemtobeapplicabletothisstudy.Theadsdifferedonlyonasingleexe- wereusedtoassessmood.InSchwarzandClore(1983;Study1),themood cutioncharacteristicthatwassimpletoprocess.Therefore,depthofmessage manipulationinducedadifferenceof1.8ona7-pointscale(w2=.22)anda scrutiny should have made little difference. difference of 2.6 on an 11-point scale (w2= .25). 48 GORN, PHAM, SIN pants’ reactions tothe ad. In contrast, object-referent items pieceswasplayed(seePretest3next).Participantswerein- should be less sensitive to a transfer of excitation because structedtolistencarefullyandformimpressionsofthemusic. they emphasize the ad itself (for a related discussion, see AsinStudy1,5minfollowingthemusiconset,participants Wyer,Clore,&Isbell,1999).Inotherwords,ifthepolariza- completedaquestionnaire.Thequestionnaireincluded(a)a tioneffectofarousalisindeedmediatedbyatransferofexci- 22-item scale assessing perceptions of the music (e.g., tation,thiseffectshouldbemorepronouncedonself-referent fast–slow, quiet–loud, and simple–complex); (b) a 3-item items (e.g., “I like–dislike the ad”) than on object-referent scalemeasuringevaluationsofthemusic(e.g.,like–dislike); items(e.g.,“Theadisgood–bad”).Thedynamiccomplexity and(c)theAffectGrid,whichservedasamanipulationcheck hypothesiswouldnotpredictthatpolarizationwoulddepend of participant’s states of pleasure and arousal. Participants on the phrasing of the items. were then directed to the “second” study. According to the dynamic complexity hypothesis The“second”studywasagainintroducedasapilotadvertis- (Paulhus & Lim, 1994), high arousal may polarize subse- ing test conducted by the marketing department for a private quentadevaluationsifconsumersprocessasimplerrepresen- company.Eachparticipantwasexposedfor40sectooneoftwo tationoftheadandiftheprimarycomponentofthesimplified ads.Oneadhadaclearpositiveaffectivetoneandtheotherhada representationisevaluative.Participants’representationsof clear negative affective tone (see Pretest 4 next). Participants thetargetadwereassessedthroughcognitiveresponses.The then completed a questionnaire, which assessed their evalua- dynamic complexity hypothesis would predict that aroused tionsoftheadandtheircognitiveresponseswhilereadingthead. consumersshouldreport(a)fewerthoughtsinresponsetothe Attheendofthesession,participantswereorallyaskedwhatthe adand(b)ahigherproportionofevaluativetononevaluative purposeofthestudywasandtowritedowntheiranswersona thoughts. The excitation transfer explanation does not hy- separatesheetofpaper.Theywerethendebriefed. pothesizeanyspecificchangeincognitiverespondingunder high arousal. Measures. The main dependent measure, ad evalua- tion,wasassessedonseven7-pointsemanticallydifferential Method items(a=.88):“Theadispleasant–unpleasant,”“Theadis good–bad,”“Theadisenjoyable,”“Ilike–dislikethead,”“I Participants and design. Participants in this study reactfavorably–unfavorablytothead,”“Ifeelnegative–posi- consistedof128businessundergraduateswhoparticipatedto tivetowardthead,”“Theadisfun–notfuntoread.”Partici- earn course credit. They were randomly assigned to one of pantswerealsoaskedtolistallthethoughtsandfeelingsthat eightconditionsofa2(arousal)×2(valence)×2(adaffective cametomindwhilereadingthead.Twojudges,workingin- tone)between-subjectsdesign.UnlikeinStudy1,theaffec- dependently,classifiedeachthoughtorfeelingaseitherposi- tivetoneoftheadwaseitherclearlypositiveorclearlynega- tive,negative,orneutral(agreement=95%).Disagreements tive.Furthermore,valenceofaffectivestate(pleasantvs.un- were resolved by a third judge. pleasant) was crossed with two levels of arousal (high vs. low). Crossingthese two factors presents three advantages. First,itreducesthelikelihoodthateithermanipulationiscon- Pretests 1 and 2: Valence and arousal of affective founded with the other. One could argue, for instance, that states. Weagainusedmusictomanipulatebothvalenceand states of high arousal are less pleasant than states of lower arousalofaffectivestates.Althoughmusichasoftenbeenused arousal.Second,itallowsustotestthenullpredictionthatva- to manipulate affect valence, the use of music to manipulate lenceoftheaffectivestatewillnothaveaneffectwhentheaf- arousalorthogonallytovalenceisauniquefeatureofthisstudy. fectivetoneoftheadisunambiguouslypositiveornegative, Inafirst,within-subjectpretest,22participants,ingroups andtotestitacrossbothhigherandlowerlevelsofarousal. of4to6,wereaskedtolistento16piecesofmusic(asubsetof Third,previousresearchonarousaleffectsonpersuasionhas the48piecespretestedinStudy1),onepieceatatimefor2 ignored potential interactions between arousal and valence min.Afterlisteningtoeachpiece,participantsreportedtheir (Pham, 1996; Sanbonmatsu & Kardes, 1988). Crossing the mood states using the Affect Grid. Based on this pretest, 4 twofactorsallowsus toexaminesuchinteractions.In addi- musical pieces were selected. They were: (a) “Eine Kleine tiontothesebetween-subjectsfactors,awithin-subjectsfac- Nachtmusik:Allegro”byMozart,whichinducedhighplea- torwasformedbytheinclusionoftwotypesofad-evaluation sure (M = 7.23) and high arousal (M = 7.36); (b) items (self-referential vs. object referential). “Saraseeruhasana” by Naada Loludai, which induced low pleasure(M=3.14)andhigharousal(M=6.73);(c)“What- Procedure. The procedureclosely followedthat used everWeImage”byDavidFoster,whichinducedhighplea- inStudy1.Thesessionswereagain runoneparticipantata sure (M= 6.91)andlowarousal(M = 4.14); and(d)“Raga time.Thestudywasintroducedastwounrelatedstudies,con- Bhopali” by Mohanam, which induced low pleasure (M = ductedindifferentrooms.The“first”studywaspurportedly 3.73) and low arousal (M= 3.50). about musical appreciation. Participants were seated in a Inasecond,between-subjectspretest(N=31),eachofthe comfortableloungechairwhileoneoffourselectedmusical fourselectedpieceswasplayedtoaseparategroupofpartici- MOOD VALENCE, AROUSAL, AND AFFECTIVE TONE 49 pantsinasoundproofroom.Afterlisteningtothepiecefor5 generalpurpose.Theirdatawerediscardedfromtheanalysis, min,participantsreportedtheirmoodsusingtheAffectGrid. performedon125observations.Toassessthemanipulations Theseself-reportsofpleasureandarousalweresubmittedto2 of affective states, participants’ self-ratings of arousal and × 2 ANOVAs in which the two factors were the level of pleasureafterlisteningtothepieceofmusicwereeachsub- arousal(highorlow)andvalence(pleasantorunpleasant)ex- mittedtoa2(affectdimension)×2(valencemanipulation)× pectedtobeinducedbyeachpiece.Asexpected,bothfactors 2 (arousal manipulation)mixedANOVA. As expected, the had strong main effects on the corresponding self-reports. analysisuncoveredastrongAffectDimension×ArousalMa- Self-reportsofpleasureweregreaterwhenthemusicalpieces nipulation interaction, F(1, 119) = 153.07, p < .0001 (w2= werepleasant(M=6.43)thanwhentheywereunpleasant(M .55),aswellasastrongAffectDimension×ValenceManipu- = 3.60), F(1, 27)= 28. 83, p < .0001 (w2= .44). Similarly, lationinteraction,F(119)=99.37,p<.0001(w2=.44).Fol- self-reportsofarousalweregreaterwhenthemusicalpieces low-upanalysesshowthatthearousalratingswerestrongly were highly arousing (M = 6.70) than when they were less influencedbythearousalmanipulation(M=3.76vs.6.97), arousing (M = 4.39), F(1, 27) = 6.92, p < .02 (w2 = .16). F(1,119)=182.06,p<.0001(w2=.58),andtheyweremuch Self-reportsofpleasurealsoexhibitedasmallbutsignificant lessinfluenced,althoughsignificantlyso,bythevalencema- Arousal×Valenceinteraction,F(1,27)=7.33,p<.02(w2= nipulation,F(1,119)=5.51,p<.03(w2=.01).Similarly,the .06), showing that the valence manipulation was slightly pleasureratingsweremostlyaffectedbythevalencemanipu- strongerforthemorearousingpieces.Noothereffectswere lation(M=3.61vs.7.01),F(1,119)=358.65,p<.0001,(w2= significant,allps>.20,suggestingthatthefourpiecesmanip- .67).TheywerealsoaffectedbyasmallValence×Arousal ulatedpleasureandarousallargelyindependently.Thisma- StateManipulationinteraction,F(1,119)=5.31,p<.05(w2= nipulation was further assessed in the main experiment. .01).Insummary,thefourmusicalpiecessucceededinma- nipulatingpleasureandarousalstateslargelyindependently, Pretests 3 and 4: Affective tone. The purpose of although not perfectly orthogonally. thesepretestswastoidentifytwoadvertisementswithaclearly positiveor negativeaffective tone.In Pretest 3, 64 participants Effects on ad evaluation. Ad evaluations were sub- wereshownoneofeightprintadvertisements.AsinStudy1,the mittedtoa2(arousal)×2(pleasure–valence)×2(affectivetone) adswereforafictitiousinsurancecompanyandwereprofession- between-subjectsANOVA.Themeansacrossconditionsarere- allyproducedtoinducedifferentlevelsofaffectivetone.Thepos- portedinTable1.Theanalysisrevealedamaineffectofaffec- itive-affectivetoneadwasthesameastheoneusedinStudy1.It featuredthepictureofahappyfamily,thecopystressedthebene- tivetone,F(1,117)=79.90,p<.0001(w2=.39).Evaluations fitsoftheinsurancecompany,andtheheadlineread“YOUCAN weremorefavorableforthepositive-affectivetoneversionof ENJOYMOREFROMLIFE!”Thegoalinconstructinganega- thead(M=4.17)thanforthenegativeaffectivetoneversion(M tiveadwastodifferentiateitsaffectivetoneasmuchaspossible =2.89).Asexpected,giventheaffectiveunambiguityoftheads, fromthatofthepositivead.Thenegativeaffectivetoneadfea- the valence of participants’ affective states did not have any tured the picture of a distressed-looking girl described as a mainorinteractioneffectontheiradevaluations,F<1.Partici- 5-year-oldorphanwhoseparenthaddiedinacaraccident.The pantswhowereinapleasantaffectivestatewerenotmorefavor- headlineoftheadread“SHECOULDHAVELIVEDBETTER! able intheiradevaluations(M = 3.49)thanwereparticipants PROTECTYOURCHILDRENANDTHEIRFUTURE.”The whowereinanunpleasantaffectivestate(M=3.58).Thisresult affectivetoneoftheadswasassessedbyfour7-pointsemantic isconsistentwiththeresultsofStudy1. Moreimportant,therewasasignificantArousal×Affec- differentialitems(a= .95):“Thisadmademe feel happy–un- tiveToneinteraction,F(1,117)=4.22,p<.05(w2=.03).As happy,” “This ad made me feel pleased–displeased,” “This ad predicted, the simple effect of the ad’s affective tone was mademefeeldelighted–distressed,”“Thisadmademefeeljoy- greater under high arousal (M = 4.41 vs. M = ful–depressed.”Asdesired,thetwoadsdifferedinaffectivetone positive negative 2.85),F(1,117)=58.63,p<.0001(w2=.32)thanunderlow (M=2.69vs.M=5.27),t(16)=5.58,p<.01.Toensurethatthe arousal (M = 3.94 vs. M = 3.02), F(1, 117) = twoads were equal in terms of overall quality, 20 participants positive negative 24.29, p<.0001(w2=.16).Thispatternofresultsindicates (Pretest4)evaluatedtheadsafterlisteningfor5mintoapieceof thathigharousalpolarizedadevaluationsinthedirectionof music(selectedonthebasisofthepreliminarypretestreportedin thead’saffectivetone,whichsupportsH2.4Noothereffects Experiment1)designedtoinduceaneutralmood.Asexpected, thetwoversions of the adwere evaluated similarly(M = positive 5.02andM =4.88),t<1. negative 4Wealsotestedthesimpleeffectsofarousalwithineachlevelofaffective Results toneofthead.Consistentwiththepolarizationhypothesis,inthepositive-af- fectivetonecondition,adevaluationsweresignificantlymorefavorableunder higharousalthanlowarousal,F(1,117)=5.24,p<.05.Inthenegativeadtone Demand and manipulation checks. Three partici- condition,adevaluationsweresomewhatlessfavorableunderhigharousal pants expressed at least some understanding of the study’s thanunderlowarousal.However,thedifferencewasnotsignificant,F<1. 50 GORN, PHAM, SIN TABLE1 identicaltotalnumberofthoughts(M =3.21vs.M higharousal low AdEvaluationasaFunctionofValence,Arousal, =3.16),F<1.Asimilaranalysiswasthendoneonthe andAffectiveTone arousal proportionofvalencedthoughts(i.e.,[positive+negative]/ PositiveAffectiveTone NegativeAffectiveTone total).Itrevealedamaineffectofaffectivetone,F(1,115)= 13.39,p<.001(w2=.09).Thenegativelytonedversionofthe Pleasant Unpleasant Pleasant Unpleasant Affective Affective Affective Affective adpolarizedparticipants’thoughtsmorestrongly(M=86%) Arousal State State State State than did the positively toned version (M = 66%). This was mostlyby increasingthenumberof negative thoughts, F(1, Low 3.89a 4.02a 2.95a 2.98a 115)=5.76,p<.02(w2=.04),anddecreasingthenumberof High 4.23a 4.56b 2.85b 2.78b neutral thoughts, F(1, 115) = 17.70, p < .001 (w2 = .12). an= 16.bn= 15. Arousal itself did not increase the proportion of valenced thoughtsaswouldbe predictedby the dynamic complexity weresignificant.Thetwodifferentexplanationsofthispolar- hypothesis, F< 1. ization effect are examined next. Furtheranalysisshowedthatarousaldidinfluencepartici- Recallthat,accordingtotheexcitationtransferexplanation, pants’cognitiveresponses—albeitnotinthemannerpredicted preexistingarousalpolarizesevaluationsbecausethisarousal bythedynamiccomplexityhypothesis.Wecomputedthenet ismisattributedasbeingpartofagenuineaffectiveresponseto valenceofparticipants’thoughtsbysubtractingthenumberof thetarget.Shouldthisexplanationbecorrect,thepolarization negative thoughts from the number of positive thoughts. A effectofarousalshouldbemorepronouncedonitemsstressing three-wayANOVAonthisnetvalenceindexrevealedamain the participants’ reactions to the ad (e.g., “I like–dislike the effectoftheaffectivetoneofthead,F(1,117)=4.97,p<.03 ad”)thanonitemsstressingtheaditself(e.g.,“Theadisenjoy- (w2 = .03). Expectedly, the net valence of participants’ able–notenjoyable”).Wetestedthisreasoningbyexamining thoughtswasmorenegativeinthenegative-adcondition(M= theArousal×Toneinteractionforeachevaluationitem,sepa- –1.45)thaninthepositive-adcondition(M=–.70).Moreim- rately.As canbe seeninTable 2, the polarization effect ap- portant,therewasasignificantArousal×AffectiveToneinter- peared to be consistently more pronounced for the items action,F(1,117)=4.58;p<.04(w2=.03).Although,under phrasedinaself-referentialformatthanfortheitemsphrasedin lowarousal,theaffectivetoneoftheadhadnoinfluenceonthe a object-referent manner. A post hoc contrast of the pooled net valence of participants’ thoughts (M = –1.31 vs. positive self-referential items and the pooled object-referent items M =–1.34),F<1,underhigharousal,thenetvalenceof negative showsthatthesimpletwo-wayinteractionbetweenArousal× the thoughts was significantly more negative among partici- Tone was significantly greater for the self-referential items, pantsexposedtothenegativetonead(M=–1.57)thanamong F(1,117)=5.71,p<.02(w2=.04)thanfortheobject-referent participantsexposedtothepositivetone ad(M =–.06), F(1, items,F(1,117)=1.90,p=.17(w2=.01).Thiscontrastwasre- 117) = 9.32, p < .01 (w2 = .06). Therefore, the influence of flectedinasignificantthree-wayArousal×AffectiveTone× arousalonthenetvalenceofparticipants’thoughtslargelypar- TypeofReferenceinteraction,F(1,117)=4.47,p<.04(w2= alleleditseffectonadevaluation.5 .03),whichisdepictedinFigure1.Thispatternoffindingsis consistentwithanexcitationtransferexplanation.Totestthe Discussion otherprocessexplanation,wealsoexaminedparticipants’cog- nitiveresponses. Theresultsofthisstudyclearlysupportthehypothesis(H2) that when an ad has an unambiguous affective tone, the Effects on cognitive responses. Table 3 summa- arousal component of consumers’affectivestatesmaypo- rizestheeffectsofarousalandaffectivetoneonparticipants’ larize ad evaluations in the direction of the ad’s affective cognitiveresponses.Themeansarecollapsedacrosstheva- tone.Itwasfoundthatparticipantsevaluatedtheadmorefa- lencefactor,whichdidnothaveanysignificantinfluenceon vorably when it hada positive-affective tone than whenit theseresponses.Asdiscussedearlier,thedynamiccomplex- hadanegative-affective tone.However,thistendencywas ity hypothesis postulates that high arousal narrows percep- significantlystrongerinthehigh-arousal conditionthan in tionstoaprimarydimensionthatisevaluative.Asimplifica- thelow-arousalcondition. tion would imply fewer ad-related thoughts, whereas Althoughexploratory,theresultsalsoshedsomelighton narrowingtoanevaluativedimensionwouldimplyagreater theprocessunderlyingthispolarizationeffect.Thereseemed proportionofvalencedtononvalencedthoughts.Thesecorre- tobelittleevidencethatthiseffectwascausedbythetypeof lated predictions were tested as follows. Participants’ total evaluative attention narrowing postulated by the dynamic number of thoughts were submitted to a 2 (arousal) × (va- complexity hypothesis. Although null effects should, of lence)×2(adtone)ANOVA.Theanalysisdidnotrevealany significanteffectofthesefactorsonthetotalnumberofcog- nitiveresponses(smallestp=.19).Inparticular,participants 5A mediation analysis confirmed that the valence of participants’ in the high- and low-arousal conditions reported an almost thoughts mediated the polarization effect of arousal on ad evaluation. MOOD VALENCE, AROUSAL, AND AFFECTIVE TONE 51 TABLE2 Theevidencewasmorecongenialwiththeexcitationtrans- Arousal×AdToneInteractiononEachAdEvaluationItem fer hypothesis. It was found that the polarization effect of Items(IntheOrderTheyWereAssessed) FValue pValue arousalwasstrongerwhentheadevaluationitemsemphasized participants’reactionstothead(“I___thead”)thanwhenthe The ad is pleasant–unpleasanta 0.21 .65 itemsfocusedontheaditself(“Theadis___”).Thispatternof The ad is bad–gooda 2.78 .10 I like–dislike the adb 3.80 .05 resultsisconsistentwiththemisattributionmechanismpostu- The ad is enjoyable–not enjoyablea 0.79 .38 latedbyexcitationtransfer.Consumerswhosestateofarousal I react favorably–unfavorably to the adb 4.97 .03 hasbeenheightenedbyacontextualcause(e.g.,theprogram- I feel negative–positiveb 4.45 .04 ming context) and who are then exposed to an affectively The ad is fun–not fun to reada 0.29 .59 valencedadmayexperienceintensifiedaffectiveresponsesto aObject-referentialitems(beginningwith“Thead…”).bSelf-referential the ad. They may infer from these intensified affective re- items (beginning with “I … ”). sponsesthatthey“feelstrongly”aboutthead(eitherpositively or negatively)andtherefore evaluate itmore extremely.The differentialinfluenceofarousalandadtoneonself-referential versusobject-referentitemsrulesoutthepossibilitythathighly arousedparticipantsweresimplymorealertduringadevalua- tion.Itwasalsofoundthatarousedparticipantshadpolarized thoughts and feelings in response to the ad and that these thoughtsandfeelingsmediatedtheextremityoftheirevalua- tions. This finding suggests that the polarization effect of arousalwasnotamereresponsescalingeffect,butagenuine changeinparticipants’subjectiverespondingtothead. Although the results appear to favor excitation transfer overdynamiccomplexity,itisneverthelesspossiblethatour analysesmaynothavebeenequallysensitivetothetwotypes ofprocesses.Forinstance,theevidenceforexcitationtransfer comesprimarilyfromad-evaluationscaleresponses.Incon- FIGURE 1 Study 2: Arousal × Tone × Reference interaction. trast,theevidence(orlackofit)fordynamiccomplexityco- mesfromopen-endedthoughtlistings,whicharelikelytobe less sensitive thanclose-ended ratings. It therefore remains TABLE3 possiblethatthepolarizationeffectofarousalwascausedby CognitiveResponsesasaFunctionofArousal andAffectiveTone bothmisattributionandattention-narrowingprocesses(e.g., Easterbrook, 1959) of the type postulated by the dynamic PositiveAffective NegativeAffective complexityhypothesis.AselaborateintheGeneralDiscus- Tone Tone sionsection,itisalsopossiblethatthetwoexplanationsapply Low High Low High to different ranges of the arousal continuum. Arousal Arousal Arousal Arousal Finally,theresultsconvergedwiththoseofStudy1insug- gestingthatwhenthetargetadhasaclearaffectivetone,the Positive thoughts 0.44 1.10 0.53 0.63 coloringeffectofvalenceislesslikelytobeobserved.This Negative thoughts 1.75 1.16 1.88 2.20 Neutral thoughts 1.25 1.00 0.47 0.33 lackofavalenceeffectcannotbeattributedtoaweakmanip- Total 3.44 3.26 2.88 3.17 ulation.Themanipulationcheckindicatesthat,asinStudy1, Polarizedthoughts(%)a .63 .68 .85 .87 themanipulationofvalencewasstrong—a3.4differenceona Net valenceb –1.31 –0.06 –1.34 –1.57 9-pointscale(w2=.67).Itsstrengthwas,infact,comparable a(positive + negative)/total.b(positive – negative). to that of the arousal manipulation (w2= .58). course,beinterpretedwithcaution,higharousaldidnotap- GENERAL DISCUSSION peartoreducethenumberofthoughtsparticipantshadinre- sponsetothead,anditdidnotincreasetheproportionofthese Summary thoughtsthatwerevalenced.Alongwiththefindingthathigh arousal increased (rather than decreased) the effects of the Adexposuresdonotoccurinavacuum.Foravarietyofrea- targetad’stone,theabsenceofamaineffectofarousalonthe sons,consumersexposedtoadvertisementsmaybeinvarious number of ad-related thoughts rules out the argument that affectivestates(e.g.,theyhavebeenrelaxingonthebeachvs. highly aroused participants were more distracted during ad ridingacrowdedsubwayafterabusydayofwork).Itisthere- evaluation than less aroused participants were. foreimportanttounderstandhowtheseaffectivestatesmay 52 GORN, PHAM, SIN influenceadvertisingresponses,includingadevaluations.Of It is noteworthy that the arousal manipulation used in course, this issue is not new. Several studies have, for in- Study2(a5-minexposuretoamusicpiece)wasrelativelyin- stance,examinedhowhigharousalmayinfluencepersuasion nocuous—significantlymilderthanthoseusedinearlierre- (e.g.,Pham,1996; Sanbonmatsu&Kardes,1988).An even searchon arousal effects.Thatsuchamildmanipulationof greaternumberofstudieshaveexaminedhowthevalenceof arousalhadasignificant influenceonadvertisingresponses affectivestates(i.e.,goodvs.badmoods)influenceadvertis- suggeststhatsucheffectsmaybepervasiveinrealworldset- ingresponses(e.g.,Batra&Stayman,1990;Gardner&Wil- tings.Themildnessofourarousalmanipulationhastheoreti- helm, 1987; Goldberg & Gorn, 1987; Schwarz, Bless, & cal significance as well. In retrospect, it may be precisely Bohner,1991;Yi,1990).Yetcomparedtopreviousstudies, becausethismanipulationwasrelativelymildthatwefound this research is unique in two important respects. greatersupportfortheexcitationtransferhypothesisthanfor First,bymanipulatingarousalandvalenceindependently, thedynamic complexityhypothesis.Ourmanipulationmay Study2offersthedistinctadvantageofseparatingtheeffects havebeentoomildtoinducethekindofprocessinginterfer- ofthesetwodimensionsofaffectivestates.Previousstudies encepositedbythedynamiccomplexityhypothesis.Incom- have generally focused on one dimension and ignored the parison,studiesthathaveshownsuchinterferenceshaveused other.Itisthereforepossiblethatpartoftheresultsattributed strongermanipulations,suchascaffeine(e.g.,Humphreys& to valence may reflect arousal, and vice versa.6 Revelle, 1984), physical exercise (e.g., Pham, 1996; Theseconddistinguishingfeatureofthisresearchliesinits Sanbonmatsu & Kardes, 1988), and loud white noise (e.g., investigationofanimportantcontingencyoftheeffectsofaf- Paulhus & Kim, 1994). fective states on ad evaluations. The results indicate that, Whereas dynamic complexity may require fairly high whereasvalenceofaffectivestatesmaycoloradevaluationsin arousal,interestingly,excitationtransfermaybemorelikely amood-congruentdirectionwhentheadisambiguous(Study undermoremoderatearousal.Thisisbecausewhenarousalis 1),thiscoloringeffectislesslikelywhentheadhasacleareraf- very intense (e.g., immediately after a dramatic emotional fectivetone(Studies1and2).Ourfindingsechotheresultsof event),itsactualsourceislikelytobeverysalient.Asaresult, researchinothersettingsinwhichmoodcongruencyeffectson intense arousal is less likelyto be misattributedtoa subse- evaluationaremitigatedwheneverthetargethasaclearintrin- quenttarget(e.g.,Cantoretal.,1975).Therefore,itispossible sicvalence(Isen&Shalker,1982;Miniardetal.,1992).They that bothmoderate andveryhigh levels of arousal produce suggestthatmoodcongruencyeffectsmaynotbeaspervasive polarization—theformerviaexcitationtransfermechanisms, androbustaspreviouslythought(Mayeretal.,1992). andthelatterviadynamiccomplexitymechanisms.Thisis- Interestingly,itispreciselywhenthecoloringeffectofva- sueisworthyoffurtherinvestigation,usingabroaderrange lence is less likely that the polarization effect of arousal be- of arousal levels. comesmorelikely.ItwasfoundinStudy2that,whenthead’s affectivetonewasclearlypositiveornegative,higharousalpo- Assimilation–Contrast, Elaboration larizedadevaluationsinthedirectionofthead’saffectivetone. Likelihood, or Affect Infusion? Thissuggeststhatpreexistingarousalstateswhileexposedto anadmayinteractwiththeaffectivetoneoftheaditself.Al- Itisinstructivetorelateourfindingstootherpotentialexplana- thoughbothdynamiccomplexityandexcitationtransfermech- tionsofaffectiveinfluencesonevaluation.Thecoloringeffect anismscouldhavebeenatwork,ourresultsappeartosupport of valence can be related to several theoretical frameworks. thelatterexplanation.Consumersmaymisattributetheirpre- Withinanassimilation–contrastframework, one wouldposit existingarousalasbeingpartoftheiraffectiveresponsetothe that,duringadexposure,thevalenceofthepreexistingmood, aditself.Arousedconsumersmayreportmoreextremeevalua- along with mood-congruent cognition, becomes integrated tionsbecausethey“feelstrongly”aboutthead. withtheconsumer’srepresentationofthetargetad—thereby resultingintheevaluationoftheadbeingassimilatedtoward the valence of the preexisting mood(e.g., Schwarz & Bless, 1992; Strack, Schwarz, & Gschneidinger, 1985). Within the 6Apotentiallimitationofourstudiesisthatweuseddifferenttypesofmu- sic(Indianvs.Western)tomanipulatemoodvalence.Onecouldarguethat Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) framework (Petty & thetwotypesofmusicprimeddifferenttypesofassociations,whichthen Cacioppo,1986),onewouldpositthatthevalenceofthepreex- taintedsubsequentadevaluations.Thisexplanationwouldnotaccountfor istingmoodservesasaperipheralcueinevaluatingthead(e.g., ourresults.First,ourcognitiveresponsesinStudy2gavenoindicationofany Batra & Stayman, 1990; Petty, Schumann, Richman, & imagerythatcouldbetracedbacktothemusicthatparticipantsheardinthe Strathman,1993).WithintheAIM(Forgas,1995),onewould previoustask.Second,adifferencebetweenWesternandIndianmusiccould atbestexplainmaineffectsandnotinteractions.Finally,themainresultof positthatthevalenceofthepreexistingmoodandmood-con- Study2pertainstheArousal×AdToneinteraction.BothIndianandWestern gruentcognitionsthatitprimesareusedasadditionalinputsin musicwereusedforeachlevelofarousal.Weacknowledge,however,that constructiveprocessesofadevaluation. keepingthetypeofmusicconstantacrossconditionswouldbedesirablede- Notethatthesethreeexplanationarenotmutuallyexclu- signproperty.Aninterestingpossibilitywouldbetomanipulatemoodva- sive. They do differ, however, intheir ability tohandle the lenceandarousalbyvaryingthetonalityandtempoofasinglepieceofmusic that remains constant in all other respects (Kellaris & Kent, 1993). finding that the coloring effect of valence—whether called

Description:
evaluated the ad on four 7-point items: “I like (dislike) the ad,” “The ad is . may be less pervasive and pronounced than implied by previ- ous research
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.