ebook img

What Is Melodic Accent? PDF

29 Pages·2008·3.79 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview What Is Melodic Accent?

What Is Melodic Accent? Converging Evidence from Musical Practice Author(s): David Huron and Matthew Royal Source: Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, Vol. 13, No. 4 (Summer, 1996), pp. 489-516 Published by: University of California Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40285700 Accessed: 06/10/2010 18:18 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ucal. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal. http://www.jstor.org MusicP erception © 1996 byt he regentso f the Summer1 996, Vol. 13, No. 4, 489-516 universityo f California What is Melodic Accent?C onvergingE videncef rom Musical Practice DAVID HURON ConradG rebelC ollege, Universityo f Waterloo MATTHEW ROYAL Facultyo f Music, Universityo f WesternO ntario The theoretical and experimental literatures pertaining to pitch-related accent are reviewed. From these literatures, eight competing notions of melodic accent are identified. All eight conceptions of melodic accent were investigated through correlational studies of three contrasting samples of music. Statistical correlations were calculated for each accent type with respect to the corresponding metric position or with respect to the syllabic/melismatic status of associated sung text. The results for all three studies are most consistent with a perceptual model of melodic accent developed by Joseph Thomassen (1982). The remaining concep- tions of melodic accent receive little or no empirical support. In addition, this study reveals an endemic use of text-melody displacement in a sample of Gregorian chant- suggesting that the chant melodies were constructed so as to avoid strong rhythmic stresses. accentm ayb e defineda s an increasedp rominencen, oticeability, or saliencea scribedt o a givens ounde vent.C oopera ndM eyer( 1960) characterizeda ccent as "a stimulusw hich is markedf or consciousness" Sucha coustics timulia rep resumedt o be markedt hroughd ynamic,a gogic, harmonic,m elodic,t imbrai,o r other forms of stress.O f the varioust ypes of accent proposed by music theorists, one of the most contentious has been the so-called "melodica ccent."I n this paper,w e review some of the pertinentt heoreticala nd perceptuall iteraturesr elatedt o melodic accent. Using a correlationalm ethod,w e then comparea nd evaluatee ight differ- ent notions of melodica ccentb y analyzingt hrees ampleso f music.T o an- ticipateo ur results,t he analysesw ill show that the musicalp racticei s most Requests for reprints may be sent to David Huron, Conrad Grebel College, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada N2L 3G6. (e-mail: [email protected]) 489 490 David Huron & Matthew Royal consistent with an experimentally derived perceptual model of melodic ac- cent developed by Thomassen (1982). Theoretical Notions of Melodic Accent By melodic accent, theorists mean accent arising from pitch-related ma- nipulations such as changes of pitch height, pitch interval, or pitch con- tour. In the case of pitch height, two hypothetical types of elemental ac- cents can be distinguished: treble accent - in which higher pitches are presumed to be more salient than lower pitches, and bass accent- in which lower pitches are presumed to be more salient. Benward and White (1989) for example, subscribe to the first view- namely, that higher pitches re- ceive greater perceptual weight. This notion of accent is shared by linguists who have studied pitch contours in speech ('t Hart, Collier, & Cohen, 1990). Other theorists have suggested the reverse- that lower pitched sounds tend to be perceived as more stressed (e.g., Parncutt, 1989). A third possibility would regard both extremes of high and low pitch as more salient than mid-registerp itches. We might dub this third view of melodic accent, registrai extreme accent. This view is one of two implied in Lerdahl and Jackendoff 's (1983) notion of phenomenal accent: By phenomenala ccentw e mean any event at the musicals urfacet hat gives emphasist o a momenti n the musicalf low. Includedi n this cat- egory are . . . leaps to relativelyh igh or low notes. . . (p. 17) Also implied in Lerdahl and Jackendoff's definition is the notion that melodic accent is evoked by large pitch movements- what might be dubbed interval size accent. According to this notion, the degree of melodic accent is proportional to the size of the interval movement and is independent of the direction of pitch motion. Along these lines, Graybill (1989) proposed that "a leap to a note, either high or low, will tend to accentuate that note, especially if the immediately surroundingc ontext is stepwise" (p. 22). Again, two variants of this view of melodic accent arise by introducing interval direction as an additional factor. In the first of these views, size-related accents are presumed to arise only for ascending pitch movements (interval ascent accent), whereas in the second view, size-related accents would arise only for descending pitch movements [interval descent accent). Graybill (1989) claimed that ascending leaps are more accented than descending leaps. Rothgeb (1990) has argued more pointedly that interval size accent arises only in the case of ascending motions. In all of the views that link melodic accent to pitch movement, the accent has been presumed to occur at the point of arrival on the second pitch forming the interval. Melodie Accent 491 Yet another view suggests that melodic accent arises from changes of direction in pitch contours. According to this view, it is the "pivot points" in melodic contours that generate the greatest salience. Lester (1986, p. 33) has espoused this view, claiming that pitches that stand at the top or bot- tom of a melodic segment are accented. Graybill has also espoused this notion - although he claims that changes of melodic direction evoke less strong accents than treble or bass accents. By way of summary, we can distinguish at least seven different concep- tions of melodic accent that are either implied or explicit in the music theory literature: (1) treble accent, (2) bass accent, (3) registrai extreme accent, (4) interval size accent, (5) interval ascent accent, (6) interval descent ac- cent, and (7) contour pivot accent. Other conceptions of pitch-related accent arise from tonality-related considerations (e.g., scale degree or harmonic implication). In addition, further types of pitch-related accents are thought to arise from recurring pitch patterns, such as found in sequential motivic writing (e.g., Graybill, 1989). However, these latter forms of accent will not be addressed in this paper. Finally,i t should be noted that some theorists- notably William Caplin- have voiced skepticism regarding the existence of melodic accent. Caplin (1978) suggested that because of the way high pitches are performed, the presumed melodic accent is merely an artifact of increased intensity (i.e., dynamic accent): . . . however,p itch-heighta ccenti s perhapsq uestionableI. n fact, if one left all other variableso ut of consideration,i t would be difficult to assertt hat the highestn ote of a successiono f notes was perceiveda s accented. . . That does not mean, however,t hat the so-calledp itch- height accent is a mere fantasy since, in actual fact, singers show a tendencyt o give highern otes greateri ntensity(, pp. 18-19 [ourt ransla- tion]) Caplin's view is consistent with physiological research in speech intona- tion, which has shown that the pitch of the speaking voice is correlated with gross respiratory energy- and hence with the loudness of the voice ('t Hart et al., 1990). It is possible that pitch-height related accent may be a learned association with dynamic accent. In reviewing the extant theoretical writings, there appears to be little agreement among music theorists regarding the nature of melodic accent. Note that this lack of consensus may itself be suggestive. If melodic accent exists, perhaps its effect is minor compared with dynamic and agogic ac- cents. Alternatively, much of the apparent confusion may originate from the difficulty of discriminating between certain of the above conceptions of melodic accent. For example, if it were the case that all melodic accents 492 David Huron ôt Matthew Royal Fig. 1. Two melodic intervals that illustrate diverging predictions for melodic accent. If melodic accent is attributable to pitch height ("treble accent"), then F5s hould be perceived as more accented than C5. Alternatively, if melodic accent arises because of large upward intervals ("interval ascent accent"), then C5 should be perceived as more accented than F5. arise due to treble accent alone, then most observations would tend to be consistent with interval ascent accent as well. That is, if the higher of two pitches is always more salient, then the second pitch of an ascending inter- val would also always appear to be more salient. Similarly, other possible confounds could exist between registrai extreme accent and either of treble accent or bass accent- or between registrai extreme accent and contour pivot accent. For example, notes corresponding to changes of pitch direc- tion are more likely to coincide with points of registrai extremes. At the same time, there are some subtle differences between these pro- posed notions of melodic accent that can lead to divergent predictions. Hence, it may be possible to establish which of two or more competing conceptions of melodic accent better accounts for listener experience and/ or compositional practice. For example, consider the pitch movements from C4 to C5, and from A4 to F5 (as shown in Figure 1). According to the inter- val ascent view of melodic accent, C5w ould receive a greater accent than F5 because the interval size is larger (P8 versus m6). However, according to the treble accent view, F5 would be predicted to be more salient than C5 because it is higher in absolute pitch. In summary, we might expect a considerable degree of predictive simi- larity between the various proposed accent types. In a correlational study, this will necessarily mean a degree of shared variance. Under these circum- stances, a high correlation may be spurious if another proposed accent type accounts for the same variance. Perceptual Evidence for Melodic Accent Apart from the theoretical accounts of melodic accent, a handful of per- ceptual studies are also pertinent. Squire (1901) carried out early experi- ments concerning the influence of pitch height on perceived accent. Squire manipulated the pitches of a recurring two-tone pattern; she then deter- mined whether her listeners tended to hear the two tones as forming an Melodie Accent 493 iambic (weak-strong)o r trochaic( strong-weak)r hythm.S quire'sl isteners showed considerablev ariabilityi n their responses,a nd Squirec oncluded that these perceptionsa re listener-dependenTt. hat is, her resultsd id not supportt he notion that eitherh ighero r lower pitchest end to be perceived as havingg reaters tress. Woodrow( 1911) carriedo ut mored etailede xperimentsw herei ntensity, duration,a nd pitch were pitted againste ach other in order to determine whichp arameteris moste ffectivei n inducings ubjectiveg roupingW. oodrow found that loudert ones tend to be hearda s initiatinga group (forminga trochaicr hythm),w hereasl ongert ones tend to be hearda s terminatinga group (forminga n iambicr hythm).W oodrowf ound that changeso f pitch do not systematicallyin ducee ithera group-initiatingo r a group-terminat- ing effect. More preciselyW, oodrowf ound that changeso f pitch alone are unablet o evokec onsistentlyt he perceptiono f an iambico r trochaicr hythm. Like Squire,W oodrow'sr esultsa re inconsistentw ith the notions of treble or bass accent.I ndeed,W oodrowe xplicitlyc alledi nto questiont he validity of pitch-heighta ccent. Perceptuarl esearchp ertainingt o pitch-contourm elodica ccenth as been carriedo ut by JosephT homassen( 1982, 1983). Thomassenu ndertooka set of experimentsi n which sequenceso f pitches exhibitingv arious me- lodic contours were embedded in a standardized metrical context. Thomassenc ollected data pertainingt o the perceivedr elative stress for threet argett onesw ithina metricapl assage.B othm usiciana ndn onmusician listeners participatedi n the study. On the basis of the collected data, Thomassend evelopeda predictivem odel of melodica ccent.F igure2 sum- marizest he relationshipb etweenm elodicc ontoura nd melodica ccenti den- tified in Thomassen'ss tudy. The accent values shown in Figure2 range betweenz ero( no accent)a ndo ne (maximuma ccent).A s Thomassen'ms odel is derivedd irectlyf rom perceptuald ata, the model is not easily character- ized analytically.I n the first instance,t he model is sensitiveo nly to the gross melodic contour and ignores intervals ize. In general,T homassen's model assignsm ore stresst o contourp ivot tones- that is, tones coinciding with changeso f direction.N ote, however,t hat ascending-descendinpgi v- ots are more stressedt han descending-ascendinpgi vots. In ordert o test his model of melodica ccent,T homassenu sed the model to predictt he perceiveds tresso f each note in sequenceso f four targett ones within a metricalp assage.T hese predictionsw ere then comparedw ith the resultso f a seconde xperimentasl tudyi n whicht he modelp redictionsw ere confirmed.T he valuesp redictedb y Thomassen'sm odel yieldeda correla- tion of +.90 with listener'sr esponses. As in all experimentsv, ariousa ssumptionsw ere madea nd possiblec on- founds left uncontrolled.I n Thomassen'sw ork, only three intervals izes were usedi n the melodicc ontours( one, four,a nd eight semitonei ntervals). 494 David Huron OcM atthew Royal Fig. 2. Schematic representation of basic accent configurations in Thomassen's model of melodic accent. Accent values pertain to the second and third notes of three-note contours. Thomassen's algorithm uses a moving window containing three pitches. The accent value for any given note is determined by the interaction of three overlapping three-note contours associated with that note. Of course,d ifferenti ntervals izes may influencem elodica ccentb y evoking differenth armonicim plicationsA. s Thomassend id not investigatea broader sampleo f intervals izes, possibleh armonici mplicationsa re not addressed in his model. Indeed,T homassenf ound small but consistent differences betweenc ontoursu singd ifferent-sizedin tervals.D espitet hese smalld evia- tions, the principale xperimentalr esults showed a robust effect of pitch contourt hat is largelyi ndependento f the type or size of intervalsu sed. Another possible confound in Thomassen'sr esults is the influenceo f overall pitch register.T homassen'se xperimentsu sed pure tones centered around 1000 Hz (B5-) somewhat higher than the typical musical pitch tessitura.T hus it is possiblet hat Thomassen'sr esultsa re only applicablet o "soprano"li nesa ndc annotb e generalizedto pitchc ontoursi n the midrange or bass. Two experimentapl aperst hat take Thomassen'sw ork as a springboard and examinet he interactiono f melodica ccentw ith other accentt ypes are Monahan,K endall,a nd Carterette(1 987) and Drake,D owling,a nd Palmer (1991). Monahane t al. playedt hreed ifferentt ypes of speciallyc omposed melodies to listeners.O ne group of melodiesc onsistedo f tones in which agogic and melodica ccentsc oincided.A gogica nd melodica ccenti n a sec- ond group of melodies occurreda t regulari ntervalsb ut never coincided with each other.A thirdg roupe xhibiteda gogic and melodica ccentsu sing differentm etricalr ates. Drake et al. played various melodiest o children Melodie Accent 495 and adult pianistsi n which metric,a gogic, and melodica ccentsw ere sys- tematicallyp ositionedi n and out of synchronyw ith each other.B oth ex- perimentsa ssumedt hat melodica ccenti s inducedb y (1) a contourc hange, and (2) a largep itch interval.B oth Monahane t al. and Drakee t al. found that pianistsp layedb ack the melodiesm ost accuratelyw hen all the accent types coincided and that there was a deteriorationi n performancew hen agogico r melodica ccentsw ere out of synchronyw ith each other.T he dete- riorationo f pianists'p erformancews hen both melodicc ontourc hangea nd melodicl eap were desynchronizedp rovidesp erceptuale videncec onsistent with the existenceo f both of these types of melodica ccent. In summaryt, he extant perceptualr esearchp rovidess tronge videncei n supporto f Thomassen'sm odel,g ood evidencei n supporto f intervala scent and intervals ize accent, but calls into questiont he notions of bass accent and treble accent. However,g iven the avowedly simple stimuli/contexts used (especiallyi n Thomassen'se xperiments)t, herer emainsa questiono f how these findingsr elatet o musicalp ractice. Melodic Accent in Musical Contexts In light of the wide discrepancyo f views concerningt he natureo f me- lodic accent,i t is appropriateto compareb oth the perceptuala nd theoreti- cal literaturesw ith actual musicalp ractice.T hat is, we would propose to measuret he degreet o which the musicalr epertoirep rovidese videncec on- sistentw ith one or moreo f the conceptionso f melodica ccentj ustd escribed. At the outset, it is importantt o recognizet hat accentsm ay be used for a variety of musicalp urposes.O ne such goal may be the establishmento r preservationo f meter.I n otherc ases, the goal may be the exact opposite- that is, to prevento r dismantlea ny senseo f metricf rameworkI. n yet other circumstancest,h e compositionalg oal may be to play off accentsw ithin an existing metric frameworkb ut without actuallyd ismantlingt he meter- such as in the cases of syncopationa nd hemiola.I n these cases, accentsa re purposelym iscoordinated( for a paralleli n speechp erception,s ee Handel, 1989, regardingt he structureo f tongue-twisters)A. ccentsm ay also be used for nonmetricr hythmicp urposes,s uch as adding dramatice mphasiso r highlightingt he naturals yllables tressesi n vocal text-setting. A useful way of evaluatinga ny hypotheticalf orm of accent is to relate its presencet o the presenceo f known or establishedf ormso f phenomenal accent (such as agogic or dynamica ccent).F or example,i n most musical passages,a gogic accentsa nd dynamica ccentst end to coincide with each other,a nd they also show strongp ositivec orrelationsw ith metricp osition (see Huron, 1989). For the combinedv oices in the 48 fuguesi n Bach'sW ell 496 David Huron ôc Matthew Royal TemperedC lavierf, or example,t he correlationb etweenn ote durationsa nd metric position is +.34. (That is, longer notes tend to begin on stronger metricp ositions.)W e would presumet hat this synchronizationo f phenom- enal accentw ith metricp osition is intended,a t least in part, to help estab- lish or maintaint he meter. One can study hypotheticaln otions of melodica ccenti n a similarw ay, by examiningt he coincidenceo f proposedm elodica ccentsw ith points in the metric hierarchyT. he possible relationshipsb etween metric position and melodic accents might be viewed as existing along a continuum.A t one end of the continuum,m elodic accents would be perfectlys ynchro- nizedw ith high points in the metrich ierarchyI.n this case, the most promi- nent melodica ccentsw ould coincidew ith downbeats.A t the othere xtreme, one can imaginem elodic accentsc ompletelyo ut of synchronyw ith high points in the metric hierarchy.F or any given piece, the relationshipb e- tween metricp osition and melodica ccentm ightl ie at any point along this correlationalc ontinuum.T hat is, the correlationb etweenm etricp osition and hypotheticalm elodica ccentm ay rangeb etween+ 1 and -1. In ordert o understando ur researcha pproach,i t is crucialt o consideri n some detail possible interpretationso f variousc orrelationalo utcomes. Below we de- scribe interpretationso f three circumstances:( 1) statisticallys ignificant positive correlationalv alues, (2) statisticallys ignificantn egativec orrela- tional values,a nd (3) statisticallyn onsignificantc orrelationavl alues (near zero). If we observeda serieso f momentsi n a score where a large numbero f known and hypotheticala ccentt ypesc oincided,w e mighti nfer( 1) that the composeri ntendedt o place especiallyg reat stress at these moments,a nd (2) that the hypotheticala ccentt ype actuallyc ontributes,o r is thoughtb y the composert o contribute,t o phenomenala ccent. If we observeda serieso f momentsi n a score where a large numbero f known accentsc oincide,b ut a hypotheticala ccenti s systematicallya bsent fromt hose momentsa nd appearsi nsteada t otherm oments,t hen we might infer that: (1) the composeri ntendedt o avoid especiallys trongs tressesa t these moments,a nd (2) that the hypotheticala ccentt ype actuallyc ontrib- utes, or is thought by the composert o contribute,t o phenomenala ccent. In the first of the above cases, we would expect to observea significant positive correlationb etweent he known and hypotheticala ccent types. In the second of the above cases, we would expect to observe a significant negativec orrelationb etweent he known and hypotheticala ccentt ypes. Of course, a composerm ay change musicalg oals from passaget o pas- sage or from work to work. That is, at one point, the composerm ay wish to avoid undue phenomenala ccent, and at anotherp oint, the composer may wish to create an especiallys trong phenomenala ccent. Where the composerc hangess trategyi n this way, if the data are analyzedi n the ag- gregate,t hen the result is likely to be a nonsignificantc orrelationc oeffi- Melodie Accent 497 cient nearz ero. However,t herea re otherw ays of interpretinga nonsignifi- cant (near-zero)c orrelation.W e can infer no less than four possible inter- pretations(:1 ) the composer'sa ccentg oalsm ayh avec hangedo vert he course of the passage,o r (2) the placemento f these hypotheticala ccentsi s of less or little concern to the composer,o r (3) the known and hypotheticala c- cents are being used for differentp urposesb y the composera nd so appear to be unrelated,( 4) the hypotheticala ccentd oes not exist; that is, the fea- ture in questiond oes not producea phenomenala ccent. In addition, any combinationo f (l)-(3) may resulti n a nonsignificantc orrelation. Study 1: Western Folk Melodies As in the case of agogic and dynamica ccents,w e might expect melodic accent to assist in the maintenanceo r preservationo f the perceptiono f meter( seeJ ones& Boltz,1 989;J ones,B oltz& Kidd,1 982;J ones& Ralston, 1991). If this hypothesisi s true, then a positivec orrelationw ould be pre- dicted between melodic accent and metricp osition- such as the positive correlationst hat have been observedb oth for agogica nd dynamica ccents. Becausew e havei dentifiede ightc ompetingn otionso f melodica ccent,e ight independentc orrelationsm ay be calculated.A notion of melodic accent that is spuriousw ould be expectedt o have little or no systematicr elation- ship with the metricalo rganization,a nd so would exhibit a correlation near zero. (However,a s noted above, there are other possible interpreta- tions of a nonsignificantc orrelation.)C onverselya, notion of melodic ac- cent that best capturest he presumedp erceptualp henomenonw ould be expectedt o exhibitt he highestc orrelationw ith metricp osition( eithern ega- tive or positive). SAMPLE As an initials ample,w e assembleda randoms electiono f 100 vocal melo- dies from Westernc ulture-s principallyE uropeanf olk songs. The major- ity of works were randomlys electedf rom the Essen Folksong Collection computerd atabase( Schaffrath1, 995). This databasec ontains more than 6,000 melodiese ncodedf rom a largen umbero f traditionala nd scholarly sources. The encoded data include informationc onsistent with conven- tional Westernn otation- includingm eters ignatures,k eys, badines, dura- tions, absolutep itch (equallyt empereda pproximations)a, ccidentalsr, ests, ties, and phrasei ndications. Sincet he Essenc ollectionc ontainsv ery few works from Britaina nd the United States, additionalm aterialsw ere randomlys electedf rom existing databaseso f Britishf olk ballads (Bronson,1 959) and Americanp opular melodies by StephenF oster.I n total, the sample includedv ocal melodies

Description:
ent notions of melodic accent by analyzing three samples of music. To an- ticipate our results, the analyses will show that the musical practice is most .. databases of British folk ballads (Bronson, 1959) and American popular Unix software tools for music research: The Humdrum Toolkit reference.
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.