ebook img

What are the associations between parenting, callous–unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior PDF

16 Pages·2013·0.44 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview What are the associations between parenting, callous–unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior

ClinicalPsychologyReview33(2013)593–608 ContentslistsavailableatSciVerseScienceDirect Clinical Psychology Review What are the associations between parenting, callous–unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior in youth? A systematic review of evidence Rebecca Waller a,⁎, Frances Gardner a, Luke W. Hyde b aCentreforEvidence-BasedIntervention,DepartmentofSocialPolicyandIntervention,UniversityofOxford,UK bDepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofMichigan,USA H I G H L I G H T S •DimensionsofparentingpredictCUtraitsinprospectivelongitudinalstudies. •CUtraitsareresponsivetoparentinginparent-focusedinterventionsforAB. •Theconstructof'CUtraits'asunresponsivetoparentingneedsrevisiting. •FuturestudiesneedbettermeasurementapproachestoassessCUtraitsandparenting. •Greaterprecisionisneededinstudydesigntoassessassociations. a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t Articlehistory: A growing body of research has examined callous–unemotional (CU) traits among samples of antisocial Received24August2012 youth. Debate surrounds the malleability of CU traits and their responsiveness to parenting and Receivedinrevised18February2013 parent-focusedinterventions.Thisreviewexaminesevidencefromstudiesthathaveinvestigatedvariousre- Accepted1March2013 lationshipsbetweenparenting,CUtraits,andantisocialbehavior(AB).Studieswerecategorizedaccordingto Availableonline14March2013 fivedistinctresearchquestionseachaddressingassociationsamongparenting,CUtraits,andABinadifferent way.TheresultssuggestthatdimensionsofparentingareprospectivelyrelatedtochangesinCUtraits.Sub- Keywords: groupsofyouthwithbothhighlevelsofCUtraitsandABalsoappeartohaveexperiencednegativeparenting Antisocialbehavior Callous–unemotional practices.However,negativeparentingisnotconsistentlyrelatedtoABincross-sectionalstudiesforyouth Conductproblems withhighlevelsofCUtraits.Atthesametime,parenting-focusedinterventionsappeareffectiveinreducing Parenting thelevelofABandCUtraitsinyouth.Thefindingsandimplicationsforfuturestudiesarecriticallydiscussed astheyposechallengesforcurrentetiologicaltheoriesofAB. ©2013PublishedbyElsevierLtd. Contents 1. Introduction. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 594 1.1. CUtraits:overviewanddefinitions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 594 1.2. Parenting:theoreticalandempiricallinkswithABandCUtraits . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 594 2. Methods . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 595 3. Results . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 595 3.1. Methodologicallimitationsofincludedstudies(seeTable2) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 595 3.2. DoesparentingdirectlypredictlevelofyouthCUtraits?(Fig.1a). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 599 3.2.1. Earlychildhood/preschool(ages3–6) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 599 3.2.2. Middle/latechildhood(ages7–12) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 599 3.2.3. Adolescence(ages13–18) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 600 3.2.4. Summary . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 600 Abbreviations:AB,antisocialbehavior;APQ,AlabamaParentingQuestionnaire;APSD,AntisocialProcessScreeningDevice;CD,conductdisorder;CP,conductproblems;CU, callous–unemotional;ODD,OppositionalDefiantDisorder;RCT,randomizedcontrolledtrial. ⁎ Correspondingauthorat:CentreforEvidence-BasedIntervention,DepartmentofSocialPolicyandIntervention,UniversityofOxford,32WellingtonSquare,Oxford,OX1-2ER, UK.Tel.:+441865280338. E-mailaddress:[email protected](R.Waller). 0272-7358/$–seefrontmatter©2013PublishedbyElsevierLtd. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.03.001 594 R.Walleretal./ClinicalPsychologyReview33(2013)593–608 3.3. DoesparentingpredictyouthABatdifferentlevelsofCUtraits?(Fig.1b) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 600 3.3.1. Earlychildhood/preschool(ages3–6) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 600 3.3.2. Middle/latechildhood(ages7–12) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 601 3.3.3. Adolescence(ages13–18) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 601 3.3.4. Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 601 3.4. DoesparentingdifferbetweensubgroupsofyouthcategorizedaccordingtotheirlevelofABandCUtraits?(Fig.1c). . . . . . . . . . 602 3.4.1. Earlychildhood/preschool(ages3–6) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 602 3.4.2. Middle/latechildhood(ages7–12) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 602 3.4.3. Adolescence(ages13–18) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 603 3.4.4. Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 603 3.5. DoparentinginterventionsdirectlyreducethelevelofyouthCUtraits?(Fig.1d). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 603 3.5.1. Earlychildhood/preschool(ages3–6) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 603 3.5.2. Middle/latechildhood(ages7–12) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 603 3.5.3. Adolescence(ages13–18) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 604 3.5.4. Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 604 3.6. DoCUtraitspredictormoderateeffectivenessofparentinginterventionsforAB?(Fig.1e) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 604 3.6.1. Earlychildhood/preschool(ages3–6) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 604 3.6.2. Middle/latechildhood(ages7–12) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 604 3.6.3. Adolescence(ages13–18) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 605 3.6.4. Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 605 4. Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 605 4.1. Integrationacrossstudies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 605 4.2. Limitations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 606 4.3. Conclusionsandfuturedirections. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 606 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 607 1.Introduction psychopaths,includingreducedresponsivitytoothers'distress(Blair, Colledge, Murray, & Mitchell, 2001) and insensitivity to punishment Antisocialbehavior(AB)causesharmtoindividuals,families,and (Blair,Colledge,&Mitchell,2001).CUtraitsarealsoheritable(Viding, communities, and is costly to society (Scott, Knapp, Henderson, & Blair,Moffitt,&Plomin,2005),associatedwithdifferencesinneuralre- Maughan, 2001). Developing well-evidenced etiological theories and activity(e.g.,Marshetal.,2008),andappearmoderatelystable(e.g., identifying intervention targets are key research priorities. Develop- Dadds,Fraser,Frost,&Hawes,2005). mentalpsychopathologyresearchhasconsistentlyhighlightedhetero- However,studieshavetypicallyassessedpreadolescentoradoles- geneity in the cause, development, treatment responsiveness, and cent samples of males using cross-sectional designs. These caveats long-termoutcomesbetweensubgroupsofantisocialyouth.Inthelast limitthegeneralizabilityoffindingsandmakeitdifficulttodrawcon- 15years,therehasbeenafocusonthepresenceofcallous–unemotional clusionsaboutcausality.StudiesinvestigatingthestabilityofCUtraits (CU)traitsamongasubgroupofantisocialyouth.Asignificantbodyof havealsotendedtoassessadolescentmalesovershorttimeintervals, researchhasexaminedmeasurementofCUtraits,associatedcognitive andreporthigherstabilitywhenthesameinformantisusedacross andsocioemotionalcharacteristics,andtheneurobiologicalandgenetic assessments(Andershed,2010).Inaddition,whenstudiesinvestigate profilesofantisocialyouthwithCUtraits(Frick&White,2008).Further- mean-levelversusindividual-levelstability,orbehaviortrajectories, more,ithasbeenproposedthatCUtraitsbeincludedasaspecifierof there are significant fluctuations in the level of CU traits (see conduct disorder (CD) in the forthcoming edition of the Diagnostic Fontaine, McCrory, Boivin, Moffitt, & Viding, 2011; Frick, Kimonis, and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; Frick & Moffit, Dandreaux, & Farell, 2003). Indeed, the question of how malleable 2010), making them an important clinical target. A key debate has CUtraitsareisakeyfocusofthisreview.Atthesametime,research alsofocusedonthemalleabilityofCUtraits.Studieshaveinvestigated intoCUtraitsatveryyoungagescouldhelptoidentifychildrenatrisk whetherparentingpracticesarerelatedtothedevelopmentofCUtraits, ofsevereAB,andprovideinsightintorisk/protectivefactorsatatime whetherparentinginterventionsforABarelesseffectiveforyouthwho whenpersonalitystructureisstilldevelopingandmaybemostame- havehighlevelsofCUtraits,andwhetherCUtraitsimprovefollowing nabletointervention. parentinginterventions.Thisreviewseekstosynthesizeandevaluate findingsfromobservationalandinterventionstudiesthathaveinvesti- 1.2.Parenting:theoreticalandempiricallinkswithABandCUtraits gatedassociationsbetweenparenting,CUtraits,andABinyouth. Researchovermanydecadeshashighlightedtheimportanceofpar- 1.1.CUtraits:overviewanddefinitions entingtoABdevelopment,includingcoerciveparent–childinteractions (Patterson, 1982), poor parental supervision (Loeber, Farrington, In adults, psychopathy refers to a complex syndrome, which in- Stouthamer-Loeber,&VanKammen,1998),andlowpositiveparent– cludesinterpersonal,affective,andlifestyletraits,andidentifiesmore child engagement (Gardner, Ward, Burton, & Wilson, 2003). A key severeandviolentoffenders(Hare,1991).Inrecentyears,itsdown- question,however,iswhetherparentingisrelatedtothedevelopment wardextensiontoyouthhasbeendominatedbyafocusoninterperson- ofCUtraits.Inaddition,whiletherehavebeenmanyinvestigationsinto al(e.g.,deceitfulnessandmanipulativeness)andaffective(e.g.,shallow the effectiveness of intervention programs for AB in general, less is affectandlackofguilt)dimensions,whichhavemostcommonlybeen knownaboutpreventionandtreatmentwhenyouthshowalackofem- labeledasCUtraits(Frick,2004;Frick,O'Brien,Wootton,&McBurnett, pathyandlowaffectiveresponsivity(Frick,2001). 1994).Thisfocusissupportedbyresearch,whichsuggeststhatCUtraits Ononehand,childrenwithCUtraitsappearlesssusceptibletopa- alsodesignateahigh-riskgroupofantisocialyouth.Forexample,CU rental socialization and discipline efforts (e.g., Oxford, Cavell, & traitsareassociatedwithparticularlyseverepatternsofAB(seeFrick Hughes,2003).Specifically,CUtraitsarehypothesizedtoproduceanat- &White,2008).ExperimentaldatasuggestthatyouthwithCUtraits tenuatedresponsetopunishmentcuesorthedistressofothersfollowing show a neurocognitive and socioemotional profile similar to adult behavioraltransgressions.Failuretomaketheseassociationsappearsto R.Walleretal./ClinicalPsychologyReview33(2013)593–608 595 disruptthedevelopmentofempathyandmoralsocialization,therebyin- Australia. To clarify associations between parenting, CU traits, and creasingthelikelihoodofaggressivebehavior(Blair,Peschardt,Budhani, youth AB, five research questions were identified among studies &Pine,2006;Kochanska,1997).Ontheotherhand,thereisevidence (Fig. 1a–e): (a) Does parenting directly predict level of youth CU thatparentalharshnessandlowwarmtharerelatedtoCUtraits(Frick traits?(b)DoesparentingpredictyouthABatdifferentlevelsofCU etal.,2003;Pardini,Lochman,&Powell,2007).Harsh punishmentis traits?(c)Doesparentingdifferbetweensubgroupsofyouthcatego- thoughttoelicithighlevelsofarousal,makingitdifficultforchildren rizedaccordingtotheirlevelofABandCUtraits?(d)Doparentingin- to internalize parental messages about prosocial behavior (Pardini et terventions directly reduce the level of youth CU traits? (e) Do CU al.,2007).Incontrast,parentalwarmthandresponsivenessmaywork traitsmoderateorpredicttheeffectivenessofparentinginterventions againstthedevelopmentofABbypromotingempathyandprosociality, foryouthAB? particularlyinchildrenwithfearlesstemperaments(Kochanska,1997). Giventhedistinctresearchquestionsaddressedacrossstudies,range However, developing a precise understanding of how parenting, CU of analytic techniques, and heterogeneous measurement approaches, traits,andABarerelatedinyouthhasbeenhamperedbystudiestesting meta-analysiswasnotfeasible.Studieswerethereforereviewedina markedlydifferentresearchquestionsviadifferentdesigns,invarying narrativesynthesisaccordingto:(a)researchquestion;and(b)ageof samples,andusingarangeofassessmentmethods.Thissystematicre- youthbasedonmeanageofsampleandgivenretainedstudieshadan viewaimstosynthesizetheresultsofstudiesthathaveinvestigatedvar- upper age range of 18years (early childhood/preschool, 3–6years; iousassociationsbetweenparenting,CUtraits,andAB.Specificallythe middle/latechildhood,7–12years;adolescence,13–18years).Several reviewexaminesseveralquestions,includingwhetherparentingisre- studiesexaminedmorethanoneresearchquestion,andthereforeap- latedtoCUtraits,whetherparentinginterventionsforABareeffective pearinmultiplesectionsofthereview.Asummaryofthemainfindings foryouthwithCUtraits,andwhethertherearedevelopmentaldiffer- andkeylimitationsforeachstudyispresentedinTable2.Studieswere encesintheextentortypeofassociationsbetweenparenting,CUtraits, systematically and critically appraised for methodological limitations andAB. accordingto:(a)samplesize;(b)typeofsample(e.g.,communityvs. clinic-referred; male vs. female); (c) measurement of CU traits; and 2.Methods (d)measurementofparenting. Thereviewwasbasedonasystematicsearchofeightdatabases,in- 3.Results cludingMEDLINE,PsycINFO,andDissertationsandThesesAbstracts. Thesearchstrategycombinedtermstoidentifystudiesinvestigating 3.1.Methodologicallimitationsofincludedstudies(seeTable2) CUtraitsandparentingamongyouth:(adolescen*ORboy*ORchild* ORgirl*ORinfant*ORjuvenile*ORpreadolescen*ORpre-adolescen* Withinlongitudinalstudies,samplesizestendedtobelarge(8stud- ORpreschool*ORpre-school*ORschoolchild*ORtoddler*ORteen* ieswithN>500),whereaswithincross-sectionalstudies,samplesizes ORyoungORyouth)AND(callous*ORpsychopathyORpsychopathic weresmaller(8studieswithNb200).Inseveralcross-sectionalstud- ORpsychopathORsociopath*ORunemotional)AND(caregiver*OR ies, sample sizes were very small (Nb100). Treatment studies also care-giver*ORfamil*ORfather*ORmaternalORmother*ORparent* tendedtohavesmallsamplesizes(7studies,Nb200).Theriskofre- ORpaternal).Nodate,publication,orlanguagerestrictionswereim- duced generalizability of findings due to small samples, particularly posed.Abstractsofidentifiedarticleswerescreenedforthefollowing forcross-sectionalandtreatmentstudies,needstobeconsideredalong- inclusioncriteria:(a)presentationofdatatestingcross-sectionalor sideresults.Withinthe12longitudinalstudies,5examinedcommunity longitudinalassociationsbetweenparenting,CUtraits,andABusing sampleswithequalnumbersofmalesandfemales,whichwerealso validatedorpreviouslypublishedmeasuresofparentingandCUtraits; reported as reflecting the ethnicity of the general population. The and(b)assessmentofyouthwithanupperagerangeof18yearsold, remaining seven studies assessed at-risk or aggressive youth, and from community, school, clinic, or research settings. Treatment tended to comprise majority Caucasian or African-American partici- studiesmeetingthesecriteriawereretainedif:(a)treatmentfocused pants. One longitudinal study assessed only females (Kroneman, on changing parenting behavior or parent–child interactions; and Hipwell, Loeber, Koot, & Pardini, 2011) and another, only males (b)CUtraitsweretestedasanoutcomeormoderatorofeffectiveness. (Pardini&Loeber,2008).Ingeneral,becauseofsizeandtypeofsample, Norestrictionswereplacedondesign,exceptthatstudiesrelyingon longitudinal studies appear to have greater generalizability. Within youthretrospectivereportsofparentingwereexcluded. cross-sectionalstudies,fivewereclinic-referredoraggressivesamples, Thesearchidentified2606records.First,titlesandabstractsofall and were either all or majority male. Necessarily, treatment studies identifiedrecordswerescreened.Studieswereretainedforfurtherin- assessedclinic-referredoradjudicatedsamples,andthreeintervention spectioniftheycorrespondedtotheaimsofthereview.Fromthere, studieswereconductedusinghigh-riskyouthwithincommunityset- the full texts of 64 potentially relevant studies were examined to tings.Nevertheless,withoneexception(Hydeetal.,2013),intervention assess whether they met the inclusion criteria of the review. After studiesassessedmajority/exclusivelymalesamples. inspecting these 64 papers, 34 were removed (typically, these did Acrossthe30studies,24usedtheAntisocialProcessScreeningDe- notassessparentingorCUtraits).Severalstudiesinvestigatingmod- vice (APSD; Frick & Hare, 2002) or its predecessor, the Psychopathy eration of the effectiveness of non-parenting interventions by CU ScreeningDevice(PSD),toassessCUtraits.TheAPSDisa20-itemmea- traits(e.g.,Waschbusch,Carrey,Willoughby,King,&Andrade,2007) sure,whichincludesa6-itemCUtraitsubscale.Ithasbeenvalidated or treatment of incarcerated psychopathic youth (e.g., Caldwell, across different formats, including parent and teacher rating and Skeem, Salekin, & Van Ryoboek, 2006) were also identified but not self-report scales. Nevertheless, there are various well-documented retained.Twostudiesthatreliedonretrospectivereportsofparenting drawbacks of the CU trait subscale of the APSD, including the small by incarcerated adolescents (Edens, Skopp, & Cahill, 2008; Fritz, numberofitems(n=6),poorinternalconsistencyofsubscales,and Ruchkin,Kaposov,&Klinteberg,2008)wereexcludedbecauseofthe limitedrangeofresponseoptions(n=3),whichreducesthevariance difficultyofobtainingreliablereportsofparentingfromthistypeof ofscoresobtained(Kotler&McMahon,2010).Despitetheselimitations, sample.Thefinalpoolof30studiescomprised26differentsamples, thefactthatsomanystudiesusedtheAPSDtoassessCUtraitsenables and includes research published between 1997 and 2013, and one greatercomparabilityofresults. unpublisheddissertation. Of the six studies that did not use the APSD, two assessed Table1summarizesstudycharacteristics.Of30studies,12were ‘deceitful-callous behavior’ in the same sample of preschool children longitudinal, 10 cross-sectional, and 8 investigated parenting inter- (Hyde et al., 2013; Waller, Gardner, Hyde, Shaw, Dishion, & Wilson, ventions. All but three studies were carried out in the US, UK, or 2012),oneassessed‘interpersonal-callousness’inadolescents(Pardini 596 R.Walleretal./ClinicalPsychologyReview33(2013)593–608 Table1 Characteristicsofincludedstudies. Study Country Sample Typeofsample %female Agerange Ethnicity Measureofparenting MeasureofCUtraits size (years) Longitudinalstudies Barkeretal.(2011) UK 6673 Representative 43 2,4&13 Mixed,majorityCaucasian Questionnaire(P) 6-itemquestionnaire, CUtraits(P) Fontaineetal.(2011) UK 9578 Representative 53 4,7,&12 PredominantlyCaucasian Questionnaire(P) 3CUitems,APSD; 4prosocialitems,SDQ(T) Fricketal.(2003) USA 98 Aggressive 47 8–12 Mixed,majorityCaucasian APQ&questionnaire 6CUitems,APSD(PT&Y) (P&Y) Hawesetal.(2011) Aus 1008 Representative 47 3–10 PredominantlyCaucasian APQ(P) 3CUitems,APSD; 4prosocialitems,SDQ(P) Kimonisetal.(2004) USA 98 Aggressive 47 8–12 Mixed,majorityCaucasian APQ(P&Y) 6CUitems,APSD(PT) Kronemanetal.(2011) USA 1233 High-risk 100 7–8 Mixed,majority Questionnaire(P) 4CUitems,APSD(PT) African-American Larssonetal.(2008) UK 4430 Representative 47 3–4 PredominantlyCaucasian Questionnaire(P) 3CUitems,APSD; 4prosocialitems,SDQ(T) Muñozetal.(2011) USA 98 Aggressive 47 12–16 Mixed,majorityCaucasian APQ(P) 6CUitems,APSD(Y) PardiniandLoeber(2008) USA 506 High-risk 0 13–14 Mixed,majority Questionnaire(PY) 8items,ICquestionnaire(P) African-American Pardinietal.(2007) USA 120 Aggressive 41 9–12 Mixed,majority APQ&questionnaire 6CUitems,APSD(PT) African-American (P&Y) Vidingetal.(2009) UK 4508 Representative 54 7,12 PredominantlyCaucasian Questionnaire(P) 3CUitems,APSD; 4prosocialitems,SDQ(P&T) Walleretal.(2012) USA 731 High-risk 49 2,3,4 Mixed,majorityCaucasian Observed& 5items,DCbehavior questionnaire(P) questionnaire(P) Cross-sectionalstudies Enebrinketal.(2005) Swe 41 Clinic-referred 0 6–13 PredominantlyCaucasian Interviews&case 6CUitems,APSD(P) records(P&A) FalkandLee(2011) USA 208 Clinic-referred 30 6–9 Mixed,majorityCaucasian APQ(P) 6CUitems,APSD(P) Hipwelletal.(2007) USA 990 High-risk 100 7–8 Mixed,majority Questionnaire(P) 4CUitems,APSD(PT) African-American KoglinandPetermann Lux 177 Representative 50 5–7 Mixed,majorityCaucasian APQ(P) 6CUitems,APSD(P) (2008) Loneyetal.(2007) USA 83 Representative 53 7–13 Mixed,majorityCaucasian APQ(P) 6CUitems,APSD(PT) Oxfordetal.(2003) USA 199 Aggressive 35 8–10 Mixed,majorityCaucasian APQ(P) 6CUitems,APSD(PT) Pasalichetal.(2011) Aus 95 Clinic-referred 0 4–12 PredominantlyCaucasian Observed&speech 3CUitems,APSD; samplescoding(O&P) 4prosocialitems,SDQ(PTY) Vitaccoetal.(2003) USA 136 Representative 100 10–15 Hispanic APQ(Y) 6CUitems,APSD(Y) Woottonetal.(1997) USA 166 Clinic-referred 24 6–13 PredominantlyCaucasian APQ(P) 6CUitems,PSD(PT) Yehetal.(2011) USA 1210 Representative 52 9–10 Mixed,majorityCaucasian Parentaffect CPS(P) questionnaire(Y) Treatment/interventionstudies HawesandDadds(2005) Aus 49 Clinic-referred 0 4–8 nr Observed& 3CUitems,APSD; questionnaire(O&P) 4prosocialitems,SDQ(P) HawesandDadds(2007) Aus 49 Clinic-referred 0 4–8 nr Observed& 3CUitems,APSD; questionnaire(O&P) 4prosocialitems,SDQ(P) Hydeetal.(2013) USA 731 High-risk 49 2,3,4,5 Mixed,majorityCaucasian Observed& 5items,DCbehavior questionnaire(P) questionnaire(P) KolkoandPardini(2010) USA 177 Clinic-referred 19 6–11 Mixed,majorityCaucasian na 6CUitems,APSD(T) Kolkoetal.(2009) USA 139 Clinic-referred 15 6–11 Mixed,majorityCaucasian na 6CUitems,APSD(T) McDonaldetal.(2011) USA 66 High-risk nr 4–9 Mixed,majorityCaucasian Questionnaire(P) 16-itemtotalPSD(P) SomechandElizur(2012) Israel 209 High-risk 20 2–5 PredominantlyIsraeli APQ-Revised(P) 3CUitems,APSD; 8items,ICU(P) White(2010)and USA 134 Clinic-referred 28 11–17 Mixed,majority APQ(P&Y) ICU(Y) Whiteetal.(2012) African-American Note.Forinformantonmeasures:P,parent;Y,youth;T,teacher;PT,combinationofparentandteacher;PC,combinationofparentandchild;O,observed;A,alternativeapproach. APSD,AntisocialProcessScreeningDevice;APQ,AlabamaParentingQuestionnaire;CPS,ChildPsychopathyScale;DC,deceitful-callousbehavior;IC,InterpersonalCallousness;ICU, InventoryofCallous–UnemotionalTraits;PCL-YV,PsychopathyChecklist-YouthVersion;PSD,PsychopathyScreeningDevice.Forlongitudinalstudies,agerangereportedforfirst assessment,orforageatoutcome(ifrelevant). &Loeber,2008;Pardini,Obradovic,&Loeber,2006),oneusedamodified measureCUtraits.Finally,White(2010)1assessedCUtraitsamongad- versionoftheChildPsychopathyScale(CPS;Lynam&Gudonis,2005)to judicatedadolescentsusingtheInventoryofCallous–Unemotionaltraits assess psychopathy in middle childhood (Yeh, Chen, Raine, Baker, & (ICU;Frick,2004).SomechandElizur(2012)alsouseditemsfromthe Jacobson, 2011), and one used a CU trait scale similar to the APSD preschoolICUcombinedwithAPSDitems. (Barker, Oliver, Viding, Salekin, & Maughan, 2011). Several studies (e.g.,Hawes&Dadds,2005;Viding,Fontaine,Oliver,&Plomin,2009) usedthreeoftheAPSDCUtraititems,andcombinedthemwithfour (negativelycorrelating)itemsoftheProsocialBehaviorsubscaleofthe 1 Apeer-reviewed/publishedversionisnowavailable(Whiteetal.,2012).Thepub- StrengthsandDifficultiesQuestionnaire(SDQ;Goodman,1997).Many lishedversionofthepaperdoesnotincludeanalysesusingparentingmeasures,which wereincludedintheoriginaldissertation(White,2010).However,thepublishedpa- ofthesealternativesaretherefore‘home-grown’measurescomprising perincludes useful analytic approachesforexaminingchangeinCUtraits andAB questionnaire items within studies that did not originally set out to scores.Assuch,botharereferenced. R.Walleretal./ClinicalPsychologyReview33(2013)593–608 597 a) b) c) d) e) f) g) Fig.1.Diagrammaticrepresentationsofstudydesignsincludedinthereview(a–e)andproposedalternativetreatmentdesigns(f&g).a)Doesparentingdirectlypredictlevelof youthCUtraits?b)DoesparentingpredictantisocialbehaviorregardlessofthelevelofyouthCUtraits?c)Doesparentingdifferbetweensubgroupsofyouthcategorizedaccording tolevelortrajectoryofantisocialbehavior(AB)andCUtraits?d)DoparentinginterventionsdirectlyreducethelevelofyouthCUtraits?e)DoCUtraitspredictormoderate effectivenessofparentinginterventionsforyouthAB?f)DoparentinginterventionssimultaneouslyreducelevelofyouthCUtraitsandAB?g)Dointerventionsthattargetspecific dimensionsofparentinguniquelyreducelevelofyouthCUtraitsversusAB? Acrossall30studies,23usedparentreporttoassessCUtraits.Of somestudies.Howeverwhilethereareadvantagestothisapproach, these23studies,13reliedsolelyonparentreportandtheremaining it also leads to loss of scale- and item-level information. Likewise, 10combinedparentratingswithteacherorchildratings(orboth). while cut-off scores are used in various studies, the validity of a Commonly,thisapproachinvolvessummingratingsatanitemlevel ‘high’versus‘low’distinctionforCUtraitsamongyouthisyettobe or creating a best estimate score, which combines ratings by using established.Toretainscale-anditem-levelinformation,futurestudies thehigherscorefromreportersforeachitem.Inanalternativeap- could also create latent variable scores, which combine parent-, proach, scores were obtained by calculating the proportion of re- teacher-,andself-reports,andcouldevenincorporateofficialreports, porterswhoclassifiedthechildashighonCUtraits(Pasalich,Dadds, including court records (see Trentacosta, Hyde, Shaw, & Cheong, Hawes, & Brennan, 2011). The use of the best estimate approach 2009).Finally,sevenofthe30reviewedstudiesprovidednodataon helps incorporate multiple informants, provides a cross-context as- theinternalconsistencyoftheirCUtraits'measure.Ofthe23studies sessment of behavior, may increase validity, and is thus favored in thatdidprovideCronbach'salphas,10werereportedasbeingb.70, 598 R.Walleretal./ClinicalPsychologyReview33(2013)593–608 Table2 Resultsandmajorrisksofbiasofincludedstudiespresentedaccordingtoresearchquestion/design(seeFig.1). Study Keyfindingsrelevanttoaimsofreview Mainmethodologicallimitations a)DoesparentingdirectlypredictlevelsofyouthCUtraits? Barkeretal.(2011)a L Harshparentingatage4predictedboys'CUtraitsatage13. Measuresallparentreport;noalphareported forCUtraitmeasure Fricketal.(2003)a L Youthandparentreportsofnegativeparentingpartially Smallsamplesize;selectedtooverrepresent correlatedwithCUtraits4yearslater. youthwithCP Hawesetal.(2011) L Parentalpositivereinforcement,poormonitoring/ Measuresallparentreport supervision,involvementpredictedCUtraits Loneyetal.(2007) CS Parentingdysfunctionmediatedassociationbetween Relianceonparentreport;noalphasreported maternalpsychopathyandCUtraits McDonaldetal.(2011)a L Parentpsychologicalaggressionandinconsistent LowalphaforCUtraitmeasure(.34); parentingrelatedtopsychopathicfeatures measuresallparent-report PardiniandLoeber(2008) L Poorparent–childcommunicationpredictedhigher CUtraitsmeasurefromarchivedparent-reported initialandfinallevelsofCUtraits items;malesample Pardinietal.(2007)a L Parent-reportedpunishmentandchild-reported Lowalphaformeasureofcorporalpunishment(.29) parentalinvolvementpredictedCUtraits Vidingetal.(2009) L NegativeparentaldisciplinepredictedCUtraitsbut Lowalphasformeasuresofnegativeparental notMZtwindifferencesinCUtraits disciplineandCUtraits Vitaccoetal.(2003) CS Poormonitoringandinconsistentdisciplinenotrelated Allfemale,Hispanicsample;youthreportonly; toCUtraits noalphasreported Walleretal.(2012) L Observedandparent-reportedharshness(notobserved LowalphaforCUtraitmeasureatages2(.57) positiveparenting)predictedCUtraits. and3(.64) b)DoesparentingpredictyouthABatdifferentlevelsofCUtraits? FalkandLee(2011) CS In1/8modelstested,lowerpositiveparentingpredicted Relianceonparentreport CDsymptomsatlowCUtraits. Hipwelletal.(2007) CS CPassociatedwithharshpunishmentandlowparental 4itemsonlyforCUtraitmeasure;lowalpha(.60); warmthatlowlevelsofCUtraits allfemalesample Hydeetal.(2013)a L CUtraits-parentinginteractiondidnotpredictgrowthin Low-moderatealphaforCUtraitmeasureat CPfromages2–4. age3(.64) KoglinandPetermann(2008) CS Inconsistentdisciplineassociatedwithboys'aggressive Measuresallparentreport;noalphasreported behavioratlowlevelsofCUtraits. Kronemanetal.(2011) L Lowparentalwarmthpredictedhigherinitiallevelsof Measuresallcomprisedparentreport;lowalpha CD/ODDandfasterdecreasinglevelsofCD/ODDover formeasurementofCUtraits(.60);femalesample 5yearsathighlevelsofCUtraits. Muñozetal.(2011) L ForlowCUtraitsgroup,higherparentalcontrolpredicted Smallsample;largeagerange;selectedtoover knowledge,andhighersolicitationpredictedcontroland representyouthwithCP;only1yearfollow-up lessknowledgeledtoincreasedparentalcontrol Pardinietal.(2007)a L CUtraits-parentinginteractionsdidnotpredictAB1yearlater Lowalphaformeasureofcorporalpunishment(.29) Pasalichetal.(2011) CS Maternalandpaternalobservedcoercionrelatedtoboys' Small,clinic-referredsamplewithlargeagerange CPatlowlevelsofCUtraits.Maternalwarmthrelatedto boys'CPathighormeanlevelsofCUtraits Woottonetal.(1997) CS In3/6interactions,ineffectiveandlowpositive Small,clinic-referredsample;largeagerange; parentingwererelatedtoCPatlowCUtraits. parentreport Yehetal.(2011) CS Lowerpositiveandhighernegativeparentalaffectpredicted Lowalphasformeasurementofnegativeparental reactiveaggressionatlowCUtraits.Highernegativeparental affect(male,.53;female,.60) affectpredictedproactiveaggressionathighormeanCUtraits. c)DoesparentingdifferbetweensubgroupsofyouthcategorizedaccordingtotheirlevelofABandCUtraits? Barkeretal.(2011)a L AB+CU+versusAB−CU−groupexperiencedharsher Parentreportforallmeasures;noalphareported parentingandlowerwarmth.AB+CU+versus forCUtraitmeasure AB+CU−grouphadhighermaternalpsychopathology, negativefeelingsandharsherparenting Enebrinketal.(2005) CS AB+CU+versusAB+CU−groupexperiencedpoorer Verysmall,clinic-referredsampleofmales homecircumstancesandmorefamilystress Fontaineetal.(2011) L AB+CU+versusAB−CU−groupexperiencedmore Population-basedsampleoftwins;verysmall% negativeparentalfeelingsandharshdiscipline.AB+CU ofsampleinsomejointtrajectorygroups increasinghadmorefamilyandparentingriskfactors Fricketal.(2003)a L StableCU+youthhadlowerlevelsofyouth-reported Verysmallsubgroups;largeagerange;over-selected positiveparentingandhigherCP sampleforCP Kimonisetal.(2004) L AB+CU+andAB−CU+versusAB−CU−and Smallsample,largeagerange;over-selectedsample AB+CU−groupexperiencedlowerparent-reportedmonitoring/ forCP;relianceonparentreportformeasures supervisionandyouth-reportedparentalinvolvement Larssonetal.(2008) L AB+CU+andAB+CU−groupshadhigherlevelsofnegative Population-basedsampleoftwins;harshparental parentalfeelingsandharshparentaldisciplineatages3,4, disciplinehadlowalphaatages3(.59),4(.56) and7.ControllingforearlierCPmadethisnon-significant. and7(.56). d)DoparentinginterventionshaveadirecteffectonthelevelofyouthCUtraits HawesandDadds(2007) T Pre-andpost-treatmentchangesinCUtraits. Nocontrolgroup;verysmallsample;maleonly; allparentreported McDonaldetal.(2011)a I ReductionsinCUtraitsintreatmentgroupandmediated LowalphaforCUtraits(.34);smallsample; throughimprovedparenting relianceonparent-report Kolkoetal.(2009) T ReductionsinCUtraitsshowninbothtreatmentarms Nocontrolgroup;multipletreatmentcomponents; across3-yearfollow-up malesonly SomechandElizur(2012) T ReductionsinCUtraitsintreatmentgroup,maintained Attrition;measuresallparentreport;male at1yearfollow-up dominatedsample R.Walleretal./ClinicalPsychologyReview33(2013)593–608 599 Table2(continued) Study Keyfindingsrelevanttoaimsofreview Mainmethodologicallimitations e)DoCUtraitsmoderateorpredicttheeffectivenessofparentinginterventionsforyouthAB? HawesandDadds(2005) T CUtraitsassociatedwithincreasedlikelihoodofODD Nocontrolgroup;verysmallsample;maleonly; diagnosisaftertreatment. allparentreported Hydeetal.(2013)a I Age3CUtraitsdidnotmoderateinterventioneffectiveness Manychildrendidnothaveexistingbehavior problems KolkoandPardini(2010) T TeacherreportsofCUtraitsdidnotpredicttreatment Nocontrolgroup;majoritymalesample; outcomebutparentreportsof‘hurtfulness’did manytreatmentcomponents White(2010)andWhiteetal.(2012) T FFTeffectiveandhighCUtraitshadbetteroutcomes, Nocontrolgroup;highattrition;lowinternal whichrelatedtochangesinparenting consistencyformeasures Note.Fordesign,L,longitudinalstudy;CS,cross-sectionalstudy.APSD,AntisocialProcessScreeningDevice;CD,conductdisorder;CP,conductproblems;DC,deceitful-callous;IC, interpersonal-callousness;FFT,FunctionalFamilyTherapy;ODD,OppositionalDefiantDisorder;PSD,PsychopathyScreeningDevice. a Studyappearsintwosectionsasanalyticstrategyaddressestworesearchquestions. and,insomecases,aslowas.40,whichisathreattothevalidityof 3.2.1.Earlychildhood/preschool(ages3–6) measuresandhighlightstheneedforcontinuedinvestigationofthe TwostudiesinvestigatedthelongitudinalpredictionofCUtraits constructofCUtraitsamongyouthsamples. by dimensions of parenting in preschool samples. First, Hawes, Inthemeasurementofparenting,13ofthe30studiesassesseddi- Dadds,Frost,andHasking(2011),inarepresentativesample(aged mensions of parenting using the Alabama Parenting Questionnaire 3–10years; N=1008), found that parental positive reinforcement (APQ;Shelton,Frick,&Wootton,1996)andin12ofthese13studies, predictedCUtraitsoneyearlater.Therewereinteractionsbetween parentreportontheAPQwasusedexclusively.Twotreatmentstud- positive reinforcement and gender, poor monitoring/supervision iesofthesamesample(Kolko&Pardini,2010;Kolkoetal.,2009)did andage,andbetweenparentalinvolvementandsex.Second,Waller notmeasureparenting,butareretainedinthereviewbecausepar- etal.(2012)foundthatCUtraitsatages3and4werepredictedby entingwasthefocusoftreatmentmodules.Amongstudiesthatdid observedandparent-reportedharshness(atages2and3),control- notusetheAPQ,parentreportonaquestionnairewasstillthemost lingforage2CUtraitsanddemographicfactorsinalarge,highrisk commonmethod.However,althoughcommonlyemployedandrela- sample(N=731). tivelyinexpensive,parentreportissubjecttothewell-knownthreats tovalidityassociatedwithself-reportmethods,includingsocialde- 3.2.2.Middle/latechildhood(ages7–12) sirabilityeffectsordifficultiesinterpretingthemeaningofitemsre- SixstudiesinvestigatedthedirectpredictionofCUtraitsbyparent- lating to parenting constructs (e.g., time-out or proactiveness) inginmiddlechildhood.First,Vitacco,Neumann,Ramos,andRoberts (Morsbach&Prinz,2006).Theselimitationsneedtobeconsidered (2003)foundthatyouthreportsofpoorparentalmonitoringandincon- alongside findings of studies that relied solely on parent-reported sistentdisciplinewerecross-sectionallyrelatedtonarcissismandim- measures. Depending on the research question, direct observation pulsivity, but not to CU traits, in a community sample of Hispanic of parenting is a potentially stronger alternative, or complement, females(N=136).Second,Fricketal.(2003)investigatedpredictors toparent-reportedparenting,takingadvantageofassessingnatural- ofCUtraitstabilityoverfourannualassessments(N=98).Selection ly occurring parenting behavior, using relatively unbiased ob- from a community sample via stratified sampling ensured enough servers. In five studies, parenting was assessed via observation, youthwithhighCUtraits,althoughthismayhavemeantthatantisocial whichstrengthenstheconclusionsthatcanbedrawnabouttheas- youth were overrepresented. CU traits at the final assessment point sociationbetweenparentingandCUtraits,especiallyifusedtocor- werepartiallycorrelatedwithearliernegativeparenting(parentand roborate the results obtained using parent-reported measures. youth report), controlling for earlier CU traits. Third, Pardini et al. Observationalmethods,however,arealsonotimmunefromthreats (2007) investigated predictors of CU traits and AB in an aggressive to validity, including observer reactivity by parents or inadequate sample(9–12yearsold;N=120)overayear.Controllingforearlier sampling of behavior (Gardner, 2000). As such, it may be helpful AB,parent-reportedcorporalpunishmentandchild-reportedparental toconsiderthedimensionofparentingbeingassessed,theageand warmth/involvementpredictedCU traits oneyearlater.Therewasa typeofsample,andbyextension,whichmethodismostappropriate marginally significant interaction suggesting that children with low toprovideavalidassessmentofparenting(i.e.,whethertheparent- anxiety who reported low parental warmth showed increases in CU ing dimension being assessed is readily observable or better traits one year later. Fourth, Loney, Huntenburg, Counts-Allan, and assessed using youth- or parent-report). At the same time, in 11 Schmeelk(2007)foundthatdysfunctionalparentingpracticesfullyme- studies,nodatawasprovidedabouttheinternalconsistencyofthe diatedtheassociationbetweenmaternalpsychopathictraitsandchild parenting measure, and in 14 studies, parenting measures had al- CU traits in a representative sample (N=83; ages 7-14years old). phasofb.70,whichunderminestheirvalidity(seeTable2).Finally, Fifth,ina longitudinal studyof monozygotic twin pairs(N=4508), in21studies,parentreportwasusedsolelyorincombinationwith Vidingetal.(2009)foundthatage7parent-reportednegativedisci- otherreportstoconstructthemeasureofparentingandCUtraits.In plinepredictedparent-andteacher-reportedCUtraitsatage12,after onlysixstudieswastheinformantforparentingandCUtraitsdiffer- controllingforearlierCUtraits.However,age7negativedisciplinedid ent.Ifparentsareaskedtoreflectontheirbehavior,includingim- notpredictmonozygotictwindifferencesinCUtraitsatage12,control- plementation of different discipline strategies, and then evaluate ling for age 7 CU traits. The authors speculated that associations affective/interpersonal characteristics of their child, it is unclear reflectedgene–environmentcorrelations(i.e.,genesandenvironment howratingsforoneaffecttheother,makingitdifficulttointerpret providedbyparentsbutfurtherevokedbyachildwithCUtraits).As studiesthatonlyusedparent-reportedmeasures. such,negativeparentaldisciplinecouldbeanon-sharedenvironmental riskfactorforABdevelopmentbutnotforCUtraitsduringthisagepe- 3.2.DoesparentingdirectlypredictlevelofyouthCUtraits?(Fig.1a) riod (see Vidinget al., 2005). Finally, McDonald, Dodson,Rosenfield, and Jouriles (2011) assessed the association between parenting and Studiesaddressingthisresearchquestiondirectlytestedtheasso- CUtraitsinthecontextofarandomizedcontrolledtrial(RCT)ofanin- ciationbetweenacontinuousmeasureofparentingandcontinuous terventionforCPinchildrenaged4–9yearsold(N=66),recruited outcomeofCUtraits. from domestic violence shelters. Assessments took place at baseline, 600 R.Walleretal./ClinicalPsychologyReview33(2013)593–608 and4,8,12,16,and20monthspost-baseline.Twoparentingvariables dimensions of parenting and the development of CU traits. (psychologicalaggressionandinconsistency)wererelatedtoincreases While Viding and colleagues themselves present evidence for such in psychopathic features over time (total PSD score and a CU-like anassociationinwhattheydescribeasa‘phenotypicanalysis’,nega- subscale) and mediated reductions in psychopathic features in the tivedisciplineonlypredictedmonozygotictwindifferencesinAB,not treatmentgroup. inCUtraits.Thispatternofresultshighlightstheneedforstudiesto take into account passive correlations (i.e., correlations between 3.2.3.Adolescence(ages13–18) child behavior and the parenting environment because of a shared TwostudiesinvestigatedassociationsbetweenparentingandCU genetically transmitted liability) and evocative correlations (i.e., traits in adolescence. First, Pardini and Loeber (2008) investigated child'straitsevokepoorparentingresponses)whenconsideringas- predictorsofCUtraitstabilityoversevenassessmentsinahighrisk, sociationsbetweenparentandchildbehavior.Indeed,thisconclusion malesample(N=506).Parentingwasassessedviafivetime1mea- issupportedbythehighheritabilityestimatesobtainedfortheCPof suresusingsummedchildandparentreports,includingpoorsupervi- childrenwithhigh concurrent levelsofCUtraits(e.g.,Vidingetal., sion,physicalpunishment,andpoorparent–childcommunication.All 2005). parentingvariableswererelatedtohigherinitiallevelsofCUtraitsin While the majority of the studies in this section attempted to bivariateanalyses.Inafinalmultivariatemodelhowever,onlyhigher controlforearlierchildCUtraitsorAB,studiesdidnotconsistently CP, ADHD, and poor parent–child communication predicted higher control for one or both, and few studies considered in detail the initialCUtraits,andonlyhigherCPandpoorparent–childcommuni- effects of emerging CU traits versus AB on parenting behavior at cationpredictedCUtraitsatthefinalassessmentpoint.Noparenting differentages.Interestingly,onestudydidinvestigatechild-driven predictors were associated with the slope of CU traits. Second, effectsonparenting.Hawesetal. (2011)testedwhether CUtraits Barkeretal.(2011)assessedpredictorsofCUtraitsatage13within directly predicted parenting over a one-year period, controlling thecontextoflarge,ongoingpopulation-basedstudyofatwincohort for earlier AB. Higher levels of CU traits predicted inconsistent (N=6673).Maternalreportsofharshparentingatage4wereassoci- parental discipline acrossages, decreasedparental involvementin atedwithhigherlevelsofCUtraitsatage13.Inasignificantindirect older boys, and increased corporal punishment among older chil- pathway, prenatal risk predicted boys' fearlessness at age 2, which dren.Theprediction ofparentingdimensionsbyCUtraitsshowed was associated with higher levels of harsh parenting at age 4, and larger effect sizes than that by AB in general. As such, the results higherlevelsofCUtraitsatage13. highlight the role of CU traits in conferring greater risk for the development of AB by uniquely shaping parenting. Future studies 3.2.4.Summary that assess whether parenting directly predicts CU traits would Acrossallstudies,arelativelyconsistentpictureemergesacrossa therefore benefit from simultaneously considering the effects of rangeofdevelopmentalstages,suggestingthatdimensionsofpar- emergingCUtraitsonparenting.Hawesetal.(2011)provideause- enting are associated with CU traits when the relationship is fulmodelforhowtotestthisquestion.However,amorepowerful assesseddirectly.Oftheninestudiesproviding evidencethat par- analysis could involve testing cascade models in which CU traits entingisrelatedtoCUtraits,eightwerelongitudinal,andintesting andparentingpredicteachotheracrossmultipletimepoints,there- forpredictionbyparenting,controlledforearlierCUtraitsorAB.As byenablingaclearerpicturetobebuiltupofthekindsofinterac- such, there is evidence that parenting is prospectively associated tions occurring in families with a youth at risk of developing, or with CU traits, over and above earlier child-driven effects. In six already showing, high levels of CU traits. It is also worth noting studies,negativedimensionsofparentingpredictedyouthCUtraits. thatabetterunderstandingofparent–childinteractionsisvitalfor Specifically,harshparentingexperiencedinearlychildhood(Barker intervention,evenifriskforthemisunderpinnedbysharedgenetic et al., 2011; Waller et al., 2012), negative discipline (Viding et al., liability.Finally,interactionsbetweenaparentandyouthwithhigh 2009),corporalpunishment(Pardinietal.,2007)andpsychological CUtraitsmaydifferwithage,althoughfuturestudiesareneededto aggressionandinconsistentdiscipline(McDonaldetal.,2011)expe- developapreciseunderstandingofassociationsatdifferentagesof riencedinmiddlechildhood,andpoorparent–childcommunication youthtoinformintervention. inadolescence(Pardini&Loeber,2008)allpredictedhigherlevels ofCUtraits.Thereisalsoevidencethatpositiveparentingpredicts 3.3. Does parenting predict youth AB at different levels of CU traits? decreases in CU traits. Positive reinforcement in early childhood (Fig.1b) (Hawesetal.,2011),child-reportedpositiveparenting(Fricketal., 2003),andchild-reportedparentalwarmth(Pardinietal.,2007)in Studies addressing this question investigate if associations be- middlechildhoodwereassociatedwithlowerCUtraits.Inaddition, tween continuous measures of parenting and AB are moderated by interaction tests suggest that parental involvement is particularly youthCUtraits.Specifically,aninteractiontermisaddedtoregres- important to decreases in CU traits in boys, that supervision/ sionmodels(parenting×CUtraits).Ifsignificant,theassociationbe- monitoringandpositivereinforcementaremoreimportantingirls tween parenting and AB is tested at high versus low levels of CU (Hawes et al., 2011), and that parental warmth has a more pro- traits. nouncedeffectondecreasesinCUtraitsforchildrenwithlowanxi- ety(Pardinietal.,2007). Inlightoftheevidencepresentedabove,itischallengingtointer- 3.3.1.Earlychildhood/preschool(ages3–6) pretfindingsoftheonestudythatdidnotfindevidenceforadirect Twostudiesassessedmoderationoftheassociationbetweenpar- association between parenting and CU traits (Vitacco et al., 2003). enting and AB by CU traits in preschool samples. First, Koglin and Interestingly, this was one of only two cross-sectional studies, and Petermann (2008) found a trend towards significance (pb.10) for it relied on child reports of both parenting and CU traits among the interaction between low parental involvement and CU traits Hispanicfemales.Itisdifficulttogeneralizethesefindingstocommu- predictingaggressivebehaviorinarepresentativesampleofKinder- nityorat-riskmixedgenderandmulti-ethnicsamples.Furthermore, gartenchildren(N=177)assessedcross-sectionally.Inaddition,in- it is difficult to be confident about the validity of measures when consistent discipline was related to aggression, but only for boys relying solely on child reports of parenting, especially when the (n=89)withlowlevelsofCUtraits.Second,Hydeetal.(2013)test- outcome is deficient affective and socioemotional experience. It is edwhetherage3CUtraitsmoderatedtheassociationbetweenage3 alsointerestingtoconsiderthefindingsofVidingetal.(2009)along- observed positive parentingand growthin child CP from ages 2–4, side the evidence for a prospective association between negative andfoundthatitdidnot. R.Walleretal./ClinicalPsychologyReview33(2013)593–608 601 3.3.2.Middle/latechildhood(ages7–12) oppositepatternofresultsforproactiveaggressionsuggeststhatchil- Eightstudies(sixcross-sectional)investigatedmoderationofthe drenwithhighlevelsofpsychopathictraitsmaybemoreatriskof association between parenting and AB by CU traits in middle/late learning from negative displays of parental affect they experience, childhood. First, Wootton, Frick, Shelton, and Silverthorn (1997) although results need replication in other samples. In particular, investigatedwhetheradichotomousCUtraitmeasuremoderatedas- resultsneedreplicatinggiventhatotherstudieshavefoundthatpos- sociations between parenting and CP among clinic-referred males itiveparentalaffectmaybemoreimportanttoCPamongyouthwith (N=166).CUtraitswerealwaysassociatedwithCP.Interactionsbe- highlevelsofCUtraits.Indeed,itisunclearwhyparentalnegativeaf- tweenCUtraitsandineffectiveparenting,andCUtraitsandpositive fect might be related to proactive aggression for youth with high parentingweresignificant.Foreach,associationsbetweenparenting levelsofCUtraits.Futurestudiesarethereforeneededtoexaminedif- andCPwerestrongerforboyswithlowlevelsofCUtraits.However, ferential associations between dimensions of parenting (including itisdifficulttointerprettheresults,asonlythreeofsixmodelstested parentalaffect)andreactiveversusproactiveaggressionatdifferent included significant interaction terms. Second, Oxford et al. (2003) levelsofCUtraits. testedwhetherCUtraitsmoderatedassociationsbetweenineffective TwostudiesinvestigatedwhetherCUtraitsmoderatedlongitudi- parenting and CP in aggressive children (N=199), attempting to nal associations between parenting and AB. First, Pardini et al. replicateandextendthefindingsofWoottonetal.FourCPoutcomes (2007)investigatedpredictorsofCUtraitsandABinasampleofag- weretested(twodifferentteacherreportsofexternalizingbehavior, gressivechildrenoveroneyear(ages9–12years;N=120).Asone parent-reportedexternalizingbehavior,andpeer-nominatedaggres- of several analyses (see earlier), Pardini et al. tested the prediction sion)usingdichotomous(highvs.low)andcontinuousCUtraitmea- ofCPbyinteractionsbetweenvariousparentingdimensionsandCU sures. CU traits were always related to CP. The interaction term traits. No interaction terms were significant. Second, Kroneman betweenCUtraits(dichotomous)andineffectiveparentingwasonly et al. (2011) tested whether CU traits interacted with parenting to significant in predicting peer-nominated aggression score (one of predict five-year CD/ODD symptom trajectories in girls (N=1233, four models). The interaction term between CU traits (continuous) ages7–8)frommiddlechildhoodtoearlyadolescence,following-up and ineffective parenting was a significant predictor of teacher- on the cross-sectional study of Hipwell et al. (2007). Low parental reported externalizing and peer-nominated aggression scores (two warmth was more strongly associated with higher initial CD/ODD offourmodels).Despitethefactthatmoremodelsthannotfeatured symptoms and faster decreasing CD/ODD symptoms for girls with non-significantinteractionterms,theauthorsarguedthattheresults high levels of CU traits. However, by year five, the interaction be- provided partial support for CU traits moderating associations be- tweenlowparentalwarmthandCUtraitswasnolongersignificant. tweenparentingandCP. Nevertheless, parental warmth may be important in protecting Third,Hipwelletal.(2007)investigatedthemoderatingeffectsofCU againstthedevelopmentofABforgirlswhoshowhighCUtraits,par- traits on associations between CP and parenting in girls (7–8years; ticularlyduringmiddlechildhood. N=990).Inregressionanalysis,controllingfordemographic factors andCP,CUtraitswerenotdirectlyrelatedtoharshpunishmentorlow 3.3.3.Adolescence(ages13–18) warmth (design was different to other studies as parenting was the Onestudymeetingtheinclusioncriteriainvestigatedthisquestion outcome). However, the interaction between CU traits and CP was inadolescence.Muñoz,Pakalniskiene,andFrick(2011)testedbidirec- significantlyrelatedtoharshparentingandlowparentalwarmth.The tionalrelationsbetweendimensionsofparenting(control,knowledge, magnitude of the association between CP and parenting decreased andsolicitation)andABoverayear,usingcross-laggedpanelmodels with increasing levels of CU traits (although remained significant in (N=98).Ofrelevancetothereview,multi-groupmoderatoranalysis eachcase).Fourth,FalkandLee(2011)investigatedwhetherCUtraits (highvs.lowCUtraits)ofcross-laggedmodelswasconducted.Higher moderated associations between parenting and AB in children with levelsofparentalcontrolpredictedincreasesinknowledge,andhigher and without ADHD (6–9years; N=208). For children with low/ solicitation predicted increased control in the low CU trait group. meanlevelsofCUtraits,lowerpositiveparentingwasassociatedwith Having less knowledge led to increased parental control for youth parent-reported(butnotteacher-reported)CDsymptoms,controlling withlowCUtraits,whereaslessknowledgeledtodecreasedcontrol forADHD(oneoffourmodels).Therewere,however,nosignificantin- for youthwith high CU traits. This pattern of associations highlights teractionsbetweennegativeparentingandCUtraitsforparentorteach- theimportanceofyouthcharacteristics(e.g.,willingnesstolie)tofuture erreportedODDorCDsymptoms(threeoffourmodels). parenting,whichmaycontributetoincreasingABovertime. Fifth,Pasalichetal.(2011)investigatedwhetherCUtraitsmoder- atedassociationsbetweenobservedparentalcoercionversusparental 3.3.4.Summary warmth(codedfromspeechsamples)andCPinclinic-referredboys IthasbeenhypothesizedthatyouthwithhighlevelsofCUtraitsare (3–10years;N=95).Formothersandfathers,coercionwasrelated lessinfluencedbynegativedimensionsofparenting(includingharsh toCPinboyswithlowCUtraits.Incontrast,maternalwarmthwasas- discipline and coercion) because of punishment insensitivity (Blair, sociatedwith fewerCP at high/mean levelsof CU traits,suggesting ColledgeandMitchell,2001),reducedresponsivitytonegativestimuli that warmth could be a specific buffer against the development of (Blair, Colledge, Murray, et al., 2001), and physiological hypoarousal ABforboyswithhighlevelsofCUtraits.Finally,inalarge,represen- (e.g., Marsh et al.,2008). Despite appearing tobe a well-established tativesample(N=1210),Yehetal.(2011)foundthatnegativeand findingintheliterature,thereviewsuggeststhatevidenceforCUtraits positiveparentalaffectinteractedwithyouthpsychopathyscoresto moderating associationsbetween dimensions ofparenting and AB is predict reactive and proactive aggression. Low levels of positive mixed.Forexample,offivelongitudinalstudies,threefoundevidence parentalaffectandhighlevelsofnegativeparentalaffectwereassoci- of moderation by CU traits and two did not. Of the seven cross- atedwithhigherlevelsofreactiveaggression,butonlyatlowlevelsof sectionalstudies,fivereportedsomemoderation(butnotinallmodels psychopathictraits.Incontrast,higherlevelsofnegativeparentalaf- tested)andonereportedmoderationindifferentdirections(associa- fect were associated with more proactive aggression, but only at tionbetweenparentingandCPwasathighorlowlevelsofCUtraits mean or high levels of psychopathic traits. These interactions were dependingonthedimensionofparentingorABassessed).Thereview similarwhethertotalpsychopathyscoreoraffectsubscalescore(sim- therefore firstchallengesthehighlycitednotioninthefieldthatCU ilartoCUtraitscale)wasused(K.C.Jacobson;personalcommunica- traits develop independently of parenting when this study design is tion, March 2012). The moderating effect of psychopathic traits on adopted.Forexample,inthetwomostcommonlycitedpapersinvesti- associations between parenting and AB is similar to the findings of gatingthequestionofmoderationbyCUtraits(Woottonetal.,313cita- other studies for the outcome of reactive aggression. However, the tions;Oxfordetal.,103citations,November2012),neitheraddresses 602 R.Walleretal./ClinicalPsychologyReview33(2013)593–608 questionsrelatingtothedevelopmentofCUtraitsperse;bothstudies parenting may be important concomitants. For example, a parent arecross-sectionalandhaveABastheoutcome.Furthermore,inboth whohasfosteredawarmrelationshipduringearly/middlechildhood studies,interaction termsbetweenCUtraitsandparentingaremore couldbe more effectivein monitoring duringadolescence,and it is frequentlynon-significant. uncleartowhatextentathird,unobservedvariable,suchasashared Second,methodologicalcaveatsforstudiesthathaveinvestigated geneticliabilityforlowwarmth,maybetterexplaintheassociations. thequestionofCUtraitsasacross-sectionalmoderatorareparticularly Atthesametime,thesharperincreaseinself-reporteddelinquency relevanttoanyconclusionsthatcanbedrawn.Allfivecross-sectional of youth with high CU traits reported by Muñoz et al. indicates a studiesthatfoundevidenceformoderationbyCUtraitsreliedonparent needforabetterunderstandingofthestrategiesparentscouldemploy reportsofparentingandparentreportasamajorpartoftheirmeasure tomanageAB. of CU traits (e.g., Koglin & Petermann, 2008; Oxford et al., 2003; Finally,aless consistentpictureemergesfromstudiesthat have Woottonetal.,1997).Asalreadyoutlined,itisnotclearhowaparent's investigated positive dimensions of parenting. Two studies suggest ratingoftheirownparentingstrategies(includingeffectivediscipline, that lowpositive parentingisrelatedto CPforyouthwithhighCU warmth,orpositiveparent–childinteractions)mightimpactontheir traits(Kronemanetal.,2011;Pasalichetal.,2011).Itisnoteworthy perceptionoftheirchild'saffectiveandsocioemotionalcharacteristics thatasimilarpatternofresultswasfoundinthesetwostudiesthat (and vice versa). Furthermore, several studies are limited by having separately assessed boys and girls. The results also suggest that, in lowalphasfortheirmeasuresofCUtraits(Falk&Lee,2011;Hipwell middlechildhoodatleast,childrenwithhighlevelsofCUtraitsmay etal.,2007;Kronemanetal.,2011)orparenting(Pardinietal.,2007; beespeciallyresponsivetoaparent–childrelationshipcharacterized Yeh et al., 2011). It is also difficult to generalize from studies that by positive affect and warmth, which may serve to promote their assessed small, clinic-based samples (Pasalich et al., 2011; Wootton emotional responding and internalization of values (Pasalich et al., et al., 1997). Methodologically stronger studies had large samples 2011) and protect against the development of AB. In contrast, Yeh (Hipwelletal.,2007;Hydeetal.,2013)andassessedparentingversus etal.(2011)foundthatlowerpositiveparentalaffectwasrelatedto CU traits using different methods (Hyde et al., 2013; Pasalich et al., reactive aggression in children with low levels of CU traits. Lower 2011;Yehetal.,2011)tominimizesharedmethodvariance. levelsofpositiveparentingwerealsorelatedtoCPonlyatlowlevels Taking into account strengths and limitations of studies, and ofCUtraitsintwoclinic-referredsamples(Falk&Lee,2011;Wootton focusingonlyonmodelsthatincludedsignificantinteractions,there etal.,1997),althoughthemethodologicalissuesoftheselattertwo is some evidence that negative dimensions of parenting are cross- studies make the results difficult to interpret. Finally, two studies sectionallyrelatedtoABinyouthwithlowbutnothighlevelsofCU thatassessedwhetherCUtraitsmoderatedalongitudinalassociation traits.Thispatternemergedforinconsistentdisciplineinpreschoolers betweenpositiveparentingandCPdidnotreportsignificantinterac- (Koglin & Petermann, 2008), and negative affect (Yeh et al., 2011), tions(Hydeetal.,2013;Pardinietal.,2007).Furtherempiricalwork harshness(Hipwelletal.,2007),ineffectiveparenting(Oxfordetal., isthereforeneededtounderstandassociationsbetweenpositivedi- 2003; Wootton et al., 1997) and coercion (Pasalich et al., 2011) in mensionsofparentingandCPatvaryinglevelsofCUtraits.Aswith middle-childhood.Thispatternoffindingsfoundinstudies,compris- negativeparenting,futurestudieswouldbenefitfromincreasedpre- ing community, high risk, aggressive, and clinic-referred samples cision in operationalization of parenting, and from making specific of different ages, suggests that similar mechanisms may underlie predictionsabouttheimportanceofdifferentparentingpracticesat cross-sectional associations between parenting, CU traits, and AB differentages. acrossyouth. However,recentstudiescomplicatethepicture.First,Yehandcol- 3.4. Does parenting differ between subgroups of youth categorized leaguesfoundthatnegativeparentalaffectwasassociatedwithreac- accordingtotheirlevelofABandCUtraits?(Fig.1c) tiveaggressionatlowlevelsofCUtraits,butproactiveaggressionat high levels of CU traits. This finding suggests that future studies Studiesaddressingthisquestiontestavariationofthemoderator might benefit from separating different forms of aggression when questionoutlinedabove.Studiescreategroupsusingcutoffscoreson measuringAB.Atthesametime,theoverlapbetweenconceptualiza- CUtraitsandABquestionnaires(althoughbydefinition,thelevelof tionsandmeasurementofproactiveaggressionandCUtraitsneeds ABmaybehighacrosscertainsamples).Themostcommongroups furtherconsideration.However,aparticularstrengthofthisstudyis created are a high AB and high CU trait group (AB+CU+), a high the different reporters for proactive aggression (youth self-report) AB and low CU trait group (AB+CU−), and a group with low AB versusCUtraits(parent-report),reducingtheriskofsharedmethod andCUtraits(control;AB−CU−).Studiestestforsignificantdiffer- variance.Theresultsalsosuggestthattheremaybeimportantetiolog- encesinparenting(assessedprospectivelyorcross-sectionallyinre- icaldifferencesin the developmentof proactiveversusreactiveag- lation to CU traits/AB) between groups at one time point or across gressionthatmayberelatedtobothparentingandthepresenceof, timepoints(i.e.,basedonstabilityofCUtraits/AB). oroverlapwith,CUtraits.Forexample,throughsociallearningpro- cesses,childrenwithhighlevelsofCUtraitsmaylearntovalueaggres- 3.4.1.Earlychildhood/preschool(ages3–6) sivestrategiestoobtainrewardfollowingexposuretoaggressiverole Nostudieshaveinvestigatedthisquestioninearlychildhood/pre- models. Second,whileKronemanetal.(2011)didnotfindevidencethat schoolsamples(althoughseeBarkeretal.,2011). harsh parenting predicted AB differently in girls with low versus highlevelsofCUtraits,theyspeculatedthatthedevelopmentalperiod 3.4.2.Middle/latechildhood(ages7–12) inwhichharshparentinginfluencesbehaviorisearlierthanthatcap- Four studies have assessed parenting between groups of youth turedbytheageoftheirsample.Thispointhighlightsthatfewofthe classified according to AB/CU traits in middle childhood. Enebrink, includedstudiesmadepredictionsaboutwhichdimensionsofnega- Andershed,andLangstrom(2005)cross-sectionallytestedassociations tiveparentingmaybespecificallyrelevantatdifferentages.Forexam- between parenting factors and CU trait groupmembership in clinic- ple,parentalharshnesscouldbemoreimportanttoABdevelopment referredboys(N=41;ABhighacrosssample).Parentingwasassessed inyoungchildren,whereaspoorparentalmonitoringmaybeamore via interview and evaluation of case records. The AB+CU+ group salient dimension to investigate among adolescents with different (n=13)wasmorelikelytohaveexperiencedpoorhouseholdcircum- levels of CU traits. Indeed, Muñoz et al. (2011) found that among stancesandhighfamilystress(whichremainedsignificantaftercon- adolescentswithhighlevelsofCUtraits,parentswithlessknowledge trollingforODD/CDsymptoms),butdidnotdifferonothermeasures decreased their control over time. However, other dimensions of (including little caregiver continuity and ineffective parenting).

Description:
Blair, Moffitt, & Plomin, 2005), associated with differences in neural re- activity (e.g. The search strategy combined terms to identify studies investigating. CU traits OR girl* OR infant* OR juvenile* OR preadolescen* OR pre-adolescen* No date, publication, or language restrictions were im- po
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.