ebook img

Visual Rhythms in Stomatopod Crustaceans Observed in the Pseudopupil PDF

10 Pages·1992·4.5 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Visual Rhythms in Stomatopod Crustaceans Observed in the Pseudopupil

Reference: Biol. Bull 182: 278-287. (April, 1992) Visual Rhythms in Stomatopod Crustaceans Observed in the Pseudopupil THOMAS W. CRONIN Department ofBiological Sciences, University ofMaryland Baltimore County, Baltimore. Maryland 21228 Abstract. Many aspects of visual function in animals terns also express rhythmicity in membrane shedding are influenced by the operation ofendogenous rhythms. (Nassel and Waterman, 1979; Horridge et al. 1981; Wil- Usingtechniques ofintracellular optical physiology, I in- liams, 1982) or preparation for shedding (Chamberlain vestigated visual rhythms in two species of stomatopod and Barlow, 1984). Visual rhythms apparently unique to ERG crustaceans (mantis shrimps): Sqiiilla empusa. a species invertebrates include cyclic changes in amplitude active throughout the day and night, and Gonodactylm (e.g., Arechiga and Wiersma, 1969; Page and Larimer, oerstedii, which isstrictlydiurnal. Reflectance from within 1975; Barlow, 1983;FleissnerandFleissner, 1985),inrates thedeeppseudopupil ofthecompoundeyesanditschange ofaction potential production (Jacklet, 1969), and, par- upon stimulation with lightwere monitored in individual ticularly in arthropods, migration of screening pigment animals in constant conditions foruptotwoweeks. Both in secondary pigment cells(Welsh, 1930; Kleinholz, 1937; species expressed circadian rhythms in visual function. Page and Larimer, 1975; see reviews ofStavenga, 1979, In S. empusa, the pupillary response was much stronger and Autrum, 1981). duringsubjective night; littleornoresponsecould beelic- In many cases, circadian changes in sensitivity aredue itedduringsubjective day. In thisspecies, an endogenous primarily to variations in the quantum catch by the pho- rhythm caused pupillary reflectance to increase during toreceptor cells, produced by alterations either in asso- subjective day. Rhythms in G. oerstedii were of lower ciated structures such as secondary pigment cells or in amplitude than in S. empusa and were more difficult to the amount ofphotoreceptor membrane per cell. But in detect. Thedifferencesbetweenthesespecies,togetherwith some species, the actual photoreceptor cells can undergo the results of other comparative research on visual rhythmic changes that affect their ability to respond to rhythms in arthropods, suggest that circadian, rhythmic the capture of a photon by rhodopsin. For example, in processes are involved in optimizing nocturnal eyes for Limuluspolyphemus. circadian events alter electrophys- maximum sensitivity and dynamic range. iological properties of individual photoreceptor cells (Kaplan and Barlow, 1980; Barlow etal., 1987; Kassand Introduction Renninger, 1988). Endogenous rhythms in visual function are common The photoreceptorcells ofmany arthropod species are independentlycapableofadjustingtheirsensitivitytolight amonganimals. Diverse rhythmicphenomenaassociated by mobilizing granules ofprimary pigment, producing a with vision occurin both vertebrateand invertebratespe- phenomenon known asthe pupillary response(Kirschfeld cies. Forexample, in the vertebrates, events known to be and Franceschini, 1969; see review of Stavenga, 1979). under endogenous control include photoreceptor mem- Arthropod pupillary responses may be observed nonin- brane shedding (LaVail, 1976), retinomotor movements vasivelyby monitoringlight reflected fromthedeeppseu- (reviewed in Levinson and BurnsmidRe,NA1981; Burnside and dopupilofthecompoundeye(Stavengaand Kuiper, 1977; Nagle, 1983), and synthesis of coding for opsin Bernard and Stavenga, 1979; Cronin, 1989); as the pig- (Korenbrot and Fernald, 1989i Invertebrate visual sys- ment granules migrate inwards in response to photic stimulation, reflectance rises. Circadian changes in pseu- Received24October 1991; accepted 13January 1992. dopupillaryappearanceandlevelofreflectancehavebeen 278 STOMATOPOD VISUAL RHYTHMS 279 observed in many arthropod compound eyes (Stavenga, ofthe eye (see photographs in Cronin, 1986, and Cronin, 1977; see review ofStavenga, 1979). The rhythmsare ap- 1989)wascenteredwithin the fieldofviewofan incident- parently due to circadian events in secondary pigment light, photometric microscope. The entire apparatus was cells, under nervous (Page and Larimer, 1975) or neu- housed in a dark box with a black curtain covering the roendocrine (Smith, 1948; Page and Larimer, 1975; Her- front, and experiments took place in a room that was nandez-Falcon et ai, 1987) control. True pupillary re- completely blacked out and isolated from external sources sponses, however, are caused by translocations ofprimary oflight.Thecentral regionofthepseudopupil understudy, pigments, within the actual photoreceptor cells, in direct which appeared to glow dully when viewed by eye, was responsetophoticstimulation (Stavenga, 1979). Dothese isolated using an adjustable field diaphragm. Reflectance responses also express circadian rhythms? Ifso, the eyes from the pseudopupil was monitored as described in may be optimized for sensitivity and dynamic range at a Cronin (1989), using light of wavelengths >720 nm particularphaseofthedielcycle, underrhythmiccontrol. (Schott RG720 longpass filter, usedwithSquilla) or>800 In earlier work with the squilloid stomatopod crusta- nm (Schott RG800 longpass filter, used with Gonodac- cean Squilla empitsa, I found that it was difficult or im- tylus); thissource oflight illuminated the eye continually possible to elicit any changes in reflection from the deep throughout each experiment, and by itself caused no pseudopupil during the day, whereas nocturnal stimula- measurable pupillary response. At20-or30-min intervals, tionproduced large, highlyrepeatablereflectanceincreases a stimulating exposure automatically was provided, pro- (Cronin, 1989). Incontrast, thegonodactyloidstomatopod duced by passing light from a 150-W Xenon arc through species Gonodactylus oerstedii and Pseitdosquilla ciliata a monochromator (Oriel 7250 with 500-nm blazed grat- expressed pupillary responses no matter when they were ing), counter-rotating 10-cm diameter neutral density stimulated. In this report, I describe experiments testing wedges, and a linear polarizing filter. All stimuli were whetherthereisarhythmiccomponent to pupillary func- confined to the ommatidia contributing to the pseudo- tion in S. empusa and G. oerstedii. The results suggest pupil under study, and were at a wavelength of500 nm that rhythmic events strongly alterthe pupillary responses (halfbandwidth of 10 nm). Theywereproduced byopen- ofS. empusa and have a weaker influence on those ofG. ing a Uniblitz electromagnetic shutter under the control oerstedii. ofamicrocomputer, and lastedfor5 min(Squillaempusa) or 30 s (Gonodactylus oerstedii). Measurements of light Materials and Methods reflected from the pseudopupil commenced before each exposure and continued until well afterwards, and data Adultanimalswereused inallexperiments. Workwith were stored on the microcomputer's hard disk for later Squilla empusa was carried out at the Duke University analysis. The response for each stimulus was defined as Marine Laboratory in Beaufort, NorthCarolina. Animals the average reflectance during the final 20% ofthe stim- were collected locally and maintained either in running ulation's duration, divided by the average reflectance be- seawater tables exposed to indirect, natural daylight fore stimulus onset and following a period in the dark through windows along two sides ofthe room (ambient equal totheduration ofthestimulusitself(seealsoCronin photoperiod experiments) orin small containerscontain- and King, 1989). Responses are plotted asthe percentage ingnatural seawaterplaced in achamberwith acontrolled change in reflectance relative to the average dark levels. light:dark cycle (reversed photoperiod experiments). An- In some cases, the sensitivity ofthe pupillary response imals were fed fresh oysterand shrimp meat. Experiments was measured by providing a series ofstimulating inten- with Gonodactylusoerstediitook placein Baltimore, using sities over a range of 3.5 to 3.8 density units at steps of animalscollected in the Florida Keys. Theseanimalswere 0.5 units. Each serieswas produced under microcomputer kept in aquaria filled with artificial seawater in a 12 h:12 control, by rotatingthe neutral density wedges to a series h light:dark cycle, and were fed frozen shrimp. of preprogrammed settings. The intensity ofthe stimu- Reflectance from thedeeppseudopupil was monitored lating source was measured for each experiment using a using the techniques of intracellular optical physiology, calibrated PIN-10DP/SB photodiode (United Detector described in detail in Bernard and Stavenga (1979) and Technology) placed at the position ofthe animal's eye. Cronin (1989). Dorsal surfaces ofanimalswere attached, usingScutan dental plastic, toa moveable platform which Results wasthen submersed in seawater. Duringeach experiment, water in the experimental chamber (which contained Earlierwork with Squilla empusa hadrevealedthatthe about 1200 ml) was changed occasionally, at irregular ability ofan animal to express pupillary responsesappar- times. Animals were not fed during an experiment. entlyvariedwith time. Resultsofanexperimentdesigned Once mounted, each experimental animal wasaligned to detect rhythms in responsiveness are shown in Figure sothatthe pseudopupil ofeitherthedorsal or ventral half 1. The animal, a male of body length (rostrum-telson) 280 T. W. CRONIN Concurrently, baseline pseudopupillary reflectance de- creased by up to 50%. Near the time ofsubjective dawn, thechange in reflectanceduringstimulationdropped once more to near zero, again over a period of 1 to 2 h, while the baseline reflectance quickly rose to its daytime level. This rhythmical pattern was typical ofthat expressed in animalsmaintained undertheambient photoperiod. The period ofthe rhythm was estimated using Enright's per- iodogram technique (Enright, 1965); both rhythms had periods near 24 h (see Fig. 1; the difference between the twoperiodsismeaninglesswithtimeseriesofthislength). It is conceivable that the observed rhythms in baseline reflectance and responsivenesswereexpressionsofasingle phenomenon; the pupillary mechanism could rhythmi- cally assume its fully light-adapted state during the day, thus increasing reflectance and losing its ability to adapt further to light. Two types ofobservations argue against this interpretation ofthe data. First, the two rhythms did not haveexactlyinverse forms. Baselinereflectancetended tochange lessabruptlythan didthe level ofresponse, and on somedaysitschangeswere notprecisely in phasewith Day the changes in response level. More convincingly, some Figure 1. Lightreflectancefromthedeeppseudopupil(Reflectance; animals expressed rhythms in responsiveness with little top panel)and percentage change in reflectance on stimulation (% Re- or no circadian change in the baseline reflectance. For sponse; bottom panel) in an adult male individual ofSquilla empusa example, in the experiment of Figure 2, the baseline re- maintainedinconstantconditions.Measurementsweremadeat30-min intervals for a total of327 intervals, from 7 to 14 July 1987. Stimuli were at 500 nm, at a quantal intensity of2.9 X 10" quanta cm"2 s~'. The reflectancewasmeasured asdescribed in thetext for 1 min before 100 each5-minstimulation;averagevaluesareplottedonascalenormalized .Vn) 80 tothelargestvalueobtained. Percentresponsewascalculatedasdescribed o C in the text. Vertical lines are drawn at successive midnights. The light and dark bands on the abscissa represent times ofnatural sunrise and sunset(Eastern DaylightTime). Theperiod(T)ofeach rhythm, in h, is giveninthetoprightcornerofitspanel. Eachtimeserieswasanalyzed using Enright's periodogram technique (Enright, 1965). Penodogram amplitudes were computed at 0.1-h intervals for periods from 10 h to 30 h. The value given on the graph is that ofthe period in this 20-h range havingthegreatest amplitude. 85 mm,was placed in constant darkness in the apparatus at midday on the first day and stimulated each 30 min for the next 7 days. The results ofthe experiment ofFigure 1 aretypical of those of most experiments. Rhythmical variations oc- curred both in the level ofthe pupillary response and in the reflectance from the pseudopupil in the absence of stimulation. During the subjective day, pseudopupillary reflectance remained high, and little or no measurable reflectance change occurred in response to the light stim- ulus the variations that were observed were due to ap- parently random fluctuations. However, nearthe time of natural sunset, reflectance from the deep pseudopupil di- minished, andlargeincreasesin reflectanceoccurred upon stimulation. Within 1 io 2 h, the reflectance rise during stimulation changed from near zero to greaterthan 20%. STOMATOPOD VISUAL RHYTHMS 281 flectance varied only slightly over four days. The only The rhythms of responsiveness in these experiments changeswere a small increase during first dark phase and retained phases that appeared to be tightly linked to the transient changes on each successive dusk and dawn. externaldiel cycle, andthereremained thepossibilitythat These results strongly suggest that the ability ofthe pu- external timing cues were reaching the experimental an- pillary mechanism itselfto respond to light varies rhyth- imals. This possibility was tested by placing an animal in mically. Changes in baseline reflectance could not result anisolatedchambersubjectedtoalight:darkcyclephased fromarhythmical responsivenesstotheconstant 720-nm differently from the ambient cycle ofsunrise and sunset; lightusedtomonitorreflectance, forsucharhythmshould lights were switched on at 1800 (Eastern Daylight Time) produce increasing pseudopupillary reflectance at night, and offat 0600 each day. Following 12 days ofexposure when responsiveness isgreatest. I therefore conclude that to this "inverted" photoperiod, an animal was placed in although the two rhythms observed in Figure 1 may be constant conditions. Rhythms both of the pupillary re- intimately linked, they are expressions ofseparate events sponse and the baseline reflectance were now observed to within the ommatidia. Once again, periodogram analysis be in phase with the imposed cycle. Responsiveness in- suggests that the rhythms ofFigure 2 are circadian. Per- creased, and baseline reflectance decreased, during the iodogram analysisisparticularlyunreliablefortimeseries entrained dark period between 6 am and 6 pm. To avoid as short as that ofFigure 2, but inspection ofthe forms repeatedly stressing the animal by mounting it for study, oftherhythmsclearlyconfirmsthattheirperiodsare near itsvisual rhythmswere notexperimentallydefined before 24 h. imposingthe reversed photoperiod. Nevertheless, thiswas The onset ofresponsivenessatdusk occursbyasmooth the only case in which maximum responses were ever transition(Fig. 3). Although successive responsesincrease observed during the astronomical day, so it is reasonable in size, the time each takes to reach its plateau phase is to conclude that the rhythms had been entrained by the similar. It is not primarily the rate ofthe response that exposureto the"inverted" photoperiod. Such resultsalso alters,therefore,buttheactualamplitudeofthereflectance demonstrate that the rhythms observed in the earlier ex- change. The increasing sizes ofthe responses during the periments were not in response to exogenous cues. As dusktransition mimictheincreasingresponsesthat occur before, the periods ofthe rhythms were very near 24 h with increasing intensity, when theeye is maximally sen- (Fig. 4). sitive (Cronin, 1989). The dusk transition, therefore, ap- Response-mros-intensity functions were obtained pears to be a time ofrapidly increasing sensitivity ofthe from the individuals studied in Figures 2 and 4 during pupillary mechanism. both the subjective night and the subjective day. The o Q. u Oi figure 3. Changes in reflectance from the deep pseudopupil ofthe animal used in the experiment of Figure 2. during the onset ofthe nocturnal increase in responsiveness. The 6 traces were obtained at 30- min intervals, beginning at 1535 EOT (lowest trace) and ending at 1805 (highest trace) on 19 July 1987. Percent response iscomputed relativetotheaverage reflectanceinthe 1 minpriortostimulation. Vertical linesaredrawn atthe beginning(0 min)andend (5 min) ofthe stimulation interval. 282 T. W. CRONIN 100 Day Figure4. Resultsobtained froma femaleSquilla empusamaintainedinconstantconditionsfrom I to 1 1 July 1988. Priorto theexperiment, the animal was kept in a controlled light:dark cycle asdescribed in thetext;thelightanddark bandsontheabscissaindicatethetimesoflightanddarkoftheentrainingcycle. Arrowsindicate timesofthe beginningsofseriesduring which stimulation wasincreased duringeach half hour. Atall othertimes, stimulationwasataquantal intensity of 1.02 X 10" quantacnT:s~'.Gapsin the record indicatetimesofmissingdatadue toequipment failure;otherwiseasin Figure 1. functions were obtained by stimulating the eye on 8 or(| ticular diel variation in the pupillary responses was no- successive 30-min intervalswith stimuli increasingatstep; ticed. Nevertheless, it seemed likely that gonodactyloids of0.5 logunits, ultimatelyprovidingamaximum quantal could possess rhythmssimilartothose ofSquilla, butper- intensity of 3.44 X 10'2 quanta cirT2 s~' (experiment of haps more subtlein theirform. Thispossibilitywastested Fig. 2) or 2.62 X 1013 quanta cm"2 s"' (experiment of with Gonodactylusoerstedii, usinganoverallexperimental Fig. 4). design like that employed with Squilla empusa, except All nighttime series produced response-vm;-intensity that in these cases entrainment was imposed entirely by functions ofsimilar shape, with maximal reflectance in- artificial cycles oflight and dark. The pupillary response creases near 13% (Fig. 5A) or45% (Fig. 5B). In contrast, is much more rapid in G. oerstedii than in S. empusa, so thedaytime response level remained near0, risingat most I used briefer, more frequent stimuli in these experiments. to about 3% of the height of the nighttime peak at the Resultsoftwoexperiments, representing the range ofob- maximum stimulation intensity. It appears, in fact, that served experimental outcomes, are displayed in Figures the maximum pupillary response that can be generated 6 and 7. is greatly reduced during the day. To test this conjecture In the experiment of Figure 6, reflectance from the would require stimuli far more intense than what was pseudopupil priortostimulation showedaclearcircadian available in these experiments. The highest intensities to rhythm. Here, the form was rather different from what which the animal was exposed during these series were was obtained using S. empusa. Soon after the expected up to 100 times the usual test exposure; somewhat sur- time oflights-out, reflectance from the deep pseudopupil prisingly, these produced no obvious phase shifts in sub- slowly increased, ultimately becomingabout 10% greater sequent cycles ofthe rhythms (Figs. 2 and 4). than during the subjective day. Beginning near midday In the earlier work with gonodactyloid stomatopod in the entrainment cycle, this elevated reflectance once species (Cronin, 1989; Cronin and King, 1989), no par- moredeclined. A rhythm in pupillary responsivenesswas STOMATOPOD VISUAL RHYTHMS 283 12 10 o D. -3 -2 Log Intensity Figure 5. Response-vtTOu-intensity functions obtained during the experiments ofFigure 2 (A) and Figure4(B).Functionswereobtainedatthetimesindicatedbyarrowsonthosefigures;largecirclesindicate resultsobtainedduringsubjectivenightandsmallcircles,duringsubjectiveday.Opencirclescorrespondto thefirstseriesandclosedcirclestothesecond. Photicintensitiesarerelativeto3.44 x 1012quantacm"2s~' more difficult to detect, although on average, responses were more difficult to detect than in S. empusa, but they did appear to be slightly larger at subjective night. Per- could be observed in some individuals. iodogram analysis of the data suggests the presence of The rhythms that were observed were robust. They circadian cycles in both the baseline reflectance data and persisted and maintained their form, frequently with a the responsiveness data (Fig. 6). highamplitude, foraweekormoreinconstantconditions. The experiment of Figure 7 revealed the strongest The rhythms were clearly circadian; their periods were expression ofcircadian rhythms in responsiveness that I consistently revealed to be near 24 h eitherby inspection observed in over 20 experiments with G. om/tW//. Am- or by periodogram analysis ofthe data. The rhythm in plitudesofthepupillary responsewereabouttwiceaslarge responsiveness expressed by S. empusa was particularly during subjective day as during subjective night, thus impressive for its rapid rise each subjective evening and havingthe opposite form ofthoseexpressedby5. empma. its equally rapid fall in the morning. The pupillary response was present both during the day Duringtheday,thesensitivityofthepupillaryresponse and night. In this case, however, there was no evidence decreased by at least three orders of magnitude. These ofa circadian cycle in pupillary reflectance. changesaregreaterthan observed inLimuluspolyphemus. in which both the electroretinogram (ERG) and single- Discussion cell sensitivities decrease by about 1.5 log units (Barlow Asdemonstrated here, rhythmswithacircadian period et al., 1977, 1987). Otherarthropods, however, have sen- can readily be observed through observations of pseu- sitivity changes between the day and night states about dopupils in compound eyes ofstomatopod crustaceans. as large as those of S. empusa. In the crayfish Cherax In the squilloid species, Squilla empusa. two parallel destructor, dark-adapted single photoreceptor cells are rhythms ofhigh amplitude are usually observed: a cycle morethan two logunits more sensitiveat night(Bryceson, inbaselinereflectancefromthepseudopupilintheabsence 1986), while the simple eyes of scorpions rhythmically of stimulation, and a cycle in the amplitude ofthe pu- gain nearlyfourlogunitsofsensitivityeach night(review: pillary responsetolight stimulation. In someexperiments, Fleissner and Fleissner, 1985). In its natural habitat, 5. the gonodactyloid Gonodactylus oerstedii expressed the empusa probably lacks a pupillary response during the baseline reflectance rhythm, although at loweramplitude day. The maximum intensities of stimulation used in than5. empusa. Rhythmsinresponsivenessin G. oerstedii producing the response-irrws-intensity functions ofFig- 284 T. W. CRONIN _ 100 and Larimer, 1976), and no other extraretinal photore- ceptors have yet been described in stomatopods. If en- trainment is mediated solely by the compound eyes, the spatially limitedstimuli oftheseexperiments(whichwere restricted to the ommatidia of the pseudopupil of one part ofasingle eye) must havebeen insufficientto induce observable phase changes. Nevertheless, intense stimuli like those used to measure the response-vmzw-intensity functions produce no obvious phase shifts, implyingthat there may be a role for extraocular photoreception in rhythm entrainment in stomatopods. What underlying events take place within the com- pound eyes to bring about the rhythms observed in this study? Changes in the level ofbaseline reflectance can be effected in several ways (Stavenga, 1979). External to the receptor cells, these include reorganization of optical structures or associated pigment cells, movement ofpig- ment in secondary pigment cells, and masking or un- maskingofatapetum. Withinthephotoreceptors, changes in reflectance could be caused by alterations in rhabdom size or microvillar organization, or by events within the Figure 6. Results obtained from a female Gonodactylus oerstedii maintained in constant conditions from 22 to 28 January 1988. The 100 animal was kept in a controlled light:dark cycle, indicated by the light and dark bands on the abscissa, prior to the experiment. Stimulation wasat500nm,atintervalsof20min,andataquantalintensityof1.57 X 1012quantacm'2s~'. Otherwiseasin Figure 1. ure 4, on the order of 10" quanta cm 2 s ', are similar to intensities an animal would experience at a depth of onlyafewmetersinthecoastalwatersit inhabits(Forward el al.. 1988). During the daylight phase ofthe rhythm, such intensities produced no apparent response. Ifthese circadian rhythmsare to be properly phased to the diel cycle, they must be entrainable by cycles oflight and dark. Animals apparently do entrain completely to a novel light:dark cycle within 12 days, as suggested by the results of the experiment of Figure 4. Presumably, photoreceptors for this entrainment are either within the compound eyes or exist elsewhere in the animal. In fact, many invertebrate species entrain theircircadian rhythms usingextraocularpathways(see reviewofBennett, 1979). In crayfish, and probably other decapod crustaceans, photicentrainmentofcircadian rhythmscan beachieved by retinal illumination (Larimer and Smith, 1980), but maiFnitgauirneed7.incRoenssutlatnstcoobntdaiitnieodnsffrroomma31mJaalneuarGyontooda1c2tFyelbursuaoreyrs1t9e9d1i.i suchentrainment mayalso involve photoreceptorsofthe The animal was kept in a controlled lightidark cycle, indicated by the 6th abdominal ganglion (Fuentes-Pardo and Inclan-Ru- lightanddarkbandsontheabscissa,priortotheexperiment.Datafrom bio, 1987)orotherregionsoftheCNS(Pageand Larimer, the first two days ofthe experiment are not plotted, due to repeated op1fh9o7Pt6ao;greLeacaernpidtmoeLrrasrai(nmidnertShm(ei1t96h7t,6h)1ad9be8md0)oo.nmsiItnnraaplatregtdainctughllaaitro,nt)htehaecraewuodnraoklt aacrtenmfii~lmne2atcelstr"avnm1a.colePsv)eeorpfmiroe2od0ndoutmgcireanadn,mdaaaennsqaldluoyiwastlpiymsaeroqnifustaitnnhfgteaiapdllueartniean.otdoeSofntsgitirmhteuaylmuoapftwpii1eot.rnh97pwaaaxnssepl1ia0tk(e|p:5up0qpe0iualalnknamtra.ayt required for entrainment. In contrast to decapod crusta- 27.1 h;because the analysisproduced noclearmaximum, the valueot ceans, S. empusalacksthiscaudal photoreceptor(Wilkens r isnotgiven on the figure. Otherwiseasin Figure 1. STOMATOPOD VISUAL RHYTHMS 285 cytoplasm ofretinularcells, such as rearrangement ofthe concentration (see review ofFein and Payne, 1989), the perirhabdomal palisade or movements ofpigment gran- absence of the pupillary response during the day could ules. imply that electrophysiological responses of the photo- Ommatidial reorganization no doubt accounts for receptorsare also abolished at that time. However, this is much ofthe circadian change in the appearance of the unlikelytobethecase; Kirschfeldand Vogt(1980)showed pseudopupil ofLimuluspolyphemus(Slavenga, 1979), but that in fly photoreceptors, it is possible to block pigment more recent work suggests that the retinularcell pigment migration without changing retinal electrical responses. itselfmigrates under circadian control (Kier and Cham- In work with mutant flies, Lo and Pak (1981) also ob- berlain. 1990). Long-term studies ofanatomical changes served that electrophysiological responses could remain in stomatopod eyes do not exist. In work with the Med- in the absence of pigment migration. The processes of iterranean species Squilla mantis, SchifF(1974) stated that pigment translocation and membrane depolarization, duringdark adaptation, rhabdoms increase in length and while both calcium-dependent, are therefore not com- decreaseindiameter; simultaneously, thecrystallinecone pletely parallel. The diurnal loss ofpupillary responsive- contracts. Schonenberger (1977) also observed the con- ness in Squilla empusa could represent another example traction of the crystalline cone during dark adaptation, ofthedecouplingofthesetwoprocesses. Experimentsare and noted that the distal pigment cells reorganize at that desirable in which the electrical and pupillary responses time. Within the photoreceptorcells, granulesofprimary are monitored simultaneously in this species. pigment line up in neat rows, encircling the rhabdom as Despite having radically different anatomies, the com- it dark adapts. These changes occur during diurnal dark pound eyes ofS. empusa and Limulus polyphemus are, adaptation, following prolonged exposure to daylight, at to some extent, analogous in the functioning oftheir pu- times when Squilla empusa reveals no light-sensitive pu- pillary responses. Both express rhythmic changes in re- pillary responses and no evidence ofdark adaptation fol- flectance from the deep pseudopupil, and both show lowing stimulation. At present, it is uncertain whether changes in this reflectance only at night (see Stavenga, thesetwospeciesofSquilladifferintheirabilitytorespond 1979). Interestingly, demands on their visual systems to light during the day, whether the two sets of studies throughoutthecourseofeachday mayalsobeanalogous. involve different phenomena, or whether prolonged ex- Neither species is entirely inactive during the day, but posure to light during the day can in fact produce revers- mating ofLimulus, a behavior involving vision (Barlow ible light-adaptation via the pupillary mechanism. et al., 1982), occurs mostly during twilight or night (Bar- While cyclic restructuring ofommatidia or transloca- low et al., 1986). Indeed, nocturnal vision in Limulus is tion of secondary pigment could produce rhythmic extraordinary, enablingthedetectionofhigh-contrasttar- changes in baseline reflectance, events effecting majoral- gets under starlight (Barlow et al., 1982). Activity cycles terationsin the level ofthepupillary response must occur ofSquilla empusa have yet to be examined in the field, within the actual photoreceptor cells. In apposition eyes but in the laboratory, at least, this species is most active like those ofstomatopods, the pupillary response is pro- during the night (pers. obs.), and the congener, Squilla duced by radial translocation ofgranules ofprimary pig- mantis, isclearly nocturnal (Frogliaand Giannini, 1989). ment residingin the photoreceptors(Stavenga. 1979; King The ocular design of 5. empusa is that of a nocturnal andCronin, 1989). Reflectance changes may be produced animal (Cronin, 1986). It is plausible that the rhythms in in superposition compound eyes, however, by events in visual function described in this paper are characteristic secondarypigmentcells(Bernard elal., 1984;Weyrauther, physiological propertiesofa nocturnal compound eye. In 1986). Compared to the responses in stomatopod eyes, fact,throughout thearthropods, regardlessofeyetype, all these changes are slow and have considerable inertia high-amplitude rhythms in visual physiological function the process continues, or remains saturated, long after yet described are in nocturnal species. Besides Limulus, stimulation ceases. these include scorpions(simpleeyes, rhythm in ERG am- Migration of primary pigment is directly under the plitudeandsensitivity: reviewed in FleissnerandFleissner, control ofthe retinular cell, and, at least in crustaceans, 1985), crayfish (superposition compound eye, rhythm in isthought not to be influenced by hormones (Ludolph et ERG amplitude and sensitivity: Arechiga and Wiersma, al., 1973). In another arthropod, Limulus polyphemus, 1969; Page and Larimer, 1975; Bryceson, 1986), and ERG rhythmic neural input does influence the position ofpri- cockroach (apposition compound eye, rhythm in mary pigment (Kier and Chamberlain, 1990); similar amplitude: Wills et al., 1986). processes may act in crustacean eyes. The pupillary re- G. oerstedii, unlikeS. empusa. isactiveonly from dawn sponse requires the presence ofcalcium ions (Kirschfeld to dusk (Dominguez and Reaka, 1988). At evening twi- and Vogt, 1980; Frixione and Arechiga, 1981; Howard, light, it seals offthe entrance to its burrow and remains 1984). Since excitation ofarthropod photoreceptor cells enclosed all night. Its eyes are optimized for photopic is also dependent upon intracellular increases in calcium function; indeed, the specialized spectral receptor classes 286 T. W. CRONIN in itseyes require reasonably bright light to operate at all Cronin, T. W. 1986. Optical design and evolutionary adaptation in (Cronin and Marshall, 1989; see also Marshall et ai, crustacean compoundeyes.J. Crust. Biol. 6: 1-23. 1991). Because the eye is used primarily for vision when Crontihne,sTt.udWy.of19s8t9o.matAopppoldiccartuisotnacoefaninvtirsaicoenl.luJl.arCoopmtpi.calPhtyesciohln.iqAue1s64t:o photon capture is not limiting, strong rhythmic cycles in 737-750. visual function may be unnecessary. Cronin, T. W., and C. A. King. 1989. Spectral sensitivity ofvision in The expression of high-amplitude circadian visual the mantis shrimp, Gonodactylus oerstedii, determined using non- rhythms in a primarily nocturnal species like S. empusa, invasiveopticaltechniques. Biol Bull 176: 308-316. and theirabsence in thediurnal G. oerstedii, couldthere- Cronin, T. W., and N.J. Marshall. 1989. Multiplespectral classesof fore reveal fundamental differences in function between photoreceptorsin retinasofgonodactyloid stomatopodcrustaceans. J. Comp. Physiol A 166: 261-275. compound eyes designed for nocturnal (or continuous) Dominguez,J.H.,andM.Reaka. 1989. Temporalactivitypatternsin versus diurnal function. If so, the rhythms observed in reef-dwellingstomatopods: a test ofalternative hypotheses. / Exp. thisstudybymonitoringlightreflectance fromdeeppseu- Mar. Biol. Ecol. 117: 47-69. dopupilsareamanifestation ofmorepervasive underlying Enright, J. T. 1965. The search for rhythmicity in biological time- alterations that must occur to maintain visual function series.J. Theoret. Biol. 8:426-468. atdayand night. The study ofrhythmic cyclesofsensory Feinp,hoAt.o,raencedptRo.rsP:ayrnoele.s1o9f8c9a.lciPuhmoatnodtriannossdituocltitroinspihnosLpihmautel.usPpv.en1t7r1a-l function, and in particular their underlying significance 185 in Facets of Vision, D. G. Stavenga and R. C. Hardie, eds. and control, deserves more attention. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, NewYork. Acknowledgments Fleissner,G.,andG.Fleissner. 1985. Neurobiologyofacircadianclock in the visual system ofscorpions. Pp. 351-375 in Neurobiology of This material is based on work supported by the Na- Arachnids, F.G. Barth,ed. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. New York. tional Science Foundation under Grants No. BNS- Forward, R. B., Jr., T. W. Cronin, and J. K. Douglass. 1988. The 8518769 and BNS-8917183.1 thank D. G. Stavenga and visual pigments ofcrabs. II. Environmental adaptations. / Comp. L. Kass for their comments on an earlier version ofthe Physiol. A 162: 479-490. Frixione, E., and H. Arechiga. 1981. Ionic dependence ofscreening manuscript. pigment migrations in crayfish retinal photoreceptors. / Comp. Literature Cited Physiol 144: 35-43. Arechiga, H., and C. A. G. Wiersma. 1969. Circadian rhythm ofre- Froglia,C.,andS.Giannini. 1989. Fieldobservationsondielrhythms sponsivenessincrayfish visual units.J. Nenrobiol. 1: 71-85. in catchability and feeding ofSquilla mantis (L.) (Crustacea, Sto- Autrum, H. 1981. Light and darkadaptation in invertebrates. Pp. 1- matopoda) in the Adriatic Sea. Pp. 221-228 in Biology ofStoma- 91 inHandbookofSensoryPhysiology, Vol VII/6C. H.Autrum,ed. topods, E. A. Ferrero,ed. Mucchi, Modena. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, NewYork. Fuentes-Pardo, B.,andV. Inclan-Rubio. 1987. Caudalphotoreceptors Barlow, R.B.,Jr. 1983. Circadian rhythmsintheLimulusvisualsys- synchronizethecircadianrhythmsincrayfish. I.Synchronizationof tem./ Neurosci. 3: 856-870. ERG and locomotor circadian rhythms. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. Barlow, R. B., Jr., S. J. Bolanowski, Jr., and M. L. Brachman. 86A: 523-527. 1977. Efferentopticnervefibersmediatecircadian rhythmsin the Hernandez-Falcon,J., E.Moreno-Saenz,J.M. Farias,and B.Fuentes- Limuluseye. Science197: 86-89. Pardo. 1987. Role ofthe sinus gland in crayfish circadian rhyth- Barlow, R. B.,Jr., L.C. Ireland,and L. Kass. 1982. Vision hasarole micity. I. Pseudopupil circadian rhythm. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. inLimulusmatingbehavior. Nature296: 65-66. 87A: 111-118. Barls1ot9uw8d,7i.esR..C/iBr.Gc,eand.Jira.Pn,hryhEs.iyotlhKma8sp9l:iann3L,5i3m-Gu3.l7u8s.H.phoRteonrneicnegpetorr,s.aIn.dIntTr.aceSlaliutloa.r Horriindgsien,glGe.lAo.c,usJt.aDnudnimeacn,tainsdphLo.toMraercceepltjoar.s.19/81.Exp.AB2i4o-lho9u1:r c3y0c7l-e Barlow, R. B., Jr., M. K. Powers, H. Howard, and L. Kass. 322. 1986. Migration ofLimulus for mating: relation to lunar phase, Howard, J. 1984. Calcium enables photoreceptor pigment migration tideheight, and sunlight. Biol. Bull 171: 310-329. in a mutant fly. / Exp. Biol. 113:471-475. Bennett,M.F. 1979. Extraocularlightreceptorsandcircadianrhythms. Jacklet,J.W. 1969. Circadianrhythmofopticnerveimpulsesrecorded Pp. 641-663 in Handbook ofSensoryPhysiology. Vol. VH/6A. H. indarkness from isolatedeyeofAplysia. Science164: 562-563. Autrum, ed. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, New York. Kaplan, E., and R. B. Barlow, Jr. 1980. Circadian clock in Limulus Bernard. G. D., E. D. Owens, and A. V. Hurley. 1984. Intracellular brainincreasesresponseanddecreasesnoiseofretinalphotoreceptors. optical physiology ofthe eye ofthe moth Amyelois, J Exp Biol. Nature286: 393-395. 138: 155-179. Kass,L.,andG.H. Renninger. 1988. Circadianchangein functionof Berntairndul,arG.celDl.s,maenadsDu.reGd.iSntainvteancgta,.li1v9i7n9g.flieSspbeyctaranlospetnisciatlivmiteitehsoodf.re/- Limulusventral photoreceptors. I'isualNeurosci. 1: 3-11. Comp. Physiol 134: 95-107. Kier,C. K.,andS.C.Chamberlain. 1990. Dualcontrolsforscreening Bryceson, K. P. 1986. Diumalchangesinphotoreceptorsensitivityin pigment movement in photoreceptors ofthe Limulus lateral eye: areflectingsuperpositioneye. / Comp. Physiol. A 158: 573-582. circadian efferent input and light. VisualNeurosci. 4: 237-255. Burnside, B., and B. Nagle. 1983. Retinomotor movements ofpho- King,C. A.,and T.W.Cronin. 1989. Ultrastructuralevidenceforpu- toreceptors and retinal pigment epithelium: mechanisms and regu- pillary response in stomatopod photoreceptors: Cytoskeleton and lation.Pp.67-109inProgressinRetinalResearch, Vol2,N.Osborne orientation ofpigmentgranules. Invest. Ophthalmol I'isualSci. 30 andG. Chader, eds. Pergamon, Oxford, New York. (suppl):292. Chamberlain,S.C.,and R. B. Barlow,Jr. 1984. Transientmembrane Kirschfeld,K.,andN.Franceschini. 1969. EinMechanismuszurSteu- sheddinginLimulusphotoreceptors:controlmechanismsundernat- erungdesLichtfiussesinden Rhabdomeren desKomplexaugesvon ural lighting. J. Neurosci. 4: 2792-2810. Musca. Kvbernetik6: 13-22. STOMATOPOD VISUAL RHYTHMS 287 Kirsihit-Id, K.,andK.Vogt. 1980. Calciumionsandpigmentmigration Page, T. L. and J. L. Larimer. 1976. Extraretinal photoreception in in flyphotoreceptors. Naturwissenschaften67: 516-517. entrainmentofcrustacean circadian rhythms. Phoiochem. Photobiol. Kleinholz, L. H. 1937. Studiesin thepigmentarysystemofCrustacea. 23:245-251. II.Diurnalmovementsoftheretina]pigmentsofBermudandecapods. Schiff,H. 1974. AdiscussionoflightscatteringintheSquillarhabdom. Bio/ Bull 72: 176-189. Kybernetik 14: 127-134. Korenbrot,J.1.,andR. D.Fernald. 1989. Circadianrhythm andlight Schonenberger, N. 1977. The fine structure ofthe compound eye of regulateopsin mRNA in rod photoreceptors. Nature337:454-457. Squillamantis(Crustacea,Stomatopoda). CellTissueRes. 176:205- Larimer,J. L.,andJ.T.F.Smith. 1980. Circadian rhythm ofretinal 233. sensitivityincrayfish: modulation bythecerebralandopticganglia. Smith, R. I. 1948. Theroleofthesinusglandsin retinal pigment mi- J. Comp. Physiol 136: 313-326. gration in grapsoidcrabs. Bio/. Bull. 95: 169-185. LaVail,M.1976. Rodoutersegmentdiscsheddinginrelationtocyclic Stavenga, D.G. 1977. Opticsofcompoundeyesandcircadianpigment lighting. Exp. EyeRes 23: 277-280. movementsstudiedbypseudopupil observationsinvivo. Biol. Bull. Levinson,G.,and B. Burnside. 1981. Circadian rhythmsinteleostre- 153:446. tinomotormovements.Invest.Ophthalmol. VisualSci.20:294-303. Stavenga, D.G. 1979. Pseudopupilsofcompoundeyes. Pp. 357-439 Lo,ogMrfa.wtiiClo,dnitanynptdeheWwi.rtethLi.nEuRPlGaa-krd.ceel1fl9es8co1tf.iDvreoLsmiougpthhatin-ltiasn.dmueJc.leaGdneopng.iagsPtmheeyrns.tioCglo.rma7np7ua:lrei1sm5oi5n-- StaviOSenpnnrgHianta,ghneedDr..pbioGBgoe.mr,kleianonnt,fdSmHJeie.nigdsrWeao.ltrbiyKeournPighp,yiensrNi.toehlw1eo9g7rYy7eo,.trikVn.oulIl.nasecVecIltIl/sp6uoApf,ilhHym.meecAnhuoatpnrtiuesmrm,as..ed/I.. 175. Ludolph, C., D. Pagnanelli, and M. I. Mote. 1973. Neural control of WelsCho,mpJ.. HP.hys1i9o3l0..113D:iu5r5n-a7l2.rhythm ofthedistal pigment cellsin the migration ofproximal screening pigment by retinular cells ofthe eyesofcertain crustaceans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 16: 386-395. Marsshwailml,miNn.gJ.c,raMb.. CF.alLlainnedc,tCes.sAa.piKdiunsg.,aBinodl.T.BuWll..C1r4o5n:in1.591-919710.. The Weyrmaiugtrahteiro,nE.in1t9h8e6.eyeDoofrtehteinumloatchellEsphtreisglgieartkhueehsncireelelnai?ngJ.piCgommenpt compound eyes of mantis shrimps (Crustacea, Hoplocarida, Sto- Physiol. A 159: 55-60. matopoda).II.Colourpigmentsintheeyesofstomatopodcrustaceans: Wilkens, L. A.,andJ.L.Larimer. 1976. Photosensitivityinthesixth polychromatic vision by serial and lateral filtering. Phil. Trans. R. abdominalganglionofdecapodcrustaceans:acomparativestudy.J. Soc. Ser B334: 57-84. Comp. Physiol. 106: 69-75. NasseL,D.R.,andT.H.Waterman.1979. Massivediumallymodulated Williams, D.S. 1982. Ommatidialstructureinrelationtoturnoverof photoreceptormembraneturnoverincrablightanddarkadaptation. photoreceptor membrane in the locust. Cell TissueRes. 225: 595- J. Comp. Physiol. 131:205-216. 617. Page, T. L., and J. L. Larimer. 1975. Neural control of circadian Wills, S.A.,T.L. Page, and C. S. Colwell. 1986. Circadian rhythms rhythmicityinthecrayfish.II.TheERGamplituderhythm./ Comp. in theelectroretinogram ofthecockroach. J. Biol. Rhythms. 1: 25- Physiol. 97: 81-96. 37.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.