ebook img

Visual perception. Part 2, Fundamentals of awareness: multi-sensory integration and high-order perception PDF

319 Pages·2006·12.78 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Visual perception. Part 2, Fundamentals of awareness: multi-sensory integration and high-order perception

List of Contributors D. Alais, Department of Physiology and Institute for Biomedical Research, School of Medical Science, Auditory Research Laboratory, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW 2006, Australia E. Aminoff, MGH Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Harvard Medical School, 149 Thirteenth Street, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA S. Anstis, Department of Psychology, UCSD, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093-0109, USA M. Bar, MGH Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Harvard Medical School, 149 Thirteenth Street, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA P. Berbel, Instituto de Neurociencias de Alicante UMH-CSIC, Campus de San Juan, Apartado 18, 03550 San Juan de Alicante, Spain N.P. Bichot, McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Bldg. 46- 6121, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA J.W. Bisley, Center for Neurobiology and Behavior, Columbia University, 1051 Riverside Drive, Unit 87, New York, NY 10032, USA S. Blau, Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA D. Burr, Dipartimento di Psicologia, Universita` degli Studi di Firenze, Via S. Nicolo`, Florence, Italy and Istituto di Neuroscience del CNR, Via Moruzzi 1, Pisa 56100, Italy I.C. Cuthill, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Woodland Road, Bristol BS8 1UG, UK B. de Gelder, Cognitive and Affective Neurosciences Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Tilburg University, PO Box 90153, Tilburg, 5000 LE, The Netherlands V.F. Descalzo, Instituto de Neurosciencias de Alicante, Universidad Miguel Hernandez-CSIC, Apartado 18, 03550 San Juan de Alicante, Spain R. Desimone, McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 77 Massachusetts Avenue, Bldg. 46-3160, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA M.J. Fenske, MGH Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Harvard Medical School, 149 Thirteenth Street, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA E. Gallego, Department of Psychology, University of La Corun˜a, Campus de Elvin˜a, La Corun˜a, 15071, Spain R. Gallego, Instituto de Neurosciencias de Alicante UMH-CSIC, Campus de San Juan, Apartado 18, 03550 San Juan de Alicante, Spain J.V.Garcia-Velasco,InstitutodeNeurocienciasdeAlicanteUMH-CSIC,CampusdeSanJuan,Apartado 18, 03550 San Juan de Alicante, Spain M.E.Goldberg,Center forNeurobiologyandBehavior,ColumbiaUniversity,1051RiversideDrive,Unit 87, New York, NY 10032, USA J. Gottlieb, Center for Neurobiology and Behavior, Columbia University, 1051 Riverside Drive, Unit 87, New York, NY 10032, USA J.M. Groh, Center for Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Pschycology and Neuroscience, and Department of Neurobiology, Duke University, LSRC Rm B203, Durham, NC 27708, USA N. Gronau, MGH Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging, Harvard Medical School, 149 Thirteenth Street, Charlestown, MA 02129, USA ix x X. Huang, Keck Center, Department of Physiology, University of California, San Francisco, CA 94143- 0444, USA S. Kastner, Department of Psychology, Center for the Study of Brain, Mind and Behavior, Princeton University, Green Hall, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA A.Kingstone,DepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofBritishColumbia,2136WestMall,Vancouver,BC V6T 1Z4, Canada S.P.MacEvoy,DepartmentofNeurobiology,DukeUniversityMedicalCenter,Durham,NC27710,USA S.L. Macknik, Departments of Neurosurgery and Neurobiology, Barrow Neurological Institute, 350 W. Thomas Road, Phoenix, AZ 85013, USA H.K.M. Meeren, Cognitive and Affective Neurosciences Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Tilburg University, PO Box 90153, Tilburg, 5000 LE, The Netherlands L.G.Nowak,CentredeRecherche‘‘CerveauetCognition’’,CNRS-Universite´ PaulSabatier,133Routede Narbonne, 31062 Toulouse Cedex, France A. Oliva, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 77 Massachusetts Avenue, Building 46-4065, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA M.A. Paradiso, Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA C.A. Pa´rraga, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, 8 Woodland Road, Bristol BS8 1TN, UK K.K. Porter, University of Alabama, School of Medicine, Birmingham, AL 35294, USA K.D.Powell,LaboratoryofSensorimotorResearch,NationalEyeInstitute,NationalInstitutesofHealth, Bethesda, MD 20892, USA R. Righart, Cognitive and Affective Neurosciences Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Tilburg University, PO Box 90153, Tilburg, 5000 LE, The Netherlands A.F. Rossi, Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37203, USA N.Sagiv,CentreforCognitionandNeuroimaging,BrunelUniversity,Uxbridge,MiddlesexUB83PH,UK M.V.Sanchez-Vives,InstitutodeNeurocienciasdeAlicanteUMH-CSIC,CampusdeSanJuan,Apartado 18, 03550 San Juan de Alicante, Spain K.A.Schneider,DepartmentofPsychology,CenterfortheStudyofBrain,MindandBehavior,Princeton University, Green Hall, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA G. Shalev, Department of Neuroscience, Brown University, Providence, RI 02912, USA S. Soto-Faraco, Hospital Saint Joan de De´u (Edifici Docent), C/Santa Rosa 39-57, Planta 4a, 08950 Esplugues de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain C. Spence, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford OX1 3UD, UK L. Spillmann, Neurozentrum, University Hospital, Breisacher Street 64, 79106 Freiburg, Germany M. Stevens, School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Woodland Road, Bristol BS8 1UG, UK P. Stoerig, Institute of Experimental Psychology II, Heinrich-Heine-University, Building 23.03, Universita¨tsstr 1, D-40225 Dusseldorf, Germany M. Tamietto, Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy A. Torralba, Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Laboratory, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 77 Massachusetts Avenue, 32-D462, Cambridge, MA 02139, USA T.Troscianko,DepartmentofExperimentalPsychology,UniversityofBristol,8WoodlandRoad,Bristol BS8 1TN, UK F. Valle-Incla´n, Department of Psychology, University of La Corun˜a, Campus de Elvin˜a, La Corun˜a 15071, Spain W.A.C. Van De Riet, Cognitive and Affective Neurosciences Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Tilburg University, P.O. Box 90153, Tilburg, 5000 LE, The Netherlands xi J.vanDenStock,CognitiveandAffectiveNeurosciencesLaboratory,DepartmentofPsychology,Tilburg University, PO Box 90153, Tilburg, 5000 LE, The Netherlands J. Ward, Department of Psychology, University College London, 26 Bedford Way, London WC1H 0AP, UK K. Wunderlich, Department of Psychology, Center for the Study of Brain, Mind and Behavior, Princeton University, Green Hall, Princeton, NJ 08544, USA General Introduction ‘‘VisualPerception’’isatwo-volumeseriesofProgressinBrainResearch,basedonthesymposiapresented during the 28th Annual Meeting of the European Conference on Visual Perception (ECVP), the premier transnational conference on visual perception. The conference took place in A Corun˜a, Spain, in August 2005. The Executive Committee members of ECVP 2005 edited this volume, and the symposia speakers provided the chapters herein. The general goal of these two volumes is to present the reader with the state-of-the-art in visual per- ceptionresearch,withaspecialemphasisintheneuralsubstratesofperception.‘‘VisualPerception(Part1)’’ generallyaddressestheinitialstagesofthevisualpathway,andtheperceptualaspectsthancanbeexplained at early and intermediate levels of visual processing. ‘‘Visual Perception (Part 2)’’ is generally concerned with higher levels of processing along the visual hierarchy, and the resulting percepts. However, this separation is not very strict, and several of the chapters encompass both early and high-level processes. The current volume ‘‘Visual Perception (Part 2) — Fundamentals of Awareness, Multi-Sensory Inte- gration and High-Order Perception’’ contains 18 chapters, organized into 4 general sections, each ad- dressing one of the main topics in vision research today: ‘‘The role of context in recognition’’; ‘‘From perceptive fields to Gestalt. A tribute to Lothar Spillmann’’; ‘‘The neural bases of visual awareness and attention, and ‘‘Crossmodal interactions in visual perception’’. Each section includes a short introduction andfourtofiverelatedchapters.Thetopicsaretackledfromavarietyofmethodologicalapproaches,such assingle-neuronrecordings,fMRIandopticalimaging,psychophysics,eyemovementcharacterizationand computational modeling. We hope that the contributions enclosed will provide the reader with a valuable perspective on the current status of vision research, and more importantly, with some insight into future research directions and the discoveries yet to come. Manypeoplehelpedtocompilethisvolume.Firstofall,wethankalltheauthorsfortheircontributions and enthusiasm. We also thank Shannon Bentz, Xoana Troncoso and Jaime Hoffman, at the Barrow Neurological Institute, for their assistance in obtaining copyright permissions for several of the figures reprinted here. Moreover, Shannon Bentz transcribed Lothar Spillmann’s lecture, and provided general administrativehelp.XoanaTroncosowasheroicinherefforttohelpustomeetthesubmissiondeadlineby collating and packing all the chapters, and preparing the table of contents. We are indebted to Johannes MenzelandMaureenTwaig,atElsevier,foralltheirencouragementandassistance;ithasbeenwonderful working with them. xiii xiv Finally, we thank all the supporting organizations that made the ECVP 2005 conference possible: Ministerio de Educacio´n y Ciencia, International Brain Research Organization, European Office of Aer- ospaceReseachandDevelopmentoftheUSAF,Consellerı´adeEducacio´n,IndustriaeComercio-Xuntade Galicia, Elsevier, Pion Ltd., Universidade da Corun˜a, Sociedad Espan˜ola de Neurociencia, SR Research Ltd.,Consellerı´adeSanidade-XuntadeGalicia,MindScienceFoundation,MuseosCientı´ficosCorun˜eses, Barrow Neurological Institute, Images from Science Exhibition, Concello de A Corun˜a, Museo Arqueolo´xico e Histo´rico-Castillo de San Anto´n, Caixanova, Vision Science, Fundacio´n Pedro Barrie´ de la Maza, and Neurobehavioral Systems. Susana Martinez-Conde Executive Chair, European Conference on Visual Perception 2005 On behalf of ECVP 2005’s Executive Committee: Stephen Macknik, Luis Martinez, Jose-Manuel Alonso and Peter Tse SECTION I The Role of Context in Recognition Introduction Predator or prey? Big or little? City skyline or the of object-based and context-based object recogni- latest line of kitchen cabinetry? These questions tion.OlivaandTorralbadiscusstheimportanceof may seem random, but it all depends on the con- context to our ability to recognize the gist of a text in which they are posed. In fact, they are all scene at a single glance: without considering con- questions that become coherent and can be an- text, we might not be able to tell apart the city swered only if the context is known. Well, the skylinefrom the kitchen cabinets. De Gelder, Me- skyline/cabinetry question may still seem out of eren, Righart, Van den Stock, van de Riet, and theblue,butitwillallsnapintofocusasyouread Tamietto show how context also plays a critical thefollowingfourchaptersthatexplorehowvisual role in face recognition. Stevens, Cuthill, Parraga, objectrecognitioncriticallydependsonthecontext and Troscianko discuss how disruptive coloration that the objects lie in, and their relevance to the in camouflage serves to conceal objects within the observer. context of their surroundings. Fenske,Aminoff,Gronau,andBarstartthesec- tion with a chapter that discusses how top-down facilitation modifies the differential contributions Stephen L. Macknik Martinez-Conde,Macknik,Martinez,Alonso&Tse(Eds.) ProgressinBrainResearch,Vol.155 ISSN0079-6123 Copyrightr2006ElsevierB.V.Allrightsreserved CHAPTER 1 Top-down facilitation of visual object recognition: object-based and context-based contributions (cid:1) Mark J. Fenske, Elissa Aminoff, Nurit Gronau and Moshe Bar MGHMartinosCenterforBiomedicalImaging,HarvardMedicalSchool,149ThirteenthStreet,Charlestown,MA02129, USA Abstract: The neural mechanisms subserving visual recognition are traditionally described in terms of bottom-up analysis, whereby increasingly complex aspects of the visual input are processed along a hi- erarchical progression of cortical regions. However, the importance of top-down facilitation in successful recognitionhasbeenemphasizedinrecentmodelsandresearchfindings.Hereweconsiderevidencefortop- down facilitation of recognition that is triggered by early information about an object, as well as by contextual associations between an object and other objects with which it typically appears. The object- basedmechanismisproposedtotriggertop-downfacilitationofvisualrecognitionrapidly,usingapartially analyzedversionoftheinputimage(i.e.,ablurredimage)thatisprojected fromearlyvisualareasdirectly to the prefrontal cortex (PFC). This coarse representation activates in the PFC information that is back- projectedas‘‘initialguesses’’tothetemporalcortexwhereitpresensitizesthemostlikelyinterpretationsof the input object. In addition to this object-based facilitation, a context-based mechanism is proposed to trigger top-down facilitation through contextual associations between objects in scenes. These contextual associations activate predictive information about which objects are likely to appear together, and can influence the ‘‘initial guesses’’ about an object’s identity. We have shown that contextual associations are analyzed by a network that includes the parahippocampal cortex and the retrosplenial complex. The integratedproposaldescribedhereisthatobject-andcontext-basedtop-downinfluences operatetogether, promoting efficient recognition by framing early information about an object within the constraints pro- vided by a lifetime of experience with contextual associations. Keywords: object recognition; top-down; feedback; orbitofrontal cortex; low spatial frequencies; visual context; parahippocampal cortex; retrosplenial cortex; visual associations; priming Successful interaction with the visual world de- thattheyrelyontopromotehighlyefficientvisual pends on the ability of our brains to recognize recognition through top-down processes. The ev- visual objects quickly and accurately, despite infi- idence we review, from studies by our lab and nitevariationsintheappearanceofobjectsandthe others, suggests that top-down facilitation of rec- settings in which they are encountered. How does ognition can be achieved through an object-based the visual system deal with all of this information mechanism that generates predictions about an in such a fluent manner? Here we consider the object’s identity through rapidly analyzed, coarse cortical mechanisms and the type of information information. We also review evidence that top- down facilitation of recognition can be achieved (cid:1) through the predictive information provided by Correspondingauthor.Tel.:+1-617-726-7467;Fax:+1617- contextualassociationsbetweenanobjectorscene 726-7422;E-mail:[email protected] DOI:10.1016/S0079-6123(06)55001-0 3 4 and the other objects that are likely to appear together in a particular setting. In the following sections, we first consider each of these forms of top-down enhancement of object recognition sep- arately, and then consider how these object- and context-based mechanisms might operate together to promote highly efficient visual recognition. An object-based cortical mechanism for triggering top-down facilitation Fig.1. Schematicillustrationofacorticalmechanismfortrig- geringtop-downfacilitationinobjectrecognition(Bar,2003).A The traditionalview regardingvisual processingis lowspatialfrequency(LSF)representationoftheinputimageis that an input image is processed in a bottom-up projected rapidly, possibly via the magnocellular dorsal path- cascade of cortical regions that analyze increas- way,fromearlyvisualcortextotheprefrontalcortex(PFC),in additiontothesystematicandrelativelyslowerpropagationof ingly complex information. This view stems from informationalongtheventralvisualpathway.Thiscoarserep- the well-defined functional architecture of the vis- resentationissufficientforactivatingaminimalsetofthemost ualcortex,whichhasaclearhierarchicalstructure. probableinterpretationsoftheinput,whicharethenintegrated However, several models propose that both bot- with the relatively slower and detailed bottom-up stream of tom-up and top-down analyses can occur in the analysis in object-processing regions of the occipito-temporal cortex(e.g.,fusiformgyrus). cortex simultaneously (Grossberg, 1980; Kosslyn, 1994; Ullman, 1995; Desimone, 1998; Engel et al., 2001;Friston,2003;LeeandMumford,2003),and image in the temporal cortex. For example, if an recent evidence suggests that top-down mecha- imageofanovalblobontopofanarrowlongblob nisms play a significant role in visual processing were extracted from the image initially (Fig. 1), (e.g.,Kosslynetal.,1993;Humphreysetal.,1997; thenobjectrepresentationssharingthisglobalpro- Barcelo´ et al., 2000; Hopfinger et al., 2000; file would be activated (e.g., an umbrella, a tree, a Miyashita and Hayashi, 2000; Gilbert et al., mushroom, a lamp). Combining these top-down 2001; Pascual-Leone and Walsh, 2001; Mechelli ‘‘initial guesses’’ with the systematic bottom-up et al., 2004; Ranganath et al., 2004a). Neverthe- analysis could thereby facilitate visual recognition less,thewayinwhichsuchtop-downprocessingis by substantially limiting the number of object rep- initiated remains an important outstanding issue. resentations that need to be tested. The crux of the issue concerns the fact that top- down facilitation of perception requires high-level information to be activated before some low-level Orbitofrontal involvement in top-down facilitation information. of recognition Recently,Bar(2003)proposedamodelthatspe- cifically addresses the question of how top-down Theproposalthatobjectrecognitionmightbenefit facilitation of visual object recognition might be from a cortical ‘‘short-cut’’ of early, cursory in- triggered.Thegistofthis proposal concernsa cor- formationabouttheinputimageimpliesthatthere tical (or subcortical) ‘‘short-cut’’ of early informa- should be an additional cortical region that shows tion through which a partially analyzed version of recognition-related activity before other object- the input image, comprising the low spatial fre- processingregions.Thecorticalregionsmostoften quency(LSF)components(i.e.,ablurredimage),is associated with visual object recognition are situ- projectedrapidlyfromearlyvisualareasdirectlyto ated in the occipito-temporal cortex (Logothetis the prefrontal cortex (PFC). This coarse represen- etal.,1996;Tanaka,1996).Withinthisregion,the tation is subsequently used to activate predictions fusiform gyrus and the lateral occipital cortex are about the most likely interpretations of the input especially crucial for visual recognition in humans 5 (Kosslynetal.,1995;Martinetal.,1996;Baretal., related activity should develop in this site before 2001; Grill-Spector et al., 2001; Malach et al., other object-processing regions ofoccipito-tempo- 2002). The PFC has also been shown to be in- ralcortex.Totestthisprediction,Baretal.(2006) volved in visual recognition (e.g., Bachevalier and used the same object recognition task as Bar etal. Mishkin, 1986; Wilson et al., 1993; Parker et al., (2001) while obtaining magnetoencephalography 1998; Freedman et al., 2001), and recent evidence (MEG) recordings, which provide millisecond- suggeststhattheorbitofrontalcortex(OFC)might resolution measurements of cortical activity. As be specifically related to top-down visual process- predicted, the contrast between recognized and ing (Bar et al., 2001, 2006; Bar, 2003). Bar et al. not-recognized trials in this study revealed differ- (2001), for example, used functional magnetic res- ential activation in the OFC 50ms before it devel- onance imaging (fMRI) to compare the cortical oped in the object-processing regions of the activity elicited by trials in which objects were occipito-temporal cortex (Fig. 3). Moreover, a successfully recognized with that elicited by the time-frequency, trial-by-trial covariance analysis samepicturesunderidenticalconditionswhenthey of the MEG data demonstrated strong synchrony were not recognized. As expected, this contrast between occipital visual regions and the OFC at a showed differential activity in the occipito-tempo- relatively early stage (beginning at approximately ral regions previously associated with object rec- 80ms after stimulus onset), and a strong sync- ognition.However,successfulrecognitionwasalso hrony between the OFC and the fusiform gyrus associated with increased activity in a site within activity at a relatively later stage (130ms after theOFC(Fig.2).Theconnectionbetweenactivity stimulus onset). Taken together, these results pro- in the OFC and successful object recognition vide strong converging support for the proposal makes it a prime candidate for being involved in that this frontal region plays a critical role in top- top-down facilitation of visual recognition. down facilitation of object recognition. Rapid recognition-related activity in orbitofrontal Early orbitofrontal activity is triggered by low cortex spatial frequencies If the recognition-related region of the OFC iden- The finding that early activity in the OFC is re- tified in the Bar et al. (2001) study is critical for lated to successful object recognition is consistent early top-down facilitation, then recognition- with the model of top-down facilitation proposed byBar(2003).Thismodelpositsthatearlyactivity intheOFCisrelatedtothedirectprojectionfrom early visual areas of an LSF representation of the input image (Bar, 2003). The projection of this early and rudimentary information to the OFC would thereby allow it subsequently to sensitize the representation of the most likely candidate objects in the occipito-temporal cortex as ‘‘initial guesses.’’ThepossibilitythatearlyOFCactivityis driven by LSF information is supported by phys- iological findings that the magnocellular pathway Fig. 2. A group averaged statistical activation map showing recognition-related cortical activity (adapted from Bar et al., conveys LSF information (i.e., a blurred image) 2001)onthelefthemisphere(ventralview)ofabrainthathas early and rapidly (Shapley, 1990; Merigan and been‘‘inflated’’toexposebothsulci(darkgray)andgyri(light Maunsell, 1993; Bullier and Nowak, 1995; Grab- gray). Successful object recognition produced greater activity owska and Nowicka, 1996; Leonova et al., 2003), than unsuccessful recognition attempts in inferior regions of and by evidence from anatomical studies showing boththeoccipito-temporal(e.g.,fusiformgyrus)andthefrontal (e.g.,orbitofrontalcortex)lobes. bidirectional connections between early visual

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.