1PR0Eo.Rb1i1Sn7Os7oN/nA0 e1Lt4I a6Tl1Y. 6 /A7 2VN0AD4L2 ES6ON66CC4IE7A-ALR POSYUCSHALO LINOTGEYR BAUCLTLIOETNISN Watch Out! That Could Be Dangerous: Valence-Arousal Interactions in Evaluative Processing Michael D. Robinson North Dakota State University Justin Storbeck University of Virginia Brian P. Meier Ben S. Kirkeby North Dakota State University Seven studies involving 146 undergraduates examined the andarousal are largely independent intheireffectson effects of stimulus valence and arousal on direct and indirect experience(Russell&Barrett,1999),physiology(Lang, measuresofevaluativeprocessing.Stimuliwereemotionalslides Greenwald,Bradley,&Hamm,1993),andbrainactivity (Studies1to6)orwords(Study7)thatsystematicallyvariedin (Heller&Nitschke,1998),andthereforeshouldbecon- valenceandarousal.Evaluativecategorizationwasmeasured sidered as orthogonal dimensions of appraisal and byreactiontimestoevaluatethestimuli(Studies2,3,and7), emotion. Whatiscommontomultipletheories,however,isthe latenciesrelatedtoemotionalfeelings(Study3),andincidental assumed independence of valence and arousal. And effects on motor performance (Studies 4 and 5). A consistent indeed, there are dependent measures, such as self- interaction was observed such that evaluation latencies were reported emotional experience, that favor the inde- fasterifanegativestimuluswashighinarousalorifapositive pendence of the dimensions. However, this independ- stimuluswaslowinarousal.Studies1,6,and7establishthat enceneednotbetruewhenexamininginitialstagesof thefindingsarenotduetostimulusidentificationprocesses.The stimulus appraisal. This is particularly true given some findingsthereforesuggestthatpeoplemakeevaluativeinferences evidencethatduringinitialstagesofencoding,arousing on the basis of stimulus arousal. objects are presumed to be potentially dangerous, whereas nonarousing objects are presumed to be safe. In this connection, Schneirla (1959) attempted to Keywords: affect; appraisal; cognition; valence; arousal; emotion characterizeemotionalvalenceandarousalastheyapply V tolower animals,suchastheantandtheparamecium. alence and arousal are the most important dimen- He suggested that approach and avoidance behaviors sionsofconnotativemeaning(e.g.,Osgood,1969),feel- seemtobegovernedbyacurvilinearrelationshiprelat- ing states (e.g., Russell & Barrett, 1999), and emotion- ingemotional arousaltoemotional valence.Atmoder- relatedbehavior(e.g.,Lang,1995).However,thereare ate levels of intensity,approach is facilitated.Athigher some divergent theoretical proposals related to the question of how valence and arousal might interact in determining these outcomes. Some scholars, such as Authors’Note:GrantsupportisacknowledgedfromtheNationalSci- Watson(Watson,Wiese, Vaidya,& Tellegen, 1999) and ence Foundation (9817649) and the National Institute of Mental Lang(1995),proposethatarousalisnottrulyindepend- Health (068241). Correspondenceshould be addressed to Michael Robinson, Psychology Department, North Dakota State University, entofvalence.Rather,arousalcaneitherbeofthenega- Fargo, ND 58105; e-mail: [email protected]. tivevariety,reflectingtheactivationofanavoidancesys- PSPB,Vol. 30 No. 11, November 2004 1472-1484 tem,orofthepositivevariety,reflectingtheactivationof DOI:10.1177/0146167204266647 anapproachsystem.Othertheoristsarguethatvalence © 2004 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc. 1472 Robinson et al. / VALENCE-AROUSAL INTERACTIONS 1473 levels of intensity, by contrast, avoidance is facilitated. wordsofÖhman(1997),preattentiveaspectsoforient- For example, a paramecium will approach a light of a ing “are determined by simple stimulus dimensions moderateintensitybutwillavoidalightofhighintensity suchassuddenonsetorhighintensity....Abrupt,high- (Schneirla,1959).Amonghumaninfants,asimilarpat- intensity stimulation has been associated with danger ternmayapply(Izard,1993).Researchandtheoryinfact throughout evolution, and therefore sensory systems suggests thatthe ability to regulate intense stimulation havetunedimmediatelyandautomaticallytorespondto developsearlyinchildhood,specificallywiththematura- such stimulus parameters” (p. 167). tion of the prefrontal cortex (Harman & Fox, 1997). An implication of this view is that arousing stimuli, Because infantsareunable toregulate intensestimula- regardlessofwhethertheyarepositiveornegative,may tion, an intense stimulus tends to overwhelm the triggerpreattentiveaspectsoforientingandinturnbe attentionalandcopingabilitiesoftheinfant,leadingto associated with preattentive evaluations indicative of apparentdistressandavoidance(Harman&Fox,1997; potential danger. For example, a roller coaster ride, Rothbart, Posner, & Boylan, 1990). whichisenjoyableformanywhorideit,maynevertheless Valence-arousal interactions of the type mentioned beautomaticallyevaluatedasnegativebasedonitsinten- above are probably not unique to lower animals and sity and capacity for quick motion. humaninfants.Tounderstandwhythisistrue,itisuseful From Independence to Integration to appreciate the fact that there may be two distinct preattentivemechanismsrelatedtoemotionalappraisal On one hand, positive stimuli induce an approach (Robinson, 1998). orientation, whereas negative stimuli induce an avoid- ance orientation (Neumann, Förster, & Strack, 2003). Preattentive Mechanisms in Emotional Appraisal Ontheotherhand,intenseandnovelstimulitriggeran A good deal of evidence suggests that people auto- avoidanceorientation,whereasmildandfamiliarstimuli maticallyevaluatestimuli,atleastundercertaintaskcon- triggeranapproachorientation(Zajonc,2001).Accord- ditions (Klauer & Musch, 2003). Such evidence has ingtoRobinson(1998),bothofthesecomputationsare comefromlatency-(e.g.,Bargh,Chaiken,Govender,& performedindependentlyatthepreattentivelevel.How- Pratto, 1992) and judgment-based (e.g., Murphy & ever,Robinsonalsoproposedthatthesetwomechanisms Zajonc, 1993) versions of the affective priming para- contributetoone’sconsciousunderstandingofthestim- digm.Italsohascomefromotherparadigmssuchasthe ulus.Thatis,outputfromthetwomechanismsmustbe emotionalStrooptask(e.g.,Pratto,1994),theaffective integrated prior to conscious evaluation. Simon task (e.g., De Houwer & Eelen, 1998), and the Because intense stimuli are potentially dangerous implicit association test (e.g., Greenwald, McGhee, & ones,thisintegrativeactivitymaybeeasierwhenastimu- Schwartz, 1998). The valence of an object can be lusisbothnegativeandhighinarousal.Forexample,a decoded quickly, a point that is apparent in findings stimulussuchasascowling,barkingdog,whichisboth showingthatsubliminalpresentationsofvalencedstim- negativeandarousing,isclearlyapotentialthreattothe uli are effective in altering evaluations (Dijksterhuis & self.Suchastimulusshouldbeevaluatedquicklyatthe Aarts, 2003; Murphy & Zajonc, 1993). consciouslevel.Bycontrast,astimulussuchasaninjured However,thereisalsoevidenceforasecondmecha- kittenmightbemoredifficulttoevaluate.Ononehand, nism that orients attention toward novel and intense thesightofahelplessanimalengenderssomepityand stimuli. Reliable output from this mechanism include approachmotivation;ontheotherhand,theinjurypro- skinconductanceresponses,adecreaseinheartrate(for vokesnegativeaffectintheviewer.Thecombinationmay visualstimuli),andenhancedattention(Lang,Bradley, resultinaconflictedstateinwhichbothapproachand & Cuthbert, 1997). Stimulus intensity can be coded avoidance motives are present (Lang, 1995). preattentively,asestablishedbyalargebodyoffindings Bycontrast,evaluativedecisionsmaybeslowedwhen showingthatsubliminalstimuliarecapableofeliciting a stimulus is both positive and arousing. The intense theorientingresponse(Öhman,1997).Althoughatthe aspectofthestimulusmaypromoteacertaindefensive conscious level orienting occurs for both positive and orientationuntilthestimulusisbetterunderstood.And negativestimuli(Langetal.,1997),thereisareasonto yet, the positive affective nature of the stimulus pre- think that, at the preattentive level, the orienting cludesmakinganegativeevaluation.Theresultshould response may be associated with the fear system beconsiderableuncertaintyastowhetherthestimulusis (Öhman, 1997; Robinson, 1998). Of interest, however, beneficial or harmful during early stages of stimulus its inputs are not specific to threatening stimuli (e.g., encoding. For example, there are few acts that bring snakes,angerfaces).Rather,itmayrespondtoabroader morepleasurethansex.Andyet,wedonotexpecttosee range of inputs that are indicative of potential danger, attractivenakedstrangersinourlivingroom,andweare such as intensity and capacity of quick motion. In the notimmediatelypreparedtocopewithsuchopportuni- 1474 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN ties.Bycontrast,apositivelowarousalstimulussuchasa Method pieceofchocolatecakebringspleasurebutnoimmedi- Participants. Participants were 20 undergraduates ate hint of danger or uncertainty. In this case, a from the University of Illinois who received course preattentiveevaluationofthestimulusaspositivewould credit. havenocompetitionfromasecondpreattentivemecha- nism devoted to intensity or arousal. If these specula- Apparatus. A DOS-based computer controlled two tions are correct, valence and arousal may interact in Kodak Ektapro projectors. The projectors were determining conscious evaluation speed. These ideas designedtointerfacewithacomputerandbecapableof motivated the present studies. randomized presentation sequences. Projectors were placedwithinahandmadecasethatrestedontopofa5- Overview of Studies ftfilingcabinetthatprojectedimagesfrombehindand Stimuliformostofthestudiesconsistedofslidesfrom abovetheparticipantduringthecourseofthestudy.Pre- theInternationalAffectivePictureSystem(IAPS;Lang, cise timing was accomplished by using high-speed Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1999). To study direct aspects of Uniblitzshuttersaffixedtotheprojectorlenses(approx- evaluative processing, we simply asked people to, as imately 1 ms random error). quicklyandaccuratelyaspossible,categorizestimulias Participants sat in a standard chair equipped with a positiveornegativeinthreeseparatestudies(Studies2, smalldesk.Thechairwasplaced3ftinfrontofalarge(5 × 3, and 7). Because we hypothesized that emotional ft 5 ft) projection screen. Emotional images were × arousal is negatively coded, we expected valence and approximately3ft 3ftandwereprojectedontothecen- arousaltointeractsuchthatnegative/higharousaland terofthescreen.Theenvironmentwasimmersiveinthat positive/low arousal stimuli would be evaluated more images were typically life sized or larger. quickly. Stimuli. Emotional slides were chosen from Lang Intwoadditionalstudies(Studies4and5),weasked et al.’s (1999) IAPS. We were particularly interested in peopletoevaluatetheslidescovertlyandthenperforma × choosingslidestorepresentallcellsofa2(valence) 2 second task. When two tasks are closely sequenced in (arousal) design. After some preliminary testing (e.g., time,performanceonthesecondtaskisdependenton Robinson&Clore,2001),wesettledonasetof56slides. resolutionofthefirsttask,asknownintheattentionand Twenty-eightofthemwerenegativeinvalence,whereas performanceliterature(Pashler,Johnston,&Ruthruff, × 28werepositive.Withineachvalence,therewere14low 2000). We expected to observe a similar Valence arousal and 14 high arousal slides.1 Arousal interaction in these studies. Of importance, It is useful to statistically establish that valence and because stimulus and response factors were independ- arousalweremanipulatedindependentlyinthepresent ently varied in Studies 4 and 5, interactions must be design.Toestablishthispoint,wecreatedadatasetwith interpretedintermsofencodingoperationsratherthan slideasunitofanalysis(N=56).Wethenentereddummy response factors. codes for our dichotomized valence (positive vs. nega- BecauseStudies1through6allusedemotionalslides tive)andarousal(lowvs.high)factors.Wethenadded asstimuli,itseemedpossiblethatvalence/arousalinter- valence and arousal means from Lang et al.’s (1999) actionscouldbespecifictopictorialimages.Tobroaden slide norms. Finally, we computed a valence extremity thescopeofthecurrentframework,wesoughttorepli- score for each slide, which was defined in terms of the cate key results in Study 7, this time in the context of extenttowhichLangetal.’svalencemeanfortheslide stimulus words rather than images. differed from 5.0, the midpoint of their scale. A series of one-way ANOVAs established that our STUDY 1 valence designation of slides was a strong predictor of Prior to conducting Studies 2 through 7, we con- Langetal.’s(1999)valencenorms,F(1,54)=715.47,p= ducted Study 1, which was concerned with stimulus .00,butdidnotpredictarousalnorms,F(1,54)=1.36,p= selectionissues.Wesoughttoselectemotionalslidesthat .25,orvalenceextremityscores,F(1,54)=2.27,p=.14.A were independent in valence and arousal. In addition, second series of one-way ANOVAs established that our × Study1soughttoshowthatthereisnoValence Arousal arousal designation of slides was a strong predictor of interactionrelatedtothevisualcomplexityoftheslides. Langetal.’s(1999)arousalnorms,F(1,54)=186.15,p= Totheextentthatthereisnot,subsequentfindingscan .00,butdidnotpredictvalencenorms,F<1,orvalence beinterpretedintermsofevaluativeprocessingspecifi- extremityscores,F(1,54)=1.97,p=.17.Finally,inathird × cally rather than object identification processes more series of 2 (dichotomized valence) 2 (dichotomized generally.TheidentificationnormscollectedinStudy1 arousal) ANOVAs, we confirmed that dichoto- also were examined in Study 6 reported later in the mizedvalenceanddichotomizedarousaldidnotinter- article. actinpredictingLangetal.’s(1999)valencenorms,F(1, Robinson et al. / VALENCE-AROUSAL INTERACTIONS 1475 52)=2.03,p=.16,arousalnorms,F(1,52)=1.42,p=.24, 820 or valence extremity scores, F(1, 52) = 1.08, p = .30. Valence and arousal were therefore manipulated inde- 800 pendently in the stimulus materials. s d Procedures. Participants were tested within individual on c 780 testing sessions. Following a 10-min dark adaptation se period,theyengagedinthemainpartofthestudy.Par- Milli n ticipantsweretoldthatwewereinterestedinhowlongit e i 760 m would take them to identify the main theme of slides. n Ti LHoigwh AArroouussaall They were instructed to respond to “gist” information. o ati 740 Specifically,westated,“Ifyouseeadog,youshouldhit u al thespacebarbeforeyouknowexactlywhetherthedogis Ev a golden retriever or a German shepherd.” These an 720 e M instructions were designed to preclude extensive and identification-irrelevant examination of the slides. 700 There was a 3-s delay between trials and identification responses were made by hitting the spacebar on a 680 computer keyboard. Negative Positive Results Figure 1 Evaluation latencies by valence and arousal, Study 2. Prior to analyzing identification times, we log- transformed them to normalize the distribution. We except that Study 2 used a response box rather than a thenreplacedoutliersthatwere2.5standarddeviations computer keyboard. beloworabovethegrandlatencymeanwiththesecutoff × Procedures. After dark adaptation, participants were scores.Latencymeanswereanalyzedina2(valence) 2 askedtoevaluatetheslidesasquicklyandaccuratelyas (arousal) repeated-measures ANOVA. There was no possible,indicatingwhethereachslideispositive(1key; effectforvalenceonidentificationtimes,F(1,19)=1.14, leftindexfinger)ornegative(4key;rightindexfinger). p=.30.Therewas,however,amaineffectforarousal,F(1, Slideswereshowntwice,eachtimeinadifferentrandom 19)=8.19,p=.01.Thethemeofhigharousalslideswas order.2 identified faster(M=1,046ms)thanthethemeoflow arousalslides(M=1,152ms).Thisresultcouldbesome- Results whatconsistentwiththefindingthathigharousalslides Inaccurate trials were dropped and times were log- trigger more active processing within the visual cortex transformed.Wethenreplacedtimes2.5standarddevia- (Lang et al., 1998). Of more importance, however, the tionsbelowandabovethegrandlatencymeanwiththese interactionbetweenvalenceandarousalwasnotsignifi- cutoffscores. Evaluation latencies were analyzed in a 2 cant,F<1.Becausetheinteractionwasnotsignificant, × (valence) 2(arousal)repeated-measuresANOVA.Nei- interactive effects of valence and arousal cannot be therthemaineffectforvalence,F<1,northemaineffect viewed as consequences of visual complexity. forarousal,F<1,wassignificant.Ashypothesized,how- × ever,therewasaValence Arousalinteraction,F(1,20)= STUDY 2 19.75, p = .00. The means, reported in Figure 1, reveal InStudy1,weshowedthatthepresentslideswerenot thatnegativeimageswereevaluatedmorequicklywhen × associatedwithaValence Arousalinteractiononiden- highinarousal,whereaspositiveimageswereevaluated tificationlatencies.InStudy2,wesoughttoshowthatthe more quickly when low in arousal.3 × sameslidesproduceaValence Arousalinteractionper- Discussion taining to evaluation latencies. × Although Study 1 indicated no hint of a Valence Method Arousalinteractioninfluencingidentificationprocesses, × Study 2 shows that there is a rather robust Valence Participants. Participants were 21 undergraduates Arousal interaction influencing evaluation processes. from the University of Illinois who received course The latter result provides support for the valence- credit. arousal conflict theory pursued in this article. Stimuliandapparatus.Thestimuliwerethesameasin Atthispoint,itisworthconsideringandrejectingan Study1.Thehardwareforthestudywasalsoverysimilar alternative account of Study 2 results. Specifically, it 1476 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN might be proposed thatpeople seek tomaximize their 1140 exposuretopositiveeventsandminimizetheirexposure tonegativeevents.Asaresult,theymaybereluctantto s d 1120 n terminate a positive image by making an evaluation, o c e resulting in slower evaluation times. However, this s potentialaccountofourresultsisflawedforatleastthree Milli 1100 n reasons.One,suchanexplanationforthefindingsfails e i m toaccountforthelackofaninteractioninStudy1.Two, Ti 1080 itfailstoexplainpreviousdatashowingthatwhenpartic- ment LHoigwh AArroouussaall ipantsaregivenfreedomtolookataslideforasshortor g d u 1060 aslongasdesirable,itisthearousallevelofthestimulus, e J v rather than its valence, that influences viewing time ati u (Langetal.,1997).Three,thisaccountcouldpredicta al 1040 × Ev valencemaineffect,butnottheValence Arousalinter- n a action reported in Study 2. In Study 3, we sought to Me 1020 replicate and extend Study 2. 1000 STUDY 3 Negative Positive Method Figure 2 Evaluative judgment latencies by valence and arousal, Study 3. Participants. Participants were 20 undergraduates from the University of Illinois who received course credit. the1key.Asinpriorstudies,wedeletedinaccuratetrials, Stimuliandapparatus.Emotionalstimuliandappara- log-transformedlatencies,andreplacedonesthatwere tuswerethesameasthosereportedinStudy2.Torepre- 2.5 standard deviations above or below the mean. sent the taskpertaining to a particular block, a second Latencymeanswereexaminedina2(valence:positive slideprojectordisplayedtheslide“evaluate”or“experi- vs.negative) × 2 (arousal: low vs.high)× 2 (judgment: ence” during the relevant trials. evaluation vs. feeling) ANOVA. Noneofthemaineffectsweresignificant,Fs<1.Also, Procedures.Participantswereinformedthattheywould × × thethree-wayValence Arousal Judgmentinteraction (a) evaluate slides, judging whether they were good or wasnotsignificant,F(1,19)=1.50,p=.24,indicatingthat bad, and (b) indicate whether the slide causes more similarfindingspertainedtotheevaluationtaskasper- pleasantorunpleasantfeelings.Tomakethetasksseem × tainedtothefeelingjudgmenttask.Two-wayValence somewhatdistinct,weinformedparticipantsthatevalua- × Judgment, F(1,19)=2.26,p=.15,andArousal Judg- tionisprimarilybasedonstimulusqualitiesratherthan ment, F < 1, interactions were similarly not significant. experience, whereas feelings are primarily based on × However,asexpected,therewasasignificantValence experienceratherthanonstimulusqualities.Eachslide Arousalinteraction,F(1,19)=13.86,p=.00,whichalso was paired with each task exactly once. Trials were blockedsuchthatparticipantsperformedeachtaskfor was replicated within the evaluation task, F(1, 19) = 10 trials in a row. 4.43, p = .05, and the feeling judgment task, F(1, 19) = 17.00, p = .00, considered separately. Figure 2, which When participants were ready to make a judgment, reports means collapsed across task, indicates that the theypressedthe1keyontheresponsebox.Whenthe1 × Valence Arousal interaction was parallel to that key was pressed, the emotional slide was removed and reportedinStudy 2.4 theparticipantspressedthe2key(“bad”or“unpleasant feelings”)orthe3key(“good”or“pleasantfeelings”)to Discussion make their ratings. Under these procedures, time to press the 1 key becomes the important index of judg- ApotentialconcernwithStudies2and3isthatpeople ment time (see Robinson & Clore, 2002). This said, mayhesitatetoevaluatepositivestimuliaspositiveifthey effectswerecomparablewhenlatencytopressthe1key produce states related to lust, excitement, or joy. was added to latency to make the rating. Althoughsuchstatesarenotsociallyundesirableinany fundamental way, they may be seen as instrumentally Results undesirableinthattheyareperceivedasinterferingwith Asexpected,nofactorsinfluencedtimetopressthe2 performanceefficiencywithineverydaylifetasks(Erber or3key,ps>.15.Wethereforefocusedontimetopress &Erber,2000).Totheextentthatthisistrue,theindivid- Robinson et al. / VALENCE-AROUSAL INTERACTIONS 1477 ualmaychoosetorecognizeandreportsuchstatesonly 484 whentheyareundeniable.Althoughwedonotfeelthat suchresponse-relatedexplanationsforourfindingsare s legitimate, we also recognize that every public evalua- nd o 480 c tion carries some potential for scrutiny from others. e s Ifso,itseemsdesirabletoshowthatthesameValence× Milli Arousalinteractioncharacterizesperformanceontasks n inwhichthereisnopublicevaluationatall.Tothisend, me i 476 Ti wedesigned Studies4and5toexamine theincidental on Low Arousal consequences ofevaluation.Participantswere askedto ati High Arousal n evaluate the slides, ostensibly in preparation for later crimi 472 questions. Following each slide, they also engaged in a s Di second,neutraltask.Ifevaluationsaredelayedbecause ot D the participant is encountering difficulty in deciding n a 468 e whetheraslideispositiveornegative,suchevaluativedif- M ficulties should be reflected in secondary task perfor- mance(Pashleretal.,2000).Onthebasisofthisreason- 464 ing, we hypothesized that latencies on a neutral Negative Positive × secondarytaskwouldexhibitasimilarValence Arousal interaction as obtained in prior studies. Figure 3 Dotdiscriminationlatenciesbyvalenceandarousal,Study4. STUDY 4 presentation (see Robinson & Compton, 2004, for Method related studies).Fifthandfinally,thetrialended when the participant pressed the 1 key on the button box if Participants. Participants were 42 undergraduates onedotwaspresentedorhitthe2keyonthebuttonbox from North Dakota State University. if two dots were presented. Following this response, Stimuli and apparatus. The emotional stimuli and there was a 5 s interval before the next trial began. apparatus were the same as used in Study 2. A second Results slide projector was equipped to display dot stimuli requiring a response. Inaccurate responses were deleted and times were Procedures.Participantsweretoldthattheyhadtwopri- log-transformed. Subsequently, we replaced latencies marytasks.First,theyshouldattendtoandevaluateemo- 2.5standarddeviationsaboveandbelowthegrandmean tionalslidesthatwerepresentedatthestartofeachtrial withthesecutoffvalues.Meanswerecomputedforeach × becausetheymaybeaskedabouttheslideslaterinthe cellofthe2(valenceofprime:positivevs.negative) 2 × study. Second, they were instructed to determine, as (arousalofprime:lowvs.high) 2(visualfield:leftvs. quickly and accurately as possible, whether one or two right)design.Meanswerethenanalyzedinarepeated- dots were presented following each emotional slide. measures ANOVA. All effects were not significant, ps > × This dot discrimination task provided the latency data .10, except for one. Specifically, the Valence Arousal for the study. interaction was significant, F(1, 41) = 10.01, p = .00. As Figure3shows,theinteractionreplicatesthosereported The56slideswere showntwiceeach,eachtimeina inthepriorstudies,butdoessowithinthecontextofan different random sequence. Target slides consisted of incidental evaluation paradigm. oneortwodotspresentedtoeithertheleftorrightvisual field.Dotslideswerepresentedlaterallyatadistanceof Discussion approximately2.5degreesvisualangleleftorrightoffix- ation. Dot slides were randomly paired with emotional ThefindingsfromStudy4addtoourconfidencein slides. the valence-arousal conflict theory. When an environ- Each trial had the following sequence. First, a ran- mental stimulus is arousing, it is apparently judged, domly selected emotional slide was presented for 1 s. preattentively,tobepotentiallythreatening.Thisfacili- Second, the slide disappeared followed by a 500 ms tates negative evaluations but inhibits positive evalua- blankinterval.Third,alaserfixationwaspresentedfor tions. Although such a conflict is eventually resolved, 200ms.Fourth,adotslidewaspresented800msfollow- sucharesolutiontakestime,thereforeslowingconcur- ing the offset of the laser fixation. Dot slides were pre- rent performance. Study 5 sought to replicate Study 4, sentedfor100ms,aprocedurethatensureslateralized again using an incidental evaluation paradigm. How- 1478 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN ever, we replaced the dot discrimination task with a 390 motor task. STUDY 5 s nd 380 Method o c e s Participants. Participants were 15 undergraduates Milli from the University of Illinois. e in m 370 Low Arousal Stimuliandapparatus.Stimuliwerethesame56emo- Ti High Arousal nt tionalslidesusedinpriorstudies.Hardwareforthestudy e m wasalsothesame,includingtheEktaproprojectorand ve o M thecustom-madebuttonbox.However,theparticipant n a 360 sat in a chair without a desk portion. In addition, the e M chairwasplacedclosertotheslidescreensothatpartici- pants could easily reach it. Finally, the button box was affixedtotheslidescreensuchthatitslocationwasinthe middle, both horizontally and vertically, of projected 350 Negative Positive images. Figure 4 Motor latencies by valence and arousal, Study 5. Procedures.Participantswereinformedthattherewere twotasks.One,theyshouldevaluatetheemotionalslides astheywerepresented.Andtwo,theyshouldperforman fasterinthebackwarddirection(M=155ms)thaninthe indicatedmovementasquicklyaspossibleuponasignal forwarddirection(M=578ms).Inaddition,therewasa from the laser pen. During half of the blocks, partici- × Valence Arousal interaction, F(1, 14) = 5.24, p = .04. pants began each trial with their right hands on their TherelevantmeansareshowninFigure4.Noneofthe right knees. With a signal from a laser pen, they were other effects were significant, allps > .50.5 instructed to reach up as quickly as possible and press anyofthefourkeysonthebuttonbox.Duringtheother Discussion halfoftheblocks,participantsbeganeachtrialwiththeir index fingers depressing the 1 key of the button box. TheresultsofStudies4and5,similartotheresultsof With a signal from a laser pen, they were instructed to Studies2and3,cannotbereconciledwiththeviewthat remove their fingers from the button as quickly as valence and arousal are completely independent in possible. evaluativeprocessing,andtheycannotsupporttheview that arousal merely reflects activation of approach or Emotionalslideswereshowntwiceeach,eachtimein avoidance systems. Instead, our effects are compatible a different random order. Movement direction—press withproposalsthatintensestimuli,whichelicittheori- orrelease—wasrandomlypairedwithslides.Trialswere enting response (McCurdy, 1950), are categorized as blocked such that participants performed the same aversiveduringearlystagesofencoding(Zajonc,1998). movementforseventrialsinarow.Trialsconsistedofthe Beforeturningtotheissueofwhetherourresultsare following sequence. First, a picture appeared. Second, uniquetopictorialstimuli,wesoughttoconductameta- therewasa2-sdelayuntilalaserpenfireda150-mssig- analysisofthefindingsreportedinStudies1through5. nal. This signal was the impetus for performing the Therelevantanalysesservetwopurposes.One,withthis movement. Third, the participant performed the new dataset,we were able to control for identification relevant action. latencies(collectedinStudy1).Andtwo,wecouldtreat Results valenceandarousalinadimensional,ratherthancate- gorical,manner.Forthesakeofconvenience,wereferto Movement times were log-transformed to normalize this meta-analysis as Study 6. the distribution. For each movement separately, we replaced scores that were 2.5 standard deviations from the grand latency mean for that movement direction. STUDY 6 × Latency means were examined in a 2 (valence) 2 × Method (arousal) 2(movementdirection)repeated-measures ANOVA. There was a main effect for movement direc- Wecreatedanewdataset,withslide(N=56)asthe tion,F(1,14)=324.69,p=.00,suchthatmovementswere unitofanalysis.Increatingthisdataset,wefirstaddedz Robinson et al. / VALENCE-AROUSAL INTERACTIONS 1479 scored norms related to valence and arousal and their TABLE 1: Multiple Regression Predictors of Evaluation Latencies and Incidental Motor Performance, Study 6 interaction,basedontheLangetal.(1999)means.We thenaddedlog-transformedidentificationmeansfrom Predictor Study1.Suchidentificationmeanswerecomputedsepa- Dependent Measure Identification Valence Arousal Interaction rately for each slide, averaged across all of the partici- pants in that study. Finally, we added log-transformed Evaluation time, Study 2 .44* .09 .00 .45* evaluationlatenciesforeachslide,whichwerecollected Evaluation time, Study 3 .55* .21 .09 .25* inStudies2and3,aswellaslog-transformedincidental Feeling time, Study 3 .41* –.02 .02 .31* Dot discrimination, Study 4 .23 –.08 .17 .29* performancemeasures,whichwerecollectedinStudies Motor behavior, Study 5 .26 –.02 .03 .27* 4 and 5. NOTE:Withslide(N=56)asunitofanalysis,weperformedfivemulti- Results pleregressions,oneforeachofthedependentmeasuresreportedin thistable.Predictorsweresimultaneouslyentered.Depictedintheta- Inaninitialmultipleregression,weenteredzscores ble are standardized Betas resulting from these regressions. *p< .05. forvalence,arousal,andtheirinteractioninaregression predictingidentificationlatencies.Theregressionindi- catedthatthemorearousingtheslide,thefasteritsmain actionwouldgeneralizetotheevaluationofemotional themewasidentified,t=–3.20,p=.00.Therewasalsoa words. We sought to investigate this issue in Study 7. marginal hint in this regression that slides higher in Method valence were identified more quickly than were slides lowerinvalence,t=–1.94,p=.06.However,theinterac- Participants. Participants were 28 undergraduates tionbetweenvalenceandarousalwasnotsignificant,t< from North Dakota State University. 1. This regression confirms that the interactions Stimuli. Word stimuli were chosen on the basis of reported in this article are not due to the visual normscompiledbyBradleyandLang(1999).Theinves- complexity of the slides. tigatorsaskedraterstousepictorialscalestodetermine Wenextsoughttoexaminetheinteractiveeffectsof thevalence(1=unpleasant,9=pleasant)andarousal(1 valence and arousal in predicting the dependent mea- =low,9=high)ofeachofthewords.BradleyandLang sures collected in Studies 2 through 5. In these regres- also obtained word frequency norms from Kucera and sions,wesimultaneouslyenteredthefollowingterms:z Francis (1967). scoredidentification,zscoredvalence,zscoredarousal, InselectingstimulifromtheBradleyandLang(1999) andatermtorepresent thevalencebyarousalinterac- norms, we sought to satisfy multiple constraints. First, tion.Theresults,intermsofBetacoefficientsandsignifi- lowandhigharousalwordsshouldbeequalinvalence. cance, are reported in Table 1. Second, the extremity of negative and positive words Inthestudies(2and3)involvingexplicitevaluations, should be equal. Third, negative and positive words identificationlatencieswereapositivepredictorofper- shouldbeequalinarousal.Andfourth,allfourcatego- formance. In other words, knowing what something is riesofwordsshouldbeequalinwordfrequency.Acare- (i.e.,identification) is an important predictor of being fuliterativeprocesssatisfiedalloftheseconstraintsand abletodeterminewhetheritisgoodorbad(i.e.,evalua- produced 35 words per category.6 tion). Identification latencies also positively predicted To examine the effectiveness of our selection incidentalmotorperformance(Studies4and5),butnot procedures, we created dummy codes to represent × significantly so. The Valence Arousal interaction was valence(–1=negative,+1=positive)andarousal(–1= significant in all five regressions. In other words, the low, +1 = high) selection criteria. We then used these morearousingtheslide,thefaster(implicitorexplicit) dummycodestoexaminetheoriginalratingnormspro- evaluation latencies were for negative slides, but the videdbyBradleyandLang(1999).Theseanalyseswere slower they were for positive slides. conducted with word (N = 140) as the unit of analysis within repeated-measures ANOVAs. With valence rat- ingsasthedependentmeasure,valencehadasignificant STUDY 7 effect (Ms = 3.27 and 6.73 for negative and positive Althoughtheuseofaconstantsetofstimulihasbene- words,respectively),F(1,138)=3187.57,p=.00,whereas × fits, as the meta-analysis shows, this stimulus selection arousalandtheValence Arousalinteractiondidnot, procedurealsomayhavecosts.Specifically,becausewe Fs<1.Withextremityasthedependentmeasure,none onlyusedemotionalslidesasstimuliinthepriorstudies, of the factors were significant predictors, Fs < 1. With × itisunknownwhetherasimilarValence Arousalinter- arousalratingsasthedependentmeasure,arousalhada 1480 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN significant effect (Ms = 4.23 and 5.81 for low and high 920 arousalwords,respectively),F(1,138)=349.04,p=.00, × whereas valence andtheValence Arousal interaction ds 900 n didnot,Fs<1.Finally,withwordfrequencynormsasthe o c e dependentmeasure,noneofthefactorsweresignificant s predictors,Fs<1.Insum,thewordselectionprocesswas Milli 880 n successful. cies i 860 LHoigwh AArroouussaall Procedures.Thecollection ofthedatawasconducted en at inthecontextofsmallgroupsessions.Wordstimuliwere L nt 840 displayed on computer monitors. Participants were e m asked to both identify and evaluate the 140 stimulus dg u 820 words.Weusedtwocounterbalancingprocedures.First, J viarandomassignmentprocedures,12ofthe28partici- 800 pantswereassignedtoaconditioninwhichtheymade Negative Positive identifications before making evaluations; the remain- Figure 5 Judgment latencies by valence and arousal, Study 7. ing participants completed the tasks in the opposite order.Second,intheevaluationtaskitself,wecounterbal- ancedwhetherthe1keyatthetopofthekeyboard(n = × × Valence TasknortheArousal Taskinteractionswere 13) or the 9 key (n = 15) represented a positive significant,ps > .05. evaluation. × Of most importance was the significant Valence Intheidentification task,participantswere asked to Arousalinteraction,F(1,26)=43.23,p=.00.Theinterac- pressthespacebarassoonastheycouldidentifytheword tionwascross-overinnature.AsshowninFigure5,par- in question. In the evaluation task, participants were ticipantswerefastertoprocess(i.e.,identifyorevaluate) askedtoevaluatethewordbypressingeitherthe1or9 negativewordswhentheywerehighinarousal;bycon- keyatthetopofthekeyboard.Inneitherofthetasksdid trast,theywerefastertoprocesspositivewordswhenthey we provide accuracy feedback. Words were randomly were low in arousal. Finally, there was some indication selected foreachparticipantforeachtask.Trialproce- × that the Valence Arousal interaction was more pro- duresinvolvedthepresentationofasinglewordonthe nouncedfortheevaluationtaskthanfortheidentifica- computer screen. The timing began with the appear- × × tion task, but the Valence Arousal Task interaction ance of the word and ended with a relevant response. wasnotsignificant,F(1,26)=2.83,p=.10.Wesoughtto Afteraresponse,therewasa500-msdelayuntilthenext supplement these results with additional results involv- word was presented. ing word as the unit of analysis.7 Results Word-levelanalyses.Giventhattaskorderdidnotaffect Participant-levelanalyses.Priortoconductingparticipant- thepatternofresultsinanysubstantialway,meanswere levelanalyses,wedeletedtheinaccurateevaluations.We collapsedacrossthisvariable.Wecreatedanewdataset thenlog-transformedlatenciesandreplacedtimesthat withword(N=140)astheunitofanalysis.Foreachword were2.5SDsbeloworabovethegrandlatencymean,sep- separately,weenteredindependentvariablesrelatedtoz aratelyforeachtask.Log-latencymeanswerethenaver- scoredvalenceandarousal.Bothwerecontinuousmea- × agedwithincellsofa2(valence:negativevs.positive) 2 sures and both were based on the Bradley and Lang × (arousal:lowvs.high) 2(task:identificationvs.evalua- (1999) norms. We also entered frequency norms from tion) within-subject design. We also retained the KuceraandFrancis(1967).Finally,foreachwordsepa- between-subjects factor of task order in the relevant rately, we obtained identification and evaluation laten- ANOVA.Significanteffectswillbereportedintermsof cies averaged across participants. milliseconds. We initially performed a multiple regression Therewerenoeffectsfortaskorder,ps>.20,savean designedtoreplicatetheanalysesreportedabove,here × × Arousal Task TaskOrderinteraction,F(1,26)=6.87, withwordastheunitofanalysis.Inthisregression,log- p=.01,thatwasneitherpredictednorinformative.How- transformedevaluationlatencieswerepredictedonthe ever,thereweremaineffectsforvalence(Ms=904and basis of z scores related to valence, arousal, and their 861 ms for negative and positive words, respectively), interaction.Themaineffectforvalencewasnotsignifi- F(1,26)=8.99,p=.01,arousal(Ms=866and900msfor cant, t = –1.11, p = .27. However, we replicated a main lowandhigharousalwords,respectively),andtask(Ms= effectforthearousalleveloftheword,t=2.60,p=.01,as × 789and977msforidentificationandevaluationtasks, wellastheValence Arousalinteraction,t=3.76,p=.00. respectively), F(1, 26) = 10.35, p = .00. Neither the Estimatedmeans(+or–1SD),obtainedfromaregres- Robinson et al. / VALENCE-AROUSAL INTERACTIONS 1481 sion equation, revealed that the interaction was quite examining evaluation latencies. As expected, we × parallel to that reported above. observed a significantValence Arousal interaction in Inasecondmultipleregression,wesoughttotestthe Studies 2 and 3, such that negative/high arousal and robustness of the interaction with other factors con- positive/lowarousalslideswereevaluatedmorequickly trolled. In this equation, the dependent measure con- than their valence-matched counterparts. Study 7 sistedoflog-transformedevaluationlatencies.Predictor showedthataparallelpatternisapparentwhenpeople variables consisted of identification latencies and word evaluate word stimuli. frequency values in addition to z scores related to Althoughthepossibilityseemssomewhatunlikelyto valence, arousal, and their interaction. As in Study 6, us,particularlyaslatencieswereinvolvedandresponses identification latencies were a significant predictor of weremadeinthedark,itcouldbethecasethatpartici- evaluationlatencies,t=3.72,p=.00.However,frequency pantsfeltsomewhathesitantinreportingpositiveevalua- andvalencemaineffectswerenotsignificant,ts<1.Rep- tionsofslideswithinthepositive/higharousalcategory. licating the analyses reported above, there was a main Similar concerns,however,cannotbe offeredconcern- effectforarousal,t=2.59,p=.01,aswellasthehypothe- ingthecovertevaluationfindingsreportedinStudies4 × sized Valence Arousal interaction,t= 3.05,p= .00. and5.Inthesestudies,participantsmadenopubliceval- uationsoftheslides.Rather,slideevaluationdifficulties Discussion wereinferredfromperformanceonaneutralsecondary × Pictorialandwordstimulidifferinanumberofways, task. Because we observed the same Valence Arousal suchasimageabilityandcapacitytoinducestrongphysi- interactionsinthesestudies,weconcludethatevaluative ologicalactivity(Langetal.,1997).Inaddition,accessto processing was genuinely delayed in the case of word meaning obviously relies to a greater degree on negative/low arousal and positive/high arousal slides. symboliclinguisticactivity.Onemightthereforeexpect It is important to note that effects pertaining to the × some differences in the processing of pictures and Valence Arousalinteractionwereapparentbothwith words.Indeed,whereashigharousalpictureswereiden- participantastheunitofanalysis(Studies2-5and7)and tified more quickly (Study 1), high arousal words were with stimulus as the unit of analysis (Studies 6 and 7). identified more slowly (Study 7). A possible reason for Thelatterfindingsaddtothestrengthsofthearticlein thediscrepancyisthathigharousalstimuliboth(a)acti- thatcognitivepsychologistsareoftenmoreimpressedby vatetherighthemisphere(Robinson&Compton,2004) findingsthatreplicateacrossbothparticipant-leveland and(b)therighthemisphereisbetteratpictureprocess- item-level analyses. More important, however, the rele- × ing, whereas the left hemisphere is better at word pro- vant analyses were able to confirm that the Valence cessing(Hellige,1990).However,asystematiccompari- Arousalinteractionwasreplicatedwithinthecontextof son of word and picture processing was clearly not the continuousvariationsinvalenceandarousal,evenwith focus of the present studies and therefore this stimulus identification processes controlled. interpretation should be viewed with caution. Stimulus Arousal and Negative Connotation Moreimportantthantheidentification-relateddiffer- encesweretheresultsthatreplicatedacrossbothpicture Thepresentresults,althoughnovel,dovetailtosome × andwordstimulussets.Specifically,therewasaValence extentwithpriorresults.First,Langetal.(1997)report Arousal interaction such that high arousal facilitated that it is extremely difficult to find strongly negative evaluationsofnegativestimuliandinterferedwithevalu- slides that are low in emotional arousal. This is also ationsofpositivestimuli.Giventheparallelinteractions trueofemotionalwordsandsounds(Langetal.,1997). involvingtheevaluationofpicturesandwords,thecur- One inference from these prior norming data is that rentfindingscannotbeduetothenatureoftheparticu- valence and arousal covary in the real world such that larstimuliinvolved.Rather,abroaderprincipleappears negativestimulitendtobearousing.Ifthisistrue,itis to be operative. notcompletelyerroneoustoinferthat,pendingfurther analysis, an arousing stimulus is likely to be a negative GENERAL DISCUSSION one. Two, Lang (1995) has reported some preliminary dataindicatingthatnotallnegativeslideselicitthesame Major Findings level of affect-modulated startle responses. Specifically, Insevenstudies,wefoundconsistentsupportforthe stimulithatarenegativeandlowinarousal,suchasthose ideathatstimulusvalenceandarousalinteractivelyinflu- related to pity, tend to elicit smaller amplitude startle enceevaluativeprocessing.Study1firstestablishedthat responses thancould beexpected onthebasisoftheir × therewasnoValence Arousalinteractioninvolvingthe valence.Bycontrast,stimulithatarenegativeandhighin visual complexity of slides. This was an important null arousal,suchasthoserelatedtofear,tendtoelicitlarger result because it paved the way for subsequent studies amplitudestartleresponsesthancouldbeexpectedon
Description: