ebook img

Valence, arousal and dominance in the EEG during game play Boris Reuderink*, Christian Mühl ... PDF

18 Pages·2012·0.77 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Valence, arousal and dominance in the EEG during game play Boris Reuderink*, Christian Mühl ...

Int.J.AutonomousandAdaptiveCommunicationsSystems,Vol.6,No.1,2013 45 Valence, arousal and dominance in the EEG during game play Boris Reuderink*, Christian Mühl and Mannes Poel HumanMediaInteraction, FacultyofEEMCS, UniversityofTwente, P.O.Box217, Enschede7500AE,TheNetherlands E-mail:[email protected] E-mail:[email protected] E-mail:[email protected] ∗ Correspondingauthor Abstract: In this paper, we describe our investigation of traces of naturally occurringemotionsinelectricalbrainsignals,thatcanbeusedtobuildinterfaces that respond to our emotional state. This study confirms a number of known affectivecorrelatesinarealistic, uncontrolledenvironmentfortheemotionsof valence(orpleasure),arousalanddominance:(1)asignificantdecreaseinfrontal powerinthethetarangeisfoundforincreasinglypositivevalence,(2)asignificant frontalincreaseinpowerinthealpharangeisassociatedwithincreasingemotional arousal,(3)asignificantrightposteriorpowerincreaseinthedeltarangecorrelates with increasing arousal and (4) asymmetry in power in the lower alpha bands correlateswithself-reportedvalence.Furthermore,asymmetryinthehigheralpha bandscorrelateswithself-reporteddominance.Theselasttwoeffectsprovidea simplemeasureforsubjectivefeelingsofpleasureandfeelingsofcontrol. Keywords: affective computing; affective signal processing; emotion recognition;EEG;electroencephalogram;BCI;brain–computerinterfaces;alpha asymmetry; frustration; valence; arousal; dominance; adaptivecommunication systems. Reference to this paper should be made as follows: Reuderink, B., Mühl, C. andPoel,M.(2013)‘Valence,arousalanddominanceintheEEGduringgame play’,Int.J.AutonomousandAdaptiveCommunicationsSystems,Vol.6,No.1, pp.45–62. Biographicalnotes:BorisReuderinkreceivedhisMaster’sdegreein2007,after spendingtimeondifferentmachinelearningproblems,includingrecognitionof hand-writtentextonenvelopesandthedetectionoflaughterinaudio–visualdata. Brainsandintelligence–hislifelonginterest–canbecombinedhisPhDposition at the University of Twente, in which he focuses on making brain–computer interfacesfunctioninreal-worldsettingsforhealthyusers. ChristianMühlreceivedhisMaster’sdegreeinCognitiveSciencesin2007atthe UniversityofOsnabrück,Germany.Hiseducationwasfocusedonneuroscientific methods, specifically electroencephalography. Since then he is working as a research assistant in the Human Media Interaction Group at the University of Copyright ©2013InderscienceEnterprisesLtd. 46 B.Reuderink,C.MühlandM.Poel Twente,TheNetherlands.InhisPhDThesis,hesearchesforneurophysiological correlatesofaffectivestatesinthecontextofbrain–computerinteraction. MannesPoelisanAssistantProfessorintheHumanMediaInteractionGroupat theUniversityofTwente.Hismainresearchinvolvesappliedmachinelearning forvision-baseddetectionandinterpretationofhumanbehaviourandtheanalysis andclassificationofEEG-basedbrainsignals. 1 Introduction Abrain–computerinterface(BCI)offersadirectlinkbetweenthesignalsproducedbythe user’sbrainandacomputer,andisaimedtraditionallyatpatientssufferingfromamytrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS). But for healthy users, brain signals can also provide information thatcanbeusedtoaugmenttraditionalhuman–computerinteraction(HCI).Onesourceof information that a BCI may provide is an estimate of subjective emotions while they are beingexperienced. The enrichment of HCI by incorporating information about the emotional context of theinteractionortheuser’sbehaviourisstudiedinthefieldofaffectivecomputing(Picard, 1997).Affectiveinformationenablestheapplicationtorespondmoreadequatelytotheuser, e.g.byofferinghelpinsituationsofextremefrustration,stressormisunderstanding.Several modalitiesareabletoconveyinformationabouttheuserstatewithoutdirectlyintrudinginto theuser’sconsciousnessortaskathand,ase.g.theobservationofovertuserbehaviour(e.g. facialexpressionsorprosody)(Zengetal.,2007).Anothersourceofaffectiveinformation that is less dependent on overt behaviour is the group of physiological responses, such ascardiovascularresponses(Frazieretal.,2004;Langetal.,1993),electrodermalactivity (Codispotietal.,2001;Langetal.,1993)andmuscleactivity(Cacioppoetal.,1986;Magnée etal.,2007). Aspecificcaseofphysiologicalactivityisproducedbyneuronsintheneocortex.Similar totheaffectiveresponsesrecordedfromtheotherpartsofthebody,thebrainactivityhas beenstronglyassociatedwithperceptive,experientialandexpressiveemotionalprocesses (Demareeetal.,2005).Morespecifically,cognitivetheoriesofemotionsclaimthebrainto betheprimarysourceofaffectiveresponses,asitisprocessingtheverystimuli,memories or thoughts that lead to an affective reaction in brain, body and behaviour (Sander et al., 2005).Accordingtothosetheories,theuseofbrainsignalswouldenableafasterandmore direct recognition of emotional states, and be sensitive to more subtle emotions that do notcrossthethresholdofbodilyexpression.Asfirstdiscriminativeneuronalresponsesto emotionalstimulationcanalreadybeobservedwithintensofmilliseconds(Aftanasetal., 2002; Keil et al., 2001), brain activity responds faster to emotional stimulation than any otherphysiologicalmeasure.Thus,sensorsmeasuringneuronalactivityfromthebrainoffer arelativelynon-intrusivemethodoffastandcontinuousobservationoftheuser’saffective statei.e.notdependentonovertbehaviour. Severalstudieshaveshowntheprincipalcapabilitytoyieldestimatesofuserstateswith acceptable precision via physiological (Benovoy et al., 2008; Chanel et al., 2006, 2008; KimandAndré,2008;Kreibigetal.,2007;Liuetal.,2009;Mandryketal.,2006;Picard et al., 2001; Rainville et al., 2006) and neurophysiological signals (Chanel et al., 2006, Valence,arousalanddominanceintheEEGduringgameplay 47 2009;Khosrowabadietal.,2009;Koetal.,2009;Linetal.,2009;Murugappanetal.,2008; Rizonetal.,2008;Takahashi,2004).Correlatesofaffecthavebeenstudiedviaamultitude ofexogenousandendogenousaffectelicitationparadigms.Affectinductionprotocolshave madeuse, amongothers, ofpictures(Aftanasetal.,2001;Husteretal.,2009;Keiletal., 2001; Müller et al., 1999), sentences (Marosi et al., 2002), music (Sammler et al., 2007; SchmidtandTrainor,2001),videos(Aftanasetal.,2006;Krauseetal.,2000)andrecallof emotionalevents(Chaneletal.,2009).Theseneuralcorrelatescanbeusedinthedesignof anautomaticclassifierofaffect. Buttoensurethegeneralisationofthefindingsfromthe controlledcontextofthelaboratorytoareal-worldHCIcontext(ecologicalvalidity), the affectivestateisbestelicitedinawayresemblingthecontextofapplication. The goal of the present study is to find affective neurophysiological correlates of frustration, valence, arousalanddominanceinanecologicallyvalidcontext, inthiscase, game play. To evoke emotions, we introduced frustration by ignoring keyboard input periodically. Using a non-responsive controller is a well-known method in the fields of affectivecomputingandHCI(DienerandOertel,2006;Kleinetal.,2002;Scheireretal., 2002).Priorstudieshaveshownthatitisinprinciplepossibletoidentifyneurophysiological correlatesofaffectivestateselicitedbygamingevents(SalminenandRavaja,2007,2008) andwithHCIingeneral(SpiridonandFairclough,2009). Section 2 will introduce the reader to the relevant types of brain activity, results from theaffectiveneurosciencesandconsequently,specifichypothesesderivedfromtheseprior studies. 2 Emotionsandthebrain Emotions can be studied within the framework of the circumplex model (Russell, 1980). Thismodeldefinesatwo-dimensionalemotionalspace,spannedbyacontinuousvalence (pleasure)andanarousaldimension.Inthisspace,allcategoricalemotionallabelscanbe definedintermsofvalenceandarousal.Forexample,fearisassociatedwithastateofhigh arousalandnegativevalence,whereasexcitementischaracterisedbyahigharousalanda positivevalence.Athirddimension,nameddominance,canbeaddedtothisspacetosignify thesubjectivefeelingofcontrol(dominantvs.submissive).Dominancecanbeunderstood as a social or cognitive interpretation of an affective event (Russell, 1980), indexing the “interactive relationship that exists between the perceiver and the perceived [object or situation]’’(BradleyandLang,1994). Bymanipulatingtheaffectofusers,differencesinthisemotionalspacecanbemapped to specific regions of the brain. For example, processes related to emotion recognition are associated with the right hemisphere (right hemisphere theory), whereas positive and negativeemotionalstateshavebeenattributedtoregionsintheleftandrightfrontalcortices, respectively (the valence theory) (Silberman and Weingartner, 1986; Tucker, 1981). An alternativetothisvalencetheoryofemotionistheapproach/withdrawaltheory(Davidson, 1992), in which the different hemispheres are differently activated depending on the motivationaldirectionoftheemotionalstate:lefthemisphericfrontalactivityisassociated withapproach,whereasrighthemisphericactivityisassociatedwithwithdrawal.Asmost approach-related emotions are associated with a positive feeling and most withdrawal- relatedemotionswithanegativefeeling, thereisaconsiderableoverlapofboththeories. Thedefiningdifferenceliesinthehemisphericalactivationcausedbytheemotionofanger; whileitisanegativeemotion,itisassociatedwithanapproachbehaviour. 48 B.Reuderink,C.MühlandM.Poel Interestingly,thedominancedimensionenablesadifferentiationbetweenapproachand withdrawalemotionsthatcannotbedifferentiatedonthebasisofvalenceandarousalalone. Forexample,angerisaccompaniedbyhighdominance(senseofcontrol)ratings,whereas fear scores low on dominance (Demaree et al., 2005). This is especially important in the current study, as the induced frustration (low dominance) is close or might lead to anger (high dominance), which are both characterised by a negative valence and high arousal. Whileforthevalenceandarousaldimension,responsesintheelectroencephalogram(EEG) toaffectivestimulationhavebeenreported,fordominance,wecouldonlyfindHerazand Frasson (2007). Heraz and Frasson used broad-band power features in the delta, theta, alphaandbetabands,andcorrelatedcontinuousmeasurementswithself-reportedvalence, arousal and dominance. Weak correlations with dominance were found, but it is unclear whetherthereportedcorrelationsaresignificant,asnotallobservationswereindependent. Inthefollowing,wewillshortlyreviewliteratureontheassociationofemotionalprocesses andoscillatorybrainactivity. Thepreviouslyintroducedhemisphericvalencetheoryholdsthatpositiveemotionsare processed in the left frontal cortex, whereas negative emotions are processed in the right frontal cortex. As cortical activation is held to be inversely related to alpha (8–12Hz) activity(Pfurtscheller,1999),theprocessingofpositiveemotionalinformationisreflected in a decrease of alpha power of the left frontal hemisphere, the processing of negative emotional information is reflected by the decrease of alpha power over the right frontal hemisphere.Thispatternwasshowninstudiesusingawidevarietyofemotioninduction protocols (Allen et al., 2004; Davidson, 1992; Huster et al., 2009; Schmidt and Trainor, 2001).Thefrontalalphaasymmetryisthemostfrequentlyfoundcorrelateofvalence. Manyothercorrelatesforvalenceareobservedinalessrobustmanner.Forexample,the frontalasymmetrywasalsofoundinthethetarange(4–7Hz)inresponsetopositiveand negativepicturecontent(Aftanasetal.,2001)andsentences(Marosietal.,2002).Müller etal.(1999),alsopresentingpictures,didnotfindhemisphericinteractionswithvalencein thealphafrequencyrange, butinthegammarangeatthetemporallobes. Similarly, Keil etal.(2001)foundresponsesinthegammarangeforaversivepictures.Medialandfronto- medialeffectsofemotionwerefoundinseveralstudies.Anincreaseinfronto-medialtheta powerwasobservedforpositivemusicstimuli(Sammleretal.,2007)andduringmeditation (AftanasandGolocheikine,2001),whichwasinterpretedasemotionalprocessingclosely associated with attentional processes. Krause et al. (2000) found a power increase in the low-thetabandovermidlineelectrodesinresponsetoangerinducingmovies, supporting thenotionofanattention-relatedeffect.Thesameregionwasshowntobemoreactivein terms of delta (0–3Hz) oscillations when aversive stimuli were presented (Klados et al., 2009). Arousalactivatesneuralstructuresingeneral,andthereforeshouldbeassociatedwitha globaldecreaseofpowerinthealphaband(Barryetal.,2007,2009).Morelocalisedeffects inthealphabandarefoundinSchmidtandTrainor(2001)andAftanasandGolocheikine (2001),buttheeffectsseemtocontradicteachother;SchmidtandTrainorfoundacorrelation between the activation of frontal cortical regions and the perceived arousal of musical excerpts,whileAftanasetal.foundadeactivation(measuredasanincreaseinhigh-alpha power) in the frontal regions associated with increasing arousal. Other EEG phenomena observed at posterior sites with increasing arousal include the increase of power in low- frequencybands,asinthedelta(Kladosetal.,2009)andthetaband(Aftanasetal.,2002), thedecreaseofalphapower,andanincreaseinuppergammapower(Aftanasetal.,2004). Valence,arousalanddominanceintheEEGduringgameplay 49 3 Researchquestions BasedontheliteraturedescribedinSection2,weformulatedhypothesesontheinteraction of affective states and the EEG to be confirmed in an ecologically valid context for an adaptiveinterface. First, we describe the hypotheses for the valence dimension. In the delta band, we havethehypothesisHvδ:fronto-medialdeltapowerincreaseswithincreasingvalence.For the theta band, we define three distinct hypotheses: Hvθ1: with increasing valence, left hemispherical theta power increases, Hvθ2: with increasing valence, right hemispherical thetapowerdecreasesandHvθ3:withincreasingvalence,posteriorthetapowerincreases. Inthealphaband,wedefinethefollowinghypotheses:Hvα1:withincreasingvalence,left hemisphericalalphapowerdecreases, Hvα2: withincreasingvalence, righthemispherical alpha power increases. Finally, for the gamma band: Hvγ1: with increasing valence, left temporalgammapowerdecreasesandHvγ2:withincreasingvalence,righttemporalgamma powerincreases. For arousal, we formulate the hypotheses Haδ: posterior delta power increases with increasing arousal, Haθ1: posterior theta power increases with increasing arousal, Haα1: globalalphaactivitydecreaseswithincreasingarousal,Haα2:frontalalphapowerincreases with increasing arousal, Haβ1: posterior beta power is positively correlated with arousal and Haγ1: high arousal correlates with a power increase in the high-gamma band. These hypothesesaresummarisedinTable1. Table1 Hypothesesforvalence,arousalanddominancecorrelationsinthefrequencydomain Dimension Delta Theta Alpha Beta Gamma Valence↑ Hvδ:fron.-med.↑Hvθ1:l-hemi.↑ Hvα1:l-hemi.↓ – Hvγ1:l-temp.↓ Hvθ2:r-hemi.↓ Hvα2:r-hemi.↑ Hvγ2:r-temp.↑ Hvθ3:fron.-med.↑ Arousal↑ Haδ:posterior↑ Haθ:posterior↑ Haα2:global↓ Haβ:parietal↑Haγ:gamma↑ Haα1:frontal↑ Dominance↑ – – – – – 4 Methods Totesttheaboveformulatedhypotheses,thefollowingmethodologywasused: 4.1 Datacollection ToobtainEEGdatacontainingdifferentemotionalstates,weusedagamedesignedtoinduce frustrationforshortperiodsbyignoring15%ofthekeyboardinput(Reuderinketal.,2009). Intheblockswherefrustrationisinduced,thescreenperiodicallylagsaswelltosimulate an underpowered computer. The experiment runs through a sequence built from random permutationsoftwonormal,andonefrustrationblockof2mineach.Aftereachblock,the userisaskedtoratehiscurrentmentalstateontheemotionaldimensionvalence,arousal anddominancebypressinganumerickeydisplayedbelowtheSelf-AssessmentManikin (SAM)(BradleyandLang,1994),seeFigure1. 50 B.Reuderink,C.MühlandM.Poel Figure1 Thegame,andtheself-assessmentscreenforvalence,arousalanddominance (seeonlineversionforcolours) Note:In our analysis, we inverted the scale of valence and arousal, so that high valence correspondedtopositiveness,andhigharousaltoamorearousedstate. 4.1.1 Experimentalprocedure Users were asked to read and sign a form of consent, and were subsequently wired with theEEGandphysiologicalsensors.Theexperimenterbrieflyexplainedthegameandthe self-assessmentprocedure.Theparticipantwasallowedtopractisethecontrolsfor2min, andthentheexperimentwasstarted.Whenusersmentionedthatthegamewasunresponsive duringtheexperiment, theexperimenteraskedthemtocontinueplayingandpromisedto findthesourcelater.After30min,theexperimenterstoppedtheexperimentandtheusers weredebriefed. 4.2 Subjects Twelve healthy users (age 27±3.9) participated in the experiment. All participants had normal, or corrected to normal vision, and reported no use of medication. Only three of oursubjectswerefemale,andallsubjectswereright-handed.Mostparticipantshadsome experiencewithvideogames. 4.2.1 Sensorsandrecording ABioSemiActiveTwoEEGsystemwasusedtorecordtheEEGandphysiologicalsignals atasamplerateof512Hz.ForEEG,32Ag/AgClactiveelectrodeswereused,placedatthe Valence,arousalanddominanceintheEEGduringgameplay 51 locationsoftheExtendedInternational10–20system.Tomeasuretheinfluenceofocular andmuscularartefacts,werecordedtheEOG(horizontalandverticalpairs)andtwopairs ofEMGsignalsforthefingermovementusedtocontrolthegame.Additionalphysiological sensors,suchastemperature,respiration,thegalvanicskinresponseandthebloodvolume pulsewererecordedaswell,butnotusedinthepresentstudy. 4.3 Preprocessing RawEEGdatacancontainnoisefromtheenvironment,eyemovementsormuscletension. Weappliedthefollowingpreprocessingproceduretoreducetheinfluenceofthesesignals. First, the recording was decimated to 128Hz. After decimating, the channels were high- passfilteredusingafourthorderButterworthfiltertoremovefrequenciesbelow0.2Hz,and notch-filteredusingafourthorderButterworthfilterfrom49to51Hztoremovepowerline noise.TheEEGwassubsequentlycorrectedforeyemovementsusingthe(linear)regression analysissubtractionmethodofSchlögletal.(2007)basedonthecovariancebetweenEEG andEOGchannels.OnlyaftertheremovaloftheEOGinfluence,thedatawasre-referenced tothecommonaveragereference(CAR),thatsubtractstheaveragesignalfromeachsensor ateachtimepoint. 4.4 EEGfeatures Alpha asymmetries were calculated for lateral sensor pairs, using the procedure outlined inAllenetal.(2004):foreachexperimentalblock,Welch’smethodwasusedtoestimate power in different frequencies for each EEG-channel with 1Hz resolution. Within each block,wesummedthe(natural)log-powerinthealphaband(8–12Hz)foreachelectrode, and subtracted the band power of electrodes on the left hemisphere from corresponding electrodes on the right hemisphere. This results in an alpha-asymmetry index for each sensorpairforeachexperimentalblock,whichshouldcorrelatepositivelywithvalence. The same spectral estimates for each experimental block were used to calculate the correlationsbetweenlog-powerofnarrow-bandEEGoscillationsandSAMratings. Note thatwehavedeliberatelychosenforananalysisusingspectralbinsof1Hz,astheemotional response can differ for small differences in frequency bands (Krause et al., 2000). The drawbackofthischoiceisthatwehavetoperformmorestatisticaltests,andthattherefore theeffectshavetobequitestrongtopassthemultiple-testcorrection. 4.5 Statisticaltests Forthealphaasymmetry,wewillsimplyreportcorrelationsofthealphaasymmetryindex ofdifferentsensorpairs,withtheself-reportedratingsforvalence,arousalanddominance. StatisticalsignificanceisdeterminedwithaWilcoxonsigned-ranktestoverthecorrelations ofasensorpairswithemotionaldimensions.Wehavechosennottolimitoursignificance tests to only correlations with valence, but to include the frustration condition, arousal anddominanceaswell.Theseextratestsaremeanttobeillustrative;thereforewedonot performmultipletestcorrectiononthealpha-asymmetrycorrelations. For the narrow-band oscillations, we do perform multiple test correction using a combination of Bonferroni correction and Fisher’s method. Bonferroni correction conservatively corrects for performing multiple tests by dividing the critical value α by 52 B.Reuderink,C.MühlandM.Poel thenumberoftestsperformed.Butwhenalargenumberoftestsisused,itsconservative formulationresultsincriticalαvaluesthatcannotbepassedbyindividualstatisticaltests. This problem is well-known in neuroscience, and there are various statistical methods to overcomethisproblem,e.g.bycombiningevidenceoversubjects(Lazaretal.,2002). One method to combine evidence is to use the distribution of the p-values associated withmultiplestatisticaltestsinsteadofusingthedistributionofthecorrelationcoefficients ρ. Fisher’s method is suggested by Loughin for testing distributions of p-values for significance: (cid:2)k X2 =−2 logepi (1) i=1 where pi is the p-value for subject i. When all the null hypotheses are true, and all the p-valuesareindependent,X2followsaχ2distributionwith2kdegreesoffreedom. In our experiment, we have k = 12 independent subjects, therefore we compare the X2 ofthe12p-valuestoaχ2 distributionwith24degreesoffreedom.Notethatwenow combinethedifferentp-valuesfromdifferentsubjectsintoacombinednullhypothesisH , 0 that states that each of the individual null-hypothesis is true. The combined alternative, HA, is that at least one is not true (Loughin, 2004). While it is tempting to use Fisher’s methods not only over subjects, but also within the frequency band, we cannot because theobservationswithinafrequencybandarenotindependent.Bonferronidoesnotrequire independent p-values, and is therefore used to correct for the number of tests within a specificfrequencyband. To summarise: for the 12 subjects, the correlation coefficient ρ and it’s associated p-valueofthebandpowerataspecificsensorwithaemotiondimensionwascalculated. Within a frequency band (delta: 0–3Hz, theta: 4–7Hz, alpha: 8–11Hz, beta: 12–29Hz and gamma: 30+Hz), the one-sided p-values were combined over the 12 subjects using Fisher’smethod,andthesecombinedp-valuesweresubsequentlycorrectedforthenumber oftestswithintheband(α = 0.05/(2×32×b),wherebisthenumberof1Hzbandsin thefrequencyband). 5 Results 5.1 Self-assessments For each user, we have approximately 15 different measures of subjective valence, arousal and dominance. These self-assessments were rescaled to unit intervals for easy interpretation.Mostsubjectsratedthefrustrationinducingconditionlesspositivethanthe normalcondition,oversubjectsthisdifferenceissignificant(T =3,p <0.01).Whilewe expectedtofindatrendtowardsmorearousalinthefrustrationcondition,thereappearsto benodifference(T = 23,p = 0.26).Thedominancescaleindicatesthatpeopleseemto besignificantly(T =4,p <0.01)moreincontrolinthenormalcondition. Individualratingsforthevalenceanddominancedimensionsshowspreadvalueswith generally higher ratings for both valence and dominance in the frustration condition (Figure 2). Users enjoyed the game; therefore the negative emotions are slightly underrepresented. Valence,arousalanddominanceintheEEGduringgameplay 53 Figure2 Individualvalenceanddominanceratingsforthenormal(bluecircles)andfrustration (redsquares)condition(seeonlineversionforcolours) Note:Formostsubjects,theratingsarenicelyspreadwithhigherdominanceandvalencefor theratingsfortheblockswithfrustrationinduction. Table2 Thecorrelationamongexperimentalblocks(time), condition(frustrationinduction), valence,arousalanddominanceoftheself-assessments Time Cond. Val. Ar. Dom. Time 1.00 0.01 −0.04 0.07 0.01 Condition 0.01 1.00 −0.32 0.08 −0.32 Valence −0.04 −0.32 1.00 0.10 0.43 Arousal 0.07 0.08 0.10 1.00 −0.15 Dominance 0.01 −0.32 0.43 −0.15 1.00 Note:Therearenostrongcorrelationswithtime,butvalence,dominanceandtheexperimental conditionseemtobecorrelated. The correlation coefficients in Table 2 confirm the observed relation among ratings of valence, dominance and the frustration condition. While dominance and valence are supposed to be orthogonal emotional dimensions, they do not seem to be uncorrelated. Correlations with time are included as well to show trends of the self-assessments over time.Asexpected,theexperimentalconditionbarelycorrelateswithtime,andtheemotional ratingsdonotdriftovertime. Interestingly, the dominance ratings correlate more strongly with the valence ratings (ρ = 0.43) than with the experimental condition (ρ = −0.32). This shows that the dominance ratings do not simply capture the physical lack of control of the user, but the subjectiveexperienceofdominance. 54 B.Reuderink,C.MühlandM.Poel The non-orthogonality of the ratings complicate the interpretation of the results. In additiontotheoriginalratings,wealsoanalysedcorrelationswiththescoresofaprincipal component analysis (PCA) of these ratings. The loadings for the new dimensions are displayed in Table 3. More than half of the variance is explained by a component in the direction of normal keyboard control, and positive valence (happiness), which is not surprising as this is the main effect in our experiment. Orthogonal to this direction, we findasecondprincipalcomponent(PC)thatmodelsvarianceinthedirectionofthenormal conditioncombinedwithnegativevalence(sadness).Arousalanddominancedonotplay a big role in PC0 and PC1, but are modelled in PC2 (high dominance with low arousal, orsuperiority)andPC3(highdominancewithhigharousal,oranger)instead.Thesenew dimensionscanbeusedtodisentangleeffectsfoundforbothvalenceanddominance. Table3 The PCs for the space spanned by the frustration condition, valence, arousal and dominance PC0 PC1 PC2 PC3 Mainlypositive Mainlynegative Mainlyaroused emotionsand emotionsand Relaxedand anddominant controllablegame controllablegame dominantmental mentalstate Description (happiness) (sadness) state(superiority) (anger) Var.explained 52% 32% 11% 5% Condition −0.72 −0.70 0.05 0.00 Valence 0.66 −0.69 −0.13 −0.27 Arousal 0.06 −0.11 −0.67 0.73 Dominance 0.22 −0.17 0.73 0.62 5.2 Alphaasymmetries Foreachuserindividually,wecalculatedthealphaasymmetryindexasoutlinedinSection4 for each experimental block. Table 4 lists the Pearson correlation coefficients ρ between alphaasymmetriesandtheemotionalratings,averagedoversubjects.WeusedaWilcoxon signed-rank test to find ρ’s that deviate significantly from zero over subjects. While the correlationsarequitelow,wefindsignificantcorrelationsfortheunmodifiedvalenceand dominancescale.Anotableobservationisthatdominancecorrelatesbetterwiththealpha asymmetrythanvalenceoverthefronto-centralcortex.InthePCAspace,alphaasymmetry in the fronto-central is correlated more strongly with PC2, the PC that is associated with ‘superior’feelings(relaxedanddominant). For PC0 (happiness), we do find a correlation with alpha asymmetry for frontal alpha power(Fp1–Fp2).Forfrontalalphapower,wealsofoundasignificantnegativecorrelation with the condition variable, and a positive correlation with valence. PC0 is oriented in exactlythisdirection,soitisnotsurprisingthatwedofindasignificantpositivecorrelation ofalphaasymmetryinFp1–Fp2withPC0,butitremainsunclearwhetherthisasymmetry iscausedbyvalence,ormainlybytheexperimentalcondition. Ourresultsrevealedacorrelationoversubjectsofalphaasymmetrywithvalence,arousal anddominance.Traditionally,alphaasymmetryisassociatedwithvalence,butourresults suggestthatthereisastrongerlinkbetweenalphaasymmetryinfronto-centralsensorpairs withdominancethanwithvalence.

Description:
valence (or pleasure), arousal and dominance: (1) a significant decrease in frontal More specifically, cognitive theories of emotions claim the brain to .. Individual valence and dominance ratings for the normal (blue circles) and
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.