ebook img

Using Corpora to Analyze Gender PDF

235 Pages·2014·3.456 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Using Corpora to Analyze Gender

Using Corpora to Analyze Gender Also AvAilAble from bloomsbury An Introduction to Corpus Linguistics, David Oakey Corpus Linguistics and Linguistically Annotated Corpora, Sandra Kuebler and Heike Zinsmeister Using Corpora in Discourse Analysis, Paul Baker Using Corpora to Analyze Gender PAul bAKer LONDON • NEW DELHI • NEW YORK • SYDNEY Bloomsbury Academic An imprint of Bloomsbury Publishing Plc 50 Bedford Square 1385 Broadway London New York WC1B 3DP NY 10018 UK USA www.bloomsbury.com First published 2014 © Paul Baker, 2014 All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or any information storage or retrieval system, without prior permission in writing from the publishers. Paul Baker has asserted his right under the Copyright, Designs and Patents Act, 1988, to be identified as Author of this work. No responsibility for loss caused to any individual or organization acting on or refraining from action as a result of the material in this publication can be accepted by Bloomsbury Academic or the author. British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library. eISBN: 978-1-4725-2483-6 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data A catalog record for this book is available from the Library of Congress. Typeset by Newgen Knowledge Works (P) Ltd., Chennai, India CoNTeNTs Acknowledgements vi 1 Introduction 1 2 Gender difference redux: Do women really say ‘lovely’ more than men? 19 3 Nope, good guess though: How do female academics signal disagreement? 45 4 Male bias and change over time: Where are all the spokeswomen? 73 5 Discourse prosodies and legitimation strategies: Revisiting the Daily Mail’s representations of gay men 105 6 What are boys and girls made of? Using Sketch Engine to analyse collocational patterns 133 7 Triangulating methods: What can personal ads on Craigslist reveal about gender? 157 8 Conclusion 197 Notes 209 References 215 Index 225 ACKNoWleDGemeNTs I would like to express thanks to the following: The research presented in this paper was supported by the ESRC Centre for Corpus Approaches to Social Science, ESRC grant reference ES/ K002155/1. Members of the Research in Gender, Language and Sexuality group and the Corpus Research seminar group at Lancaster University, who provided useful feedback for earlier drafts of some of the analysis described in this book. Andrew Hardie, for providing me with male and female spoken data from the British National Corpus, to a somewhat exacting format. John Swales, Uta Romer and Matt O’Donnell for patiently answering my questions about the MICASE corpus. Paul Rayson, Mike Scott, Laurence Anthony and Adam Kilgarriff for providing corpus analysis software, without whom this book could not have been written. I would also like to thank Jane Sunderland and Judith Baxter, for their continuing support and good ideas over the years. CHAPTER ONE Introduction How many times did you say ‘I love you’ today? In 2012, four days before Valentine’s Day, I was contacted by a journalist who worked for an international newspaper. He was writing an article ‘on linguistic differences between men and women’ and was primarily interested in ‘outlining the main linguistic differences, along with sentences/ paragraphs as spoken by men/women to illustrate such differences’. He wanted me to give him a list of some gendered differences and he cited a study by Harrison and Shortall (2011), which had surveyed 171 college students and found that males report falling in love earlier than women and saying ‘I love you’ earlier. I replied to the journalist, saying that it is difficult to create such a list as the amount of data needed would be enormous. We would require millions of words of spoken language data, taken from large numbers of people from a wide range of backgrounds and locations. It would be a good idea to sample data at various points in time to ensure that any differences we found were stable rather than being due to something specific about a particular period of a particular society. With regard to the paper that the journalist had cited, I suggested that perhaps we cannot generalize too widely from a study which used a relatively small number of participants (99 women and 72 men) who were of similar age and current circumstances (students) and asked to remember and then report on their own linguistic behaviour (Harrison and Shortall also refer to some of these issues in the discussion section of their article1). To illustrate, I sent the journalist some information about the phrase ‘I love you’ in the British National Corpus (BNC), a large reference corpus consisting of 100 million words of British English of which 10 million words are transcriptions of recorded conversations. For about 71 per cent of this conversational data we know whether the speaker was 2 USING CORPORA TO ANALYZE GENDER male or female. Although the BNC can only directly tell us about language use in British society at the point in time that the data was collected (the early 1990s), as one of the biggest sources of naturally occurring spoken language data, at the time of writing, it is still one of the best resources that corpus linguists have access to. I found that I love you only occurred 64 times in the spoken part of the BNC, and while the female speakers in the corpus said it about three times as much as the males, the majority picture was that most of the speakers did not say ‘I love you’ (at least when they were being recorded). I offered, humorously, that people should perhaps be encouraged to say the phrase more often. Unsurprisingly, the journalist didn’t reply. I had not produced a list of words and phrases which would either confirm stereotypes about gendered language use or refute them. Either way, such a finding could have constituted ‘news’. Instead, my response could be summarized as ‘there is not enough evidence to draw much of a conclusion’. February 14th was approaching and my response was unlikely to cohere with any narrative that the journalist wanted to create. From gender difference to gendered discourses The above anecdote is illustrative of a dissonance between academic research in the field of Gender and Language over the last 20 years or so, and public/media perceptions of gender and language. But this discrepancy was not always the case. The ‘gender differences paradigm’2 was actually an early academic approach, linked to Lakoff’s (1975) ‘male dominance’ theory of language use (the view that males used language to dominate women). Fishman (1977) expanded on and contributed to this theory by proposing that women engaged in what she memorably called ‘interactional shitwork’, which among other things involved using questions and hedges to force responses from men in order to facilitate conversation. While Lakoff and Fishman focused more on the notion of male dominance than gender difference per se, there was an underlying assumption that for men to be dominant and women to be dominated, then the sexes must also use language differently. Towards the end of the 1980s, another approach, popularized by Tannen (1990) emphasized gender differences rather than male dominance. This was a perspective influenced by interactional sociolinguistics and broadly based around the view that males and females had distinct and separate ‘genderlects’ which resulted in ‘cross-cultural miscommunications’. Tannen argued that men viewed conversation as a contest, whereas women used conversation to exchange confirmation and support. On the surface, this ‘difference’ paradigm could be seen as a more politically neutral and thus uncontroversial3 way of thinking about gender INTRODUCTION 3 and language. In eschewing second-wave4 feminist claims of patriarchal dominance, ‘gender difference’ does not characterize men as oppressors and women as victims, nor does it position anybody’s language use as ‘superior’ to anybody else’s. The difference paradigm instead views males and females as growing up in largely separate speech communities and learning different ways of socializing and using language. Linguistic gender differences are therefore used to ‘explain’ interpersonal conflict within (heterosexual) couples. Such conflicts are said to be due to misunderstandings as males and females attach different meanings to the same utterances as well as having different needs. Some proponents of the paradigm claim that the sexes need to be educated in order to understand each other’s language. ‘Difference’ is thus a ‘grand’ theory, simple to grasp, blame-free and offering an explanation and solution to couples’ conflict that is widely applicable. It is easy to see why it has become so popular, particularly in the media, spawning numerous relationship ‘self-help’ books and newspaper articles about amusing linguistic gender differences that seem to confirm what we already knew or suspected about men and women. But while the gender differences paradigm is popular in the media, within academia, there has been a considerable amount of disagreement over whether men and women actually do use language differently to a significant degree, with some researchers arguing that differences do exist (e.g. Locke 2011), and others indicating that linguistic gender difference is a myth (e.g. Cameron 2008). Among those who argue for difference, there are a range of views about where such differences come from – perhaps they can be attributed to essential biological differences relating to chemicals in the brain, different reproductive systems or body musculature and size which can all impact on how people come to see themselves and are viewed by others. Possibly the differences are related to the ways that society socializes males and females differently, with different expectations regarding appropriate language behaviour for boys and girls. In the 1990s, taking a post-structuralist perspective, Judith Butler proposed that gender is performative – a form of doing rather than a form of being, so rather than people speaking a certain way because they are male or female, instead they use language (among other aspects of behaviour) in order to perform a male or female identity, according to current social conventions about how the sexes should behave. Butler pointed to female impersonators, showing that gender performances can be subverted and are therefore not intrinsically linked to a single sex. People learn what their correct gender performance for their sex should be by observing and copying other people around them. Therefore Butler (1990: 31) notes, ‘The parodic repetition of “the original”. . . reveals the original to be nothing other than a parody of the idea of the natural and the original.’ Butler also links gender performance to sexuality, referring to a ‘heterosexual matrix’ (ibid.: 5). She argues that ‘. . . for bodies to cohere and make sense there must be a stable sex expressed through a

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.