ebook img

US and Indonesian Children's Descriptions of Relational Aggression PDF

71 Pages·2015·2.41 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview US and Indonesian Children's Descriptions of Relational Aggression

Illinois Wesleyan University DDiiggiittaall CCoommmmoonnss @@ IIWWUU Honors Projects Psychology Spring 2000 UU..SS.. aanndd IInnddoonneessiiaann CChhiillddrreenn''ss DDeessccrriippttiioonnss ooff RReellaattiioonnaall AAggggrreessssiioonn:: GGeennddeerr,, DDeevveellooppmmeenntt aanndd CCuullttuurraall CCoommppaarriissoonnss Elizabeth A. Jansen '00 Illinois Wesleyan University Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.iwu.edu/psych_honproj Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Jansen '00, Elizabeth A., "U.S. and Indonesian Children's Descriptions of Relational Aggression: Gender, Development and Cultural Comparisons" (2000). Honors Projects. 81. https://digitalcommons.iwu.edu/psych_honproj/81 This Article is protected by copyright and/or related rights. It has been brought to you by Digital Commons @ IWU with permission from the rights-holder(s). You are free to use this material in any way that is permitted by the copyright and related rights legislation that applies to your use. For other uses you need to obtain permission from the rights-holder(s) directly, unless additional rights are indicated by a Creative Commons license in the record and/ or on the work itself. This material has been accepted for inclusion by faculty at Illinois Wesleyan University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. ©Copyright is owned by the author of this document. • Relational Aggression 1 Running Head: U.S. AND INDONESIAN CHILDREN'S DESCRIPTIONS u.s. and Indonesian Children's Descriptions of Relational Aggression: Gender, Developmental, and Cultural Comparisons Elizabeth A. Jansen Advisor: Dr. Doran C. French Spring 2000 Department of Psychology Illinois Wesleyan University Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment ofthe requirements for Research Honors at Illinois Wesleyan University. • Relational Aggression 2 Acknowledgements I want to thank Dr. Doran French for his extensive help with the preparation and revision of this manuscript. In addition, I would like to acknowledge Dr. Linda Kunce, Dr. Peter Verbeek, and Dr. Charles Springwood at Illinois Wesleyan University for their assistance serving as thesis committee members and revising the manuscript. Also, I want to thank Andrea Fosco, Kari Adkins, Elizabeth Lohse, Kelly Ellis, Alison Lawton, and Amy Dinardo at Illinois Wesleyan University for their work collecting and coding the data. Similarly, I want to thank Meta Rianasari, Sri Pidada, and Peter Nelwan at Padjadjaran University and Dr. Doran French at Illinois Wesleyan University for collecting and giving me permission to use the data that I analyzed as a part of this study. Relational Aggression 3 Abstract Previous studies of aggression in childhood have found that boys, as a group, are more aggressive than girls. The majority of these studies, however, focus only on physical aggression. Recently several studies have been conducted that differentiate relational aggression from physical aggression. Relational aggression involves harming others through the purposeful damage to their peer relationships (i.e., spreading rumors or ostracizing a peer from a group activity). Several studies have found sex differences in relational aggression, as well as physical aggression. The present study explores gender, developmental, and cultural differences and similarities in relational, physical, and verbal aggression in US and Indonesian children and adolescents' free descriptions of disliked peers. As hypothesized, the results of logistic regressions indicated that males were more likely than females to describe physical aggressive behavior, while females were more likely than males to describe relationally aggressive behavior. These results were found across cultures and age groups. This study extends the research on relational aggression by utilizing a new methodology for cross-cultural research on relational aggression. • Relational Aggression 4 Descriptions of Peers' Relational Aggression in U.S. and Indonesian Children and Adolescents: Gender, Developmental and Cultural Comparisons A popular children's nursery rhyme by Mother Goose goes, "What are little girls made of? Sugar and spice, and everything nice. That's what little girls are made of." This poem suggests that females may be less aggressive than males, but is that true in the playgrounds and backyards of America and abroad? One needs to look no further than the realm of Mother Goose nursery rhymes to find a contrasting viewpoint. "There was a little girl who had a little curl that hung in the middle of her forehead; when she was good she was very very good, but when she was bad she was horrid." This poem suggests that girls, too, can be bad, but how are little girls bad? Are they bad in the same way as little boys? The current study is designed to assess gender differences in aggression. In addition, we will expand this to look at the extent to which gender differences in indirect aggression are seen in different cultures and in children and adolescents. Prior to outlining the study, relevant issues in the literature will be explained. After reviewing basic definitions and typologies of aggression, the concept of relational aggression will then be explored as this is the major focus of the proposed study. Next, gender, developmental, and cultural differences in the aggression literature will be reviewed and methodological issues will be discussed. Finally, the proposed study that will focus on gender, developmental, and cultural effects on relational aggression among U.S. and Indonesian children and adolescents, will be described. Definitions of Aggression Although aggression has been the topic of much research in both social and developmental psychology, there is controversy about the definition of aggression. Aggression has been defined variously as a natural instinct, a behavior that harms another person, and a social label that we apply to different behaviors depending on our judgments about the meaning ofthose acts (Shaffer, 1994). The lack ofconsensus about the definition of aggression is due in part to the question of whether or not intention to do harm constitutes an essential feature of aggression. Some have argued that because it is difficult to accurately judge others' intentions, aggression should be defined solely on the basis of • Relational Aggression 5 the results of the action and references to intended motives should be avoided (Schaffer, 1996). For example, Arnold Buss (1961) defined aggression as "a response that delivers noxious stimuli to another organism" (p. 3). More recently, Leonard Eron defined aggression similarly as "an act that injures or irritates another person" (Brannon, 1996, p. 209). Both of these definitions focus on the behavior or consequences of aggression and not intentionality. Other researchers have argued that aggression based solely on the consequences of the actions is ambiguous because it prohibits distinguishing between aggression and accidental or non-malevolent administration of noxious stimuli. Based on Buss S or Eron's definitions of aggression, both a dentist's I filling a cavity and a person accidentally tripping another person would be considered aggressive acts. In response to these concerns, other researchers have defined aggression as "any form of behavior designed to harm or injure another living being" (Shaffer, 1994, p. 327). Note, however, that even including intentionality does not completely resolve the ambiguities ofthe aggression concept. Brannon (1996) noticed that even among researchers who include intentionality in their definitions of aggression, there is no consensus about which behaviors should be included as aggressive. Therefore, the literature on aggression has been complicated by inconsistencies in definition. There have been two major approaches to studying aggression. Some researchers have focused on theory generation and testing, e.g., ethology, psychoanalytic theory, and social learning theory. An alternative approach has been to develop typologies of aggressive behavior. In the next section, various typological approaches to aggression will be briefly outlined due to the relevance ofthe typological approach to the proposed study. Typological Approach to the Study of Aggression Largely atheoretical, typological analysis refers to the process of developing classification schemes that attempt to subdivide types of behavior. Over the years, a number of such typologies have been proposed for aggression. • Relational Aggression 6 One typology of aggression focused on differentiating hostile (reactive) and instrumental (proactive) aggression. Feshbach (1964), was one of the first to distinguish between these two types of aggression. According to Feshbach, hostile aggression is aggression that originates from anger, whereas instrumental aggression is initiated to accomplish a specific goal. Hartup (1974) continued the work on this typology. According to Hartup (1974), hostile aggression is defined as acts for which the major goal is to inflict harm on another person (e.g., tripping someone so that they will fall down and get hurt). In contrast, instrumental aggression refers to those actions that although aggressive in form and potentially harmful to another person, are motivated by goal-directed intentions (e.g., pushing another child in an attempt to get a toy the child was playing with). This classificatory system is based specifically on a distinction of intentionality ofthe behaviors. In one ofhis studies using this typology, Hartup (1974) found a decrease in the frequency of instrumental aggression during the four to seven year-old period, whereas no developmental effects for hostile aggression were found. Other typologies of aggression have avoided the concept of intentionality. Ethologist W. McGrew, for example, described agonistic behaviors (which included both aggressive behaviors and other types ofoppositional behaviors) by categorizing the behaviors themselves and not the motivations behind them. Some behavior patterns that McGrew coded in agonistic situations includes the following: beat ("overarm blow with palm side ofthe lightly clenched fist"), object beat ("beat with object held in hand"), pinch ("thumb and forefinger forcibly opposed with object or part ofbody in between"), punch ("arm is moved rapidly from horizontal position at side, forward 180 degrees in sidearm motion"), open punch ("punch with hand open, slap"), push ("arms extended forward with wrists flexed, force applied"), and kick ("leg is flexed then rapidly extended at knee and hip, usually oriented toward person or object") (McGrew, 1972, p. 70). One ofthe most frequently used typologies in the study of aggression differentiates between verbal and physical aggression. Most classification systems used to study childhood aggression have distinguished between verbal and physical aggression (see Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974, for a review). For • Relational Aggression 7 example, ethologists Archer and Westeman (1981), used the categories of verbal and physical aggression to study gender differences in the aggressive behavior of British schoolchildren. Another frequently used typology differentiates between direct and indirect aggression. Although used by many researchers over the years, indirect and direct aggression have been defined in a number of ways. Buss (1961) was one of the first researchers to make this distinction. According to Buss (1961 ), direct aggression involves direct confrontation between aggressor and target, while indirect aggression involves harming other people without confronting them directly, thus, avoiding counterattack. Indirect aggression could be verbal (i.e. spreading malicious rumors) or physical (stealing someone's notebook). The indirect aggression subscale on the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory published in 1957, included such behaviors as spreading gossip, banging on the table, pouting, and playing practical jokes (Richardson & Green, 1997). Some researchers have suggested that Buss's definition of indirect aggression was too broad and included behaviors that were not necessarily aggressive. Feshbach (1969), one of the first researchers to conduct observational studies of children's aggressive behavior,.defined indirect aggression somewhat differently than Buss. According to Feshbach (1969) indirect aggression is "a response which results in pain to a stimulus person through rejecting and excluding him" (p. 250). Indicators of indirect aggression included the following behaviors: ignoring (paying no attention to an approach), avoiding (moving away), refusals (denying requests for help or play), and exclusion (actively rejecting). In contrast to Buss's definition, which included behaviors such as pounding on the table, Feshbach only included behaviors that have a social target. Recently, more than fifteen years after the work by Buss and Feshbach, there has been renewed interest in typological analysis of children's aggression. This has been stimulated by with Crick and her colleagues' (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995; Crick, Bigbee, & Howes, 1996) modification ofthe concept of indirect aggression that has been described and researched by a group of Finnish researchers (Lagerspetz, Bjorkqvist, & Peltonen, 1988; Bjorkqvist, Lagerspetz, & Kaukiainen, 1992). This led to the subsequent development ofthe category of relational aggression (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). Crick and • Relational Aggression 8 colleagues divided aggression into two categories. The first category, relational aggression, focuses on harming others by purposely damaging their peer relationships and includes behaviors such as spreading malicious rumors, excluding friends from a play group, and trying to get other children not to play with a certain peer. All other aggressive behaviors, which do not fall under relational aggression, constitute the second category of aggression. One reason why this work has received so much attention is because of the gender differences in aggression that have emerged when distinctions between relational and non­ relational aggression are established. Because of the importance ofthis work for the present study, the next section will outline Crick's model in detail. Relational Aggression Crick and Grotpeter (1995), defined relational aggression as "harming others through purposeful manipulation and damage of their peer relationships" (p. 711). Crick and her colleagues redefine the typology of direct and indirect aggression as relational and overt aggression. According to Crick and Bigbee (1998), "relational aggression harms others through hurtful manipulation of their peer relationships or friendships, whereas overt aggression harms others through physical damage or the threat of such damage" (p. 337). Items on Crick and colleagues' peer nomination inventory assessing relational aggression include the following: "Tells friends they will stop liking them unless friends do what they say"; "When mad at a person, ignores them or stops talking to them"; and "Tries to keep certain people from being in their group during an activity or play time" (p. 713). Like Feshbach's concept of indirect aggression (i.e., "rejecting and excluding") and unlike Buss's definition of indirect aggression (i.e., "avoiding counterattack"), Crick and Grotpeter's concept of relational aggression includes some behaviors that directly confront the target and others that avoid confrontation. Recently, Crick and colleagues have stopped using the term overt aggression (Crick et aI., 1999). This change in terminology helps to clarify the issue that relational aggression itself can be either covert (spreading malicious rumors) or overt (purposeful exclusion of someone from a social group). Instead of the overt versus relational dichotomy, Crick and her colleagues (1999) have contrasted relational aggression with physical aggression which they define as harming "through damage or threat • Relational Aggression 9 of damage to another's physical well-being" and verbal aggression which they describe as threats to another's physical well being or personal insults (Crick et aI., 1999, p. 77). The key distinction between relational aggression and both verbal and physical aggression is that relational aggression is the only one that specifically focuses on damage to relationships. Table 1 provides examples of the terminological distinctions made by Crick and her colleagues. No one has clearly defined or labeled the category of aggressive behavior that can be differentiated from relational aggression. For lack of a better term, I will use the term non-relational aggression to refer to physical aggression and verbal aggression that is not focused on harming relationships. In an attempt to be consistent with the terminology used by Crick and her colleagues (1999) in their recent chapter on relational aggression, in the present study I will use the term verbal aggression to refer to non-relational verbally aggressive behavior (i.e., direct verbal insults and verbal threats of physical harm). Relationally aggressive behaviors which are verbal in form (i.e., gossiping) will be excluded from the category of verbal aggression. It is important to note, however, that much of the previous research on verbal aggression did not make a distinction between relational and non­ relational forms of verbal aggression. As described by Crick et ai, (1999), however, relational aggression has consistently emerged as a factor separate from non-relationally aggressive behaviors (both verbal and non-verbal) during factor analyses of aggression questionnaire items (Crick, Casas, & Mosher, 1997). Similar Typological Systems of Aggression Several researchers have proposed typological systems very similar to those of Crick and her colleagues. Many of these earlier typological systems influenced the development of Crick and her colleagues' category of relational aggression. In the late 1980's, a group of Finnish researchers including Lagerspetz, Bjorkqvist, and Peltonen (1988), defined indirect aggression as behaviors that "exploit social relations among peers in order to harm the person at whom the anger is directed" (Lagerspetz, Bjorkqvist, Peltonen, 1988 p. 409). In 1992, Bjorkqvist, Lagerspetz, and Osterman developed a questionnaire to assess indirect aggression called The Direct & Indirect Aggression Scales (DlAS) (Bjorkqvist, 1994). Included in this measure were questions regarding behaviors such as arguing,

Description:
After reviewing basic definitions and typologies of aggression, the concept of relational . Crick and Grotpeter (1995), defined relational aggression as "harming Indonesian sample were transcribed and translated into English by
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.