ARTICLE 1PW0Ei.nR1k1Si7Oe7lNm/A0a1Ln4I 6eT1tY 6a A7l.2 N/0D 4A2 FS7FO1E3CC0I9TAILV PES PYRCIHMOINLOGGY BULLETIN Unconscious Affective Reactions to Masked Happy Versus Angry Faces Influence Consumption Behavior and Judgments of Value Piotr Winkielman University of California–San Diego Kent C. Berridge University of Michigan Julia L. Wilbarger University of Wisconsin–Madison The authors explored three properties of basic, unconsciously beverage? Second, how do basic affective reactions in- triggeredaffective reactions: Theycan influence consequential teract with motivation? Third, can affective reactions behavior,theyworkwithoutelicitingconsciousfeelings,andthey influence behavior without being accessible to con- interactwithmotivation.Theauthorsinvestigatedtheseproper- scious awareness? In investigating these questions, we tiesbytestingtheinfluenceofsubliminallypresentedhappyver- integratesocialpsychologicalapproachestoaffectwith susangryfacesonpouringandconsumptionofbeverage(Study contemporary biopsychological models of incentive 1),perceptionofbeveragevalue(Study2),andreportsofcon- motivation. We suggest that such integration can ad- sciousfeelings(bothstudies).Consistentwithincentivemotiva- vance emotion theory as well as social psychological tiontheory,theimpactofaffectiveprimesonbeveragevalueand research on attitudes, judgment, and persuasion consumptionwasstrongestforthirstyparticipants.Subliminal (Cacioppo & Gardner, 1999; Niedenthal & Kitayama, smiles caused thirsty participants to pour and consume more 1994). beverage(Study1)andincreasedtheirwillingnesstopayand To elicit basic affective reactions, researchers often theirwantingmorebeverage(Study2).Subliminalfrownshad employ subliminal presentations of emotional facial the opposite effect. No feeling changes were observed, even in expressions.Emotionalfacialexpressionsareimportant thirstyparticipants.Theresultssuggestthatbasicaffectivereac- social stimuli, and their processing appears to involve tions can be unconscious and interact with incentive motiva- biological affect programs (Ekman, 1984). Subliminal tiontoinfluenceassessmentofvalueandbehaviortowardval- presentationsminimizecontributionsofcomplexcogni- enced objects. tiveprocessesandreducestrategicresponding(Bargh, Authors’Note:Fortheirgenerousassistanceatvariousstagesofthe Keywords: affect; emotion; priming; motivation; consciousness; project, we are grateful to Hart Blanton, Gerald Clore, Phoebe introspection Ellsworth, Kent Harber, Jan Keenan, Daniel McIntosh, Yuko Munakata,RichardNisbett,BrucePennington,DollyRenick,Oliver C Schultheiss, Jeanne Shinskey, Norbert Schwarz, Diederick Stapel, omplex emotions can result from an extended se- ValerieStone,ElliotValenstein,andRobertZajonc.Theworkonthis quence of cognitive appraisals (Ellsworth & Scherer, projectwassupportedbytheNationalScienceFoundationGrantBCS- 2003). However, basic affective reactions may only 0217294toPiotrWinkielman.Correspondenceshouldbeaddressedto involveminimalprocessingandbeelicitedbysubliminal PiotrWinkielman,DepartmentofPsychology,DepartmentofPsychol- ogy,UniversityofCalifornia,SanDiego,9500GilmanDr.,LaJolla,CA stimuli (Zajonc,2000).Inthisarticle, we address three 92093-0109; e-mail: [email protected]. questionsaboutsuchbasicaffectivereactions.First,can PSPB,Vol.31No.1,January2005 121-135 theyinfluenceconsequentialbehaviortowardahedonic DOI:10.1177/0146167204271309 stimulus,asinthecaseofconsumptionofanunfamiliar ©2005bytheSocietyforPersonalityandSocialPsychology,Inc. 121 122 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN 1996).Severallinesofresearchsuggestthatsubliminal avoidancemovements(Chen&Bargh,1999).However, facial expressions elicit basic affective reactions with there is less evidence that affective stimuli influence both judgmental and physiological consequences. more complex behaviors, especially when such stimuli Niedenthal(1990)documentedthatsubliminalexpres- are presented subliminally. However, subliminal cogni- sions influenced preference ratings for cartoon draw- tivestimulicaninfluencerelativelycomplex behaviors, ings.MurphyandZajonc(1993)exposedparticipantsto suchasselectionofagamestrategy(Neuberg,1988)or subliminal or supraliminal expressions that varied in decision to interrupt a conversation (Bargh, Chen, & either valence (happy vs. angry) or gender (male vs. Burrows, 1996). Note, however, that cognitive stimuli female).Undersubliminalpresentations,valenceofthe exert this influence by biasing the interpretation of an expression influenced judgments of subsequent ideo- ambiguoustargetsituation.Thus,theinfluenceofacog- graphs,butundersupraliminalpresentationsonlygen- nitiveprimerequires asemanticconnectiontothetar- der information influenced judgments. Using facial get (Higgins, 1996). In contrast, as we explain shortly, electromyography,Rotteveel,deGroot,Geutskens,and theinfluenceofanaffectiveprimemayonlyrequirethat Phaf (2001) found more frowning to ideographs pre- thetargetengagesaffectiveandmotivationalprocesses, cededbyangrythanhappyfaces,butonlyaftersublimi- even if the prime and target do not share a semantic nal, not supraliminal, presentations. Neuroimaging connection. studies suggest that subliminal angry and fearful faces Inthecurrentstudies,weexaminedthispossibilityby activatetheamygdalaandrelatedlimbicstructures,pre- testingtheimpactofsubliminalpresentationsofhappy sumablyviaadirectpathwayfromthevisualthalamusto andangryfacesontheactionsofpouringandconsum- theamygdala(Morris,Öhman,&Dolan,1999;Whalen ingabeverage.Weselecteddrinkingforseveralreasons. etal.,1998).Interestingly,supraliminalexpressionsacti- First, basic affective and motivational processes should vate limbic structures more under implicit processing havethemostinfluenceonabehaviortowardahedonic conditions, when participants classify faces on gender, stimulus—a stimulus with some initial value. In fact, ratherthanexplicitprocessingconditions,whenpartici- drinking is often used in social and biological psychol- pantsclassifyfacesonexpression(Critchleyetal.,2000). ogy to explore such basic processes (Edwards, 1990; Littleisknownaboutthenatureofaffectivereactions Laeng, Berridge, & Butter, 1993; Strahan, Spencer, & elicited with different types of subliminal expressions, Zanna,2003).Second,itisimportanttoexplorewhether butitappearsthatsuchreactionsareorganizedmainly theimpactofaffectiveprimingcangobeyondratingsof on a positive-negative dimension (Zajonc, 2000). simple drawings and influence a consequential behav- Accordingly, effects of happy versus angry subliminal ior.Byconsuminganunfamiliarbeverage,apersonvol- expressions on judgments combine additively with untarilyingestsasubstancethatispotentiallybeneficial effects of other basic affect inductions, such as mere orharmful.Third,documentingapossibleinfluenceof exposure(Murphy,Monahan,&Zajonc,1995).Further- affective primes on drinking is particularly informative more,changesinglobalpositive-negativejudgmentsof becausethevalueofabeverageisintrinsicallypredeter- stimuli can be obtained with different negative facial minedbyitstasteandothersensoryproperties.Ifaffec- expressions,suchasangerordisgust(Murphy&Zajonc, tive primes can overcome such predetermination, it 1993; Niedenthal, 1990). Finally, under brief presenta- would suggest a robust underlying affective reaction. tion conditions, participants have trouble extracting Finally,drinkingallowsonetoassesstheimpactofaffec- morethangeneralnegativityfromdifferentexpressions, tiveprimingwith“real-world”unitssuchasvolumeand such as anger, fear, disgust, or sadness (Murphy, 2001). price. In sum, the extant research on effects of subliminal Affect and Motivation expressions suggests that they elicit basic positive- negativeaffectivereactionsandthatthesereactionscan Previous research has focused on the role of basic have immediate judgmental and physiological conse- affect in preference judgments (Zajonc, 2000). How- quences.Buildingonthisresearch,thecurrentstudies ever, recent advances in biopsychology highlight the employed subliminal expressions to address several close connection between affect and motivation. Tra- questions about the nature of basic affective reactions. ditional theories of motivation postulated that hedonic behavior (behavior toward stimuli with affective value) Affect and Behavior wasdrivenbyneedstates(Hull,1951).Forexample,peo- Facilitation of adaptive behavior has long been con- pledrinktoreducetheunpleasantstateofthirst.Incon- sidered an important function of the affect system trast,contemporarybiopsychologicaltheoriesofmotiva- (Frijda, 1999). There is evidence that affective stimuli tionpositthathedonicbehaviorislargelydeterminedby influencestartlereflexes(Lang,1993),aswellassimple the stimulus’ incentive value: whether the stimulus trig- instrumental actions, such as immediate approach- gersapositive-negativeaffectiveresponseandpromotes Winkielman et al. / AFFECTIVE PRIMING 123 approach-avoidance motivation. That is, modern theo- strong notion of unconscious cognition (Kihlstrom, riescombineaffectandmotivationinthesameexplana- Mulvaney,Tobias,&Tobis, 2000).Anunconscious affect, toryframework(Toates,1986).Motivationalstates,such in the stronger sense, would be a reaction caused by as thirst, are still important, but they work by directly valenced stimuli and with valenced behavioral conse- influencingaffectiveandmotivationalresponsestothe quences,whichnonethelessisnotsubjectivelyfelt,even relevantfeaturesofthestimulus.Thisinfluencecanbe upon introspection. observed in a phenomenon known as alliesthesia— Several psychologists and neuroscientists have pro- change in incentive value as a function of a relevant posedtheexistenceofunconsciousaffectiveprocesses, motivational state (Cabanac, 1971). For example, peo- basedonstudiesofanimals,patients,drugaddicts,and ple perceive taste of water more favorably and want it people under hypnotic analgesia (Berridge, 1999; morewhentheyarethirsty,butnotwhentheyarehungry Kihlstrometal.,2000;Lang,1993;LeDoux,1996).How- (Rolls, Rolls, & Rowe, 1983). Neuroscientific research ever,unconsciousaffectivereactionshavenotbeendem- highlightsthecloseconnectionbetweenaffectandmoti- onstrated in normal adult human participants. One of vation postulated by incentive theories. For example, thefewindicationsofunconsciousaffectcomesfroma animal studies suggest that the mesolimbic dopamine studythatfoundmodulationofparticipants’preference system,thenucleusaccumbens,andtheamygdalasup- ratings by subliminal happy or angry expressions port both affective and motivational responses (Winkielman, Zajonc, & Schwarz, 1997). Despite (Berridge, 1996). Human neuroimaging studies also changesintheirpreferences,inexitinterviews,partici- show activation of these systems to incentive stimuli, pantsdeniedexperiencingchangesinconsciousmood, such as drugs or money (Knutson et al., 2001) and, eventhoughtheywerealertedtosuchapossibility.Fur- important for the current studies, beverages (Berns thermore,whengivenanalternativeattributionfortheir etal.,2001)andemotionalfacialexpressions(Critchley mood changes, participants still showed the affective et al., 2000; Whalen et al., 1998). priming effect. Nevertheless, failure to report an emo- Thetightlinkbetweentheaffectiveandmotivational tion or change its attribution might be due to inatten- systemssuggeststhatstimuli,suchasfacialexpressions, tion, lack of motivation, or memory distortion. thatelicitbasicaffectiveresponsesshouldmodulatethe A more convincing demonstration of unconscious incentivevalueofasubsequenthedonictarget,suchasa affect requires obtaining on-line ratings of conscious beverage,eventhoughtheseeventsareostensiblyunre- feelings immediately after subliminal exposure to the lated.Thus,wepredictedthatexposuretohappyexpres- valenced stimulus. Thus, in current studies, we asked sionsshouldtemporarilyincreasetheincentivevalueof someparticipantstoratetheirmomentarymoodimme- thebeverageandfacilitateconsumption,whereasexpo- diatelyafteraseriesofsubliminalexpressions.Ifpartici- sure to angry expressions should temporarily decrease pantsdidnotreportanymoodchangeimmediatelyafter the incentive value of the beverage and suppress con- the subliminal exposure, it would be difficult to argue sumption.Furthermore,wepredictedthatthemodula- thattheirfailuretodosowasduetoattentionormemory tionofincentivevalueshoulddependonthepreexisting deficits, especially if they subsequently demonstrated motivational state of the individual, just as the normal behavioralandjudgmentalconsequencesoftheiraffec- incentive value of a drink is modulated by preexisting tive reaction. To ensure that participants had a fair thirst (Rolls et al., 1983). If such a modification is ob- chancetodetectanyinfluenceontheirfeelings,wegave served, it would suggest that preferences are jointly them two scales tapping various aspects of emotional determined by basic affective processes, influenced by experience. As described in more detail later, Study 1 subliminal expressions, and basic motivational pro- usedatwo-itemscaleaskingparticipantstoreporttheir cesses, influenced by a physiological need. current mood on a broad positive-negative dimension and their current arousal. Study 2 used a Positive and Affect and Awareness NegativeAffectSchedule (PANAS)scalecontaining20 Can affective reactions to subliminal expressions adjectives to assess mood using differentiated terms occurwithoutawarenessofthosereactions?Mostscien- (e.g.,excited,scared,irritated)orusingageneralpositive- tistsagreethatatriggeringaffectivestimuluscanbeun- negativeindex.ThePANASscalehasgoodreliability,is conscious but assume that the resulting affective reac- sensitivetochangesovertime,andisconsideredoneof tionisitselfconscious(e.g.,Clore,1994;Öhman,Flykt, the best measures of current mood (Watson, Clark, & & Lundqvist, 2000; Zajonc, 2000). In fact, the scien- Tellegen, 1988). Finally, we interviewed participants at tific term unconscious affect is typically used to refer to theendofthestudyabouttheiraffectivereactionsand the unconsciousness of affect induction, not to the re- testedwhethertheycoulddetectthesubliminalprimes. sulting affective state. However, a stronger notion of Across these measures we predicted no effect on con- unconscious affect may be considered, similar to the scious feelings, despite evidence of affective influence 124 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN ondrinkingmeasures.Thispredictionwasbasedonthe ateabeverage.Tominimizedemandcharacteristics,par- assumption that subliminal facial expressions activate ticipants received instructions via computer and re- low-level affect mechanisms that can function inde- corded their ratings on an anonymous questionnaire, pendently from mechanisms underlying subjective ex- with the experimenter out of view. After the study, all perience,asobservedinourearlierwork(Winkielman participants were debriefed and thanked. et al., 1997). Initial motivational state. Before participants were introduced to the computer task, they completed a OVERVIEW “background”questionnaireratingtheircurrentlevelof In summary, the current studies were designed to thirst(0=notatall,11=verythirsty)andhunger(0=notat addressthefollowingquestions.First,canbasicaffective all, 11 =very hungry). reactionstriggeredbysubliminalexpressionsinfluence Sequence of experimental events. Figure 1 shows the the pouring and consumption of a beverage? Second, sequence of experimental events. Participants were does motivation (thirst)change theimpact ofaffective primedwithsubliminalexpressionsembeddedinagen- primes? Third, do subliminal facial expressions alter der classification task and then performed a beverage conscious feelings? We investigated these questions in task and a mood rating task, as explained shortly. To two studies using a modified version of the subliminal establishabaselineofparticipants’drinkingandrating affectiveprimingparadigm.Inbothstudies,participants responses, the experiment started with a set of eight first rated their motivational state—thirst and hunger. baseline trials with neutral primes, followed by a set of Next,participantswereexposedtoaseriesofsubliminal eighttesttrialsinwhichprimevalencewasmanipulated happyorangryfacesinataskrequiringgenderclassifica- between participants. Participants then received tionofsupraliminalneutralfaces.Afterpriming,partici- anothersetofbaselinetrialsfollowedbyanothersetof pants received a hedonic stimulus—a fruit-flavored testtrialswithexpressionsofreversedvalencefromthe drink. We chose this specific drink because previous initialtest.Nosignificanteffectsemergedintheselater studiesshowed motivationalmodulation ofitshedonic testtrialsinStudy1orStudy2,sotheyarenotfurtherdis- value,suggestingthatemotionalexpressionscouldexert cussed. The absence of priming on the later test trials a similar effect (Laeng et al., 1993). Study 1 examined couldreflecthabituationofaffectiveresponsestofacial affective influence on behavior toward the beverage. expressions(Wrightetal.,2001)orresistancetoinduc- Participantsfreelypouredthemselvesthebeverageand tionofoppositevalenceoncestimulusvalencehasbeen consumedasmuchastheywanted.Study2testedaffec- established(Murphy&Zajonc,1993;Winkielmanetal., tiveinfluenceonperceptionofthebeverage’svalue.Par- 1997). ticipantstastedasmallpredeterminedsampleofthebev- erageandthenratedit.Inbothstudies,presentationof Priming trials: Gender classification task. Subliminal thebeveragetaskwascounterbalancedwithself-reports expressionswereembeddedina“gender-classification” ofcurrentfeelings,oneithera2-itemscale(Study1)ora task,inwhichparticipantsreportedthegenderofeasily 20-item scale (Study 2). We predicted the following: visible neutral faces. Each trial started with a forward First,theimpactofaffectiveprimesshouldbemostpro- mask(50-mscross),followedbyasubliminalprime(16- nounced on variables tapping the beverage’s hedonic ms expression), which was immediately replaced by a value,suchasconsumption(Study1)andratingsofbev- backward mask (400-ms neutral male or female face). eragedesirability(Study2).Second,theimpactofaffec- Each baseline and test priming sequence contained tiveprimesshoulddependonmotivationalstate(thirst). eight trials with subliminal expressions of the same Third, the impact of affective primes should occur valence(i.e.,allhappy,allneutral,orallangry),withdif- without a change in conscious mood. ferentindividualfacesoneachtrial.Wechoseeighttrials pereachprimingsequencebecausethisnumberofsub- STUDY 1 liminalexpressionsappearssufficientforatransientacti- vationofthelow-level affectivecircuitry,whereas more Method trialsleadtohabituation(Whalenetal.,1998).Wechose Participants.Thirty-nineundergraduates(14men,25 happy and angry faces for three reasons. First, these women, mean age = 22 years) gave informed consent expressionsworkedinpreviousaffectiveprimingstudies and participated in individual sessions for extra credit. (Murphy&Zajonc,1993;Winkielmanetal.,1997).Sec- Uponarriving,participantslearnedthatthestudyinves- ond,itiseasiertoextractnegativevalencefrombriefly tigated how “biological rhythms influence reaction presentedexpressionsofangerthanfromfear,disgust, times,sensations,andmood”andrequiredthemtoper- or sadness (Murphy, 2001). Third, we did not want to formarecognitiontask,toratetheirmood,andtoevalu- elicit a specific consumption-related reaction, such as Winkielman et al. / AFFECTIVE PRIMING 125 Figure 1 Sequence of events in Study 1 testing the influence of subliminal expressions on consumption behavior and feelings. NOTE:Participantswereprimedwithsubliminalfaces(baselinetrials=8neutralexpressions;testtrials=8happy,8neutral,or8angryexpressions) embeddedinataskrequiringgenderclassificationofvisibleneutralfaces.Primingtrialswereimmediatelyfollowedbyeitherratingsofmoodand arousal or by presentation of the drink (in counterbalanced order). disgust, but rather a general positive-negative affective right now, at this very moment?” (–5 = unpleasant, 5 = reaction. pleasant)and“Howmucharousaldoyoufeelrightnow, Thefacestimuliwere24neutralfaces,whichservedas atthisverymoment?”(–5=low,5=high).Inthebeverage primesandmasks,and8happyand8angryfaces,which task,participantsreceivedanopaquepitchercontaining servedonlyasprimes.Ofthese40faces,20werefemale 600 ml of a beverage prepared with water, sugar, and and20male,and20wereJapaneseand20Caucasian.All lemon-lime-flavored Kool-Aid powder. To ensure that came from the Japanese and Caucasian Facial Expres- thedrinkappeared novelaftereachsetofthepriming sions of Emotion (JACFEE) set by Matsumoto and trials,thepitcherpresentedaftereachsetofprimingtri- Ekman (1988). Images were 8 × 8 cm, shown in gray alscontainedabeveragemadeoffourdifferentpropor- scale, centrally on a 15-inch monitor, 50 cm from the tionsofsugarandpowder,counterbalancedacrosspar- participant, using PsyScope software on a McIntosh ticipants.Participantspouredasmuchofthebeverageas computer. theywantedintoa250-mlcupandconsumedasmuchas theywanted.Unknowntotheparticipants,theamounts Beverage and feelings task. After the gender classifica- poured and consumed were recorded using an elec- tion task, participants rated their current feelings and tronic scale. After pouring and drinking, participants performedabeveragetask,incounterbalancedorder.In gavefourratings:“Howdeliciousisthisdrink?”(0=not thefeelingstask,weaskedparticipants,“Howdoyoufeel delicious,10=extremelydelicious);“Howmuchwouldyou 126 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN liketodrinkrightnow?”(0=none,5=2pints);“Howwell entered the baseline response, thirst, and priming. In did it quench your thirst?” (0 = not at all, 10 = extremely thesecondblock,weenteredthecrossproductofthirst well); and “How much would you pay for a can of this andpriming.Thefirstregressionblockrevealedonlya drink?” (1 =10 cents, 10 =1 dollar). main effect for prior pouring levels: Participants who poured more during the baseline phase also poured Prime perceptibility task. A forced-choice recognition moreduringthevalencedphase.Thenonstandardized task evaluated participants’ awareness of 16 emotion regression coefficient indicated that for every 1-ml expressions presented during the test sequences increaseinpouringduringthebaselinephase,pouring (Winkielmanetal.,1997).Oneachof16trials,apartici- inthevalencedphaseincreasedby.32ml,t(35)=2.06, pantwasfirstsubliminallyflashedanemotionalexpres- p< .05. sion masked by a neutral face. Immediately afterward, Thesecondregressionblockrevealedaninteraction theparticipantsawtwofacespresentedfor2secondson betweenthirstandpouring,B=6.52,t(34)=3.02,p<.01. theleftandrightpartofthescreen (oneofwhichhad The interaction is shown in the top panel of Figure 2, beenthesubliminalexpression)andwasaskedtodecide whichplotstheamountpouredasafunctionofpriming whichfaceswerepresentedintheflash.Performanceon andthirst,controllingforbaselinepouring.Atthehigh thistestwas51.6%,whichwasnotsignificantlydifferent thirst level (1 SD above the sample mean for thirst), fromchance,t(38)=1,p=.30.Regressiontestsrevealed pouringincreasedasafunctionofpriming,goingup21 no relation between performance in the forced-choice mlbetween angry(37ml),neutral(58ml),andhappy testandtheinfluenceofsubliminalprimingonthecriti- (79ml)primes,B=21.31,t(34)=3.53,p<.01.Thatis, cal dependent variables, including pouring, drinking, thirstyparticipantspoured114%moreofthebeverage and mood (ts < 1). Thirst level did not correlate with afterhappyprimesthanafterangryprimes.Atthemean forced-choiceperformance(r=.05).Theseresultssug- levelofthirst,pouringexhibitedamarginallysignificant gestthatconsciousrecognitionwasnotresponsible for increase as a function of priming, going up 7.52 ml the observed effects of emotional expressions. between angry (47ml), neutral (55ml), and happy Results (62ml) primes, B = 7.52, t(34) = 1.80, p < .08. That is, moderatelythirstyparticipantspoured32%moreofthe Descriptive statistics and analytic method. We analyzed beverageafterhappythanangryprimes.Atthelowlevel data using linear regressions with valenced phase re- ofthirst(1SDbelowthemeanofthirst),thedifference sponses(pouring,drinking,mood,arousal)ascriterion between priming conditions was not significant, B = variablesandbaselinephaseresponses,primingcondi- –6.27,t(35)<1.Insummary,thirstyparticipantspoured tion,andthirstlevelaspredictorvariables.Ourcoding morethantwicetheamountofthebeverageafterhappy for linear regression analyses assumed equal intervals primesthanangryprimes,butprimingdidnotinfluence between angry and neutral and between neutral and pouring of nonthirsty participants. happyconditions(–1=angry,0=neutral,1=happy,with higher scores indicating more positive valence). An Amountconsumed.Inthebaselinephase,participants alternative strategy was to dummy code separately for drank27.06ml(SD=22.76)or48%ofwhattheypoured. angryandhappyprimingconditions,whichallowsinde- Inthevalencedphase,participantsdrank26.79ml(SD= pendentcomparisonsoftherelativeimpactofangryver- 26.44) or 50% of what they poured. To determine sus happy priming on the criterion variable. However, whether priming and thirst influenced drinking in the thiscomparisonwasnotcentraltothecurrentresearch, valenced phase, we regressed valenced phase drinking andwedidnotcollectalargeenoughsamplesizetopur- ontobaselinedrinking,thirst,priminginthefirstregres- sue selective comparisons between angry and happy sionblock,andthecrossproductofthirstandprimingin primes. Exploratory analyses did reveal, however, that the second block. The first block revealed only a main angryandhappyprimesinteractedwiththirstinoppo- effect for prior drinking: Participants who drank more site directions. This result is consistent with our theo- before priming also drank more after priming. The retical framework and our coding strategy assuming nonstandardized regression coefficient indicated that equal intervals. forevery1-mlincreaseindrinkingbeforepriming,post- Amount poured. Participants poured 56.21 ml (SD = primedrinkingincreasedby.58ml,t(35)=3.47,p<.001. 24.63)ofthebeverageduringtheneutralbaselinephase As with pouring, the second block revealed an interac- and53.92ml(SD=23.19)inthevalencedtestphase.To tionofthirstandprimingondrinking,B=6.02,t(34)= investigate the predictors of pouring, we regressed 2.59, p < .01. This interaction is shown in the bottom valenced-phase pouring onto baseline-phase pouring, panelofFigure2.Athighlevelofthirst,primingcaused thirst,priming,andthecrossproductofthirstandprim- anincreaseindrinking,goingup17mlfromangry(12 ing. In the first block of a hierarchical regression, we ml)toneutral(29ml)andtohappy(46ml)primes,B= Winkielman et al. / AFFECTIVE PRIMING 127 Figure 2 Amountofbeverageinmilliliterspoured(toppanel)and Figure 3 Ratingsofmood(toppanel)andarousal(bottompanel)asa consumed(bottompanel)asafunctionofsubliminalprim- functionofsubliminalprimingandinitialthirst.Moodand ing and initial thirst. arousalwereeachmeasuredona–5to+5scale,rangingfrom NOTE: Values are estimated by linear regression (priming coded: positive-negative to low-high, respectively. Values are esti- angry=–1,neutral=0,happy=+1)atdifferentthirstlevels(low=1SD matedbylinearregressionandadjustedforbaselinelevels. belowthemean,mean=samplemean,high=1SDabovethemean) and adjusted for baseline levels. participants’moodtrendednonsignificantly(p=.35)in 17.11,t(34)=2.73,p<.01.Inpercentages,thirstypartici- theprime-incongruentdirection(2.34afterangry,2.02 pants drank 280% more of the beverage after happy after neutral, and 1.70 after happy primes), and their primes than after angry primes. At the mean level of patternforarousalwasnearlyflat(0.36afterangry,0.38 thirst,primedidnotsignificantlyincreasedrinking,B= after neutral, 0.40 after happy primes). 4.38,t(34)<1.00(23mlafterangry,27mlafterneutral, Median-splitonthirst.Thejustpresentedanalysesused 32 ml after happy primes, representing an 18% differ- linearregressionstoestimatetheprimingeffectatthree ence).Finally,atthelowlevelofthirst,primingalsodid levels of thirst.The results showed thatathigh level of notinfluencedrinking,B=–8.36,t(35)=1.22,p<.24,34 thirst,affectiveprimes reliably influenced participants’ ml after angry, 26 after neutral, and 17 ml after happy consumptionwithoutinfluencingtheirfeelings.Similar primes). In summary, thirsty participants drank more resultsholdwhenthedataareanalyzedbydividingpar- beverage after happy primes than angry primes, but ticipants into two independent groups using a median nonthirsty participants did not. splitonthirst.Toillustratethis,Figure4plotstheprime effect on consumption behavior and subjective experi- Feelingratings.Equivalentanalysesalsoassessedmood ence for participants who scored at or above the thirst and arousal ratings. We regressed valenced-phase feel- median. As shown, priming influenced consumption ing ratings onto baseline-phase feeling ratings, thirst, behaviorofthirstyparticipants(leftpanel),butnottheir priming,andthecrossproductofthirstandprime.The subjective experience (right panel). results,showninFigure3,revealedonlyamaineffectof prior levels. Participants who reported more positive Correlations between consumption and feeling measures. moodinthebaselinephasealsoreportedmorepositive Finally,weanalyzedthecorrelationsbetweenfeelingrat- moodinthevalencedphase,B=1.09,t(35)=10.35,p< ings and consumption behaviors during the valenced .01.Likewise,participantswhoreportedmorearousalin phase.Mooddidnotcorrelatewithpouring,r(39)=.03, the baseline phase also reported more arousal in the ordrinking,r(39)=.05.Similarly,arousaldidnotcorre- valencedphase,B=.939,t(35)=21.5,p<.01.Nomain late with pouring, r(39) = .01, or drinking, r(39) = .01. effects or interaction of priming and thirst occurred, Moreimportant,theselowcorrelationswerenotdueto regardlessofthelevelofthirst,evenwhenfeelingswere thelackofvariabilityorsensitivityofoursubjectivereport rated immediately after the presentation of valenced measures. This is indicated by a significant correlation priming(beforedrinking).Infact,thepatternforthirsty betweenarousalandmood,r(39)=.52,p<.01,replicating 128 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN Figure 4 Means and standard errors for measures of consumptionbehavior(pouringand drinking)and subjectiveexperience(mood and arousal) among thirsty participants. NOTE: Values are for participants at or above median thirst and are adjusted for baseline levels. the previously documented associationbetweenmoder- atearousalandpositivemood(Thayer,1996). tive state, even when their mood was assessed imme- diately after the subliminal primes. Nonthirsty partici- Beverage ratings during the baseline phase. To assess pants’ pouring and drinking did not change after sub- whether participants’ motivational state influenced liminal priming. Apotentially puzzling findingwasthe theirperceptionoftheinitialvalueofthedrink,wecor- absenceofprimingeffectsonbeverageratings,obtained relatedtheirratingsofthebeverageduringthebaseline after participants finished drinking as much as they phase with their initial level of thirst. Thirst correlated wished.Afterall,previousstudiesreportedeffectsofsub- positively with ratings of the beverage’s deliciousness, liminalexpressionsonratingsofvisualpatterns,suchas r(39) = .39, p < .05, and its thirst-quenching ability, cartoons or ideographs (Murphy & Zajonc, 1993; r(39) = .36, p < .05. This effect represents an enhance- Niedenthal,1990;Winkielmanetal.,1997).Thediscrep- mentofstimulusvaluebyappetitestate(alliesthesia),as ancy could be due to procedural differences between documented previously (Cabanac, 1971). previous studies and our study. First, previous studies Beverage ratings during the valenced phase. As the final exploredimmediateimpressionsofvisualpatternspre- task,afterpouringandconsumingthebeverageandrat- sented for only 1 or 2 seconds. In contrast, we allowed ingsubjectiveexperience, participantsratedthebever- participants to pour and consume as much and for as age.Analysesrevealednomaineffectofprimingandno longastheywantedbeforegivingratings,resultingina interactioneffectofprimingandthirstonbeveragerat- greaterdelaybetweensubliminalprimingandtargetrat- ings.Weaddressthisabsenceofbeverageratingeffects ing. If subliminal priming influences only the initial in the next study. impressions,thenitseffectcouldhavedissipatedbythe timeourparticipantsratedthedrink(foradiscussionof Discussion thisissue,seeZajonc,2000).Second,thirstyparticipants’ Study1resultsconfirmedourpredictionsthatsublim- consumption of different amounts (after happy versus inalfacialexpressionswouldalterconsumptionandthat angryfaces)mighthaveconfoundedanyprimingeffect theirinfluencewoulddependonrelevantmotivational ondrinkratings,especiallyifadditionalconsumptionof state.Inparticular,thirstyparticipantspouredmoreand a moderately tasty beverage depressed subsequent rat- drankmoreofabeverageafterexposuretohappyfaces ings.Third,we foundthatthestrongestpriming effect but poured less and drank less afterexposure to angry required high thirst. This raises the possibility that let- faces. Remarkably, despite these changes in behavior, ting participants quench their thirst before rating the thirstyparticipantsreportednochangeintheirsubjec- beveragecaneliminatetheprimingeffectonratings.To Winkielman et al. / AFFECTIVE PRIMING 129 addresstheseissues,Study2modifiedtheprocedurefor sensoryaspectsofthebeverage,thequestionaboutthirst assessment of beverage ratings. quenchingwasreplacedbythequestion,“Howsweetis this drink?” (0 =not sweet at allto 10 =extremely sweet). STUDY 2 Moodratings.Participantsratedtheiraffectiveexperi- ThegoalofStudy2wastoreassesstheeffectofsublim- enceonthePANASquestionnairebyindicatingona5- inalprimingonbeverageratings.IncontrasttoStudy1, point scale how they felt “right now, at this very participants did not pour the drink themselves but moment,” using thefollowing 20 items: interested, dis- insteadreceivedafixedandsmallquantityofthebever- tressed, excited, upset, strong, guilty, scared, hostile, agetosamplewithonlyonesip.Inaddition,becausepar- enthusiastic, proud, irritable, alert, ashamed, inspired, ticipantsdidnothavetopourthebeveragethemselves, nervous,determined,attentive,jittery,active,andafraid they tasted it immediately after priming. We predicted (anchors ranging from 1 = slightly or not at all to 5 = that under these conditions, subliminal affective prim- extremely). ingshouldmoreclearlyinfluenceperception ofbever- Prime perceptibility task. The subliminal presentation agevalue.Wealsousedamoredifferentiatedmeasureto was verified using the same methods as in Study 1 and examine the impact of subliminal expressions on con- again did not significantly exceed chance (54%). Nei- sciousmood.Thetwo-itemglobalscaleusedinStudy1 therwasthescoreintheforced-choicetestsignificantly capturesonlylimitedattributesofaffectiveexperience. relatedtotheinfluenceoffacialexpressionsoncritical So,perhapssomeaspectofmoodchanged,butthescale dependentvariables(ratingsofdrinkvalueandratings did not allow participants to report it. Therefore, in of mood) or on level of thirst (ts < 1). Study2,weusedaPANASquestionnairetomeasure20 nuancesofexperience(Watsonetal.,1988).Thisscale Results providedamorestringenttestofourhypothesisthatsub- Willingnesstopay.Participantsindicatedtheirwilling- liminal priming induces an affective reaction that is nesstopayforahypotheticalcanofthebeverage ona introspectively inaccessible. The design of Study 2 was scalerangingfrom10centsto1dollar(U.S.).Afterthe otherwise similar to that of Study 1. Participants first valencedprimingphase,participantswerewillingtopay ratedtheircurrentlevelofthirstandhunger.Next,they 31 cents. Because priming influenced only immediate wereexposedtoeightsubliminalhappyorangryfacial reactions in Study 1, we tested whether the effects dif- expressionsembeddedinthegenderclassificationtask. feredbetweenparticipantswhoratedthedrinkimmedi- Onthefirstsetofprimingtrials,allsubliminalfaceswere atelyafterprimingandparticipantswhofirstcompleted neutraltoestablishabaselineofratingresponses.Inthe the intervening mood questionnaire. A multivariate secondsetoftrials,wemanipulatedthevalenceofsub- analysis of variance (MANOVA) assessed the effects of liminal expressions between participants. To simplify independent variables prime (happy vs. angry) and thedesign,theprimesincludedhappyandangryexpres- order(beverageratingfirstvs.moodratingfirst),aswell sions,butnotneutralexpressions.Aftereachsetofeight as the covariate of baseline willingness to pay, on the primingtrials,participantsperformedabeveragerating dependent variable of postpriming willingness to pay. task or a mood rating task in counterbalanced order. Theanalysisrevealedaprime-by-orderinteraction,F(1, Method 28)=4.41,p<.05.Testofsimpleeffectsrevealedthatpar- ticipantswhoratedthebeveragefirstwerewillingtopay Participants. Twenty-nine undergraduates gave in- 37 cents after happy primes and 19 cents after angry formedconsentandparticipatedforextracreditinapsy- primes, F(1, 28) = 4.72, p < .04. For participants who chology course (6 male students, 23 female students, rated mood first,priming had no influence on willing- mean age = 20 years). After completing the study, all ness to pay (33 vs. 34 cents, p = .44). In short, affective participants were debriefed and thanked. primingnearlydoubledthedrink’smonetaryvalue,but Beverage sampling and ratings. Consumption was re- this effect was short-lived and only occurred when strictedbyofferingparticipantsonlya40-mlbeveragein participants responded immediately after priming. an 80-ml cup. Participants took a single sip and then A MANOVA testing for the interaction of priming, answered the following questions. Three questions order, and thirst (as measured by our continuous 11- assessedperceptionofthehedonicandincentivevalue pointscaleonbeverageratings)revealedonlyamargin- of the beverage: (a) “How delicious is the drink?” (b) allysignificantprime-by-thirsteffect,F(2,28)=3.43,p= “How much of this drink would you like to drink right .07.However,becausesuchinteractionwassignificantin now?” and (c) “How much would you pay for a can of Study1,andbecausethestatisticalpowerofStudy2was this drink?” (scale anchors as in Study 1). To assess low compared to Study 1, we performed linear regres- whetheraffectivepriminginfluencedperceptionofthe sionstestingforeffectsofprimingatdifferentlevelsof 130 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN primingatdifferentthirstlevels.Linearregressionanal- yses revealed a significant effect of priming for participantsathighthirstlevel,whowantedtodrink1.02 unitsmore afterhappyprimes thanafterangryprimes (2.0,orabouthalf-cupversus0.98,orabout1-2sips),B= 1.02,t(28)=2.16,p<.05.Atthemeanthirstlevel,there wasanonsignificantincrease of.48unitsinwantingto drinkmoreafterhappythanangryprimes(1.48vs.1.0), B=.48,p=.16.Atthelowthirstlevel,primehadnoeffect onwantingtodrinkmore(.98vs.1.03),B=.05,p=.92. No such effects were obtained with hunger (ts < 1). In summary,thirstyparticipantswantedtodrinkmoreafter beingprimedwithhappyversusangryfacialexpressions, but nonthirsty participants did not. Otherratings.Subliminalaffectiveprimingdidnotin- fluence participants’ ratings of liking or sweetness. We return to this issue in the discussion. Moodratings.Table1presentsPANASratingsbyprim- ingconditionanditemtype.Weexaminedthepriming influenceonmoodinseveralways.First,weanalyzeda Figure 5 Ratingsofthedrinkasafunctionofsubliminalprimingand subsetofpositiveitems(α=.83).Second,weanalyzeda initial thirst. subsetofnegativeitems(α=.84).Third,weanalyzeda NOTE:Toppanel:willingnesstopay(incents)amongparticipants global mood index, created by subtracting negative whoratedthebeveragefirst.Bottompanel:wantingformorebever- age,regardlessoforder(scalevalues:none,1-2sips,half-cup,1cup,1 items from positive items (α = .72, mean after happy = pint,and2pints).Valuesareestimatedbylinearregressionatdifferent 1.18,SE=.08,meanafterangry1.14,SE=.08).Priming thirst levels and adjusted for baseline levels. influencednoneofthesemoodscores,regardlessoftask thirst.Thoseregressionstreatedthevalenced-phaserat- order and thirstlevel. Finally, we analyzed the priming ingsascriterionvariablesandbaseline-phaseratingsas effect on each of the 20 mood scale items. At the high covariates.Primingwascodedsothathigherscoresindi- andmeanlevelofthirst,participantsshowednoprime cated more positive valence (0 = angry, 1 = happy). effectsonanymooditem.Atthelowlevelofthirst,partic- ThetoppanelinFigure5showsthepatternofresults. ipantsshowedonemarginaleffect(p=.08),ratingthem- Atthehighlevelofthirst(1SDabovethemean),partici- selves as slightly more irritated after angry (1.55) com- pantswerewillingtopay28centsmoreafterhappythan paredwithhappyprimes(0.97).But,again,atthehigh angryprimes(38vs.10cents),B=.28,t(13)=2.25,p= thirstlevel,therewasnohintofthiseffect(p=.67),with .05.Atthemeanlevelofthirst,participantswerewilling participants rating their irritation as lower after angry topay17centsmoreafterhappythanangryprimes(36 (1.59)thanhappyprimes(1.77).Furthermore,asingle vs. 19 cents), which was marginally significant, B = .17, marginaleffectatlowthirstshouldbeinterpretedwith t(13)=2.03,p<.08.Atthelowlevelofthirst(1SDbelow caution given there were 20 mood ratings. As a whole, themean),participantswerewillingtopaynomoreafter theabsenceofmoodeffectsisconsistentwithStudy1,in happythanangryprimes(36vs.28cents),B=.08,t(13)= whichprimingalsodidnotinfluenceratingsofsubjec- .67,p<.53.Hungerdidnotinfluencewillingnesstopay, tive experience. suggestingmotivationalspecificity(ts <1).Insummary, Mediansplitonthirst.Inthejustpresentedanalyses,we thirsty participants who rated the drink immediately usedlinearregressionstoestimatetheprimingeffectat after priming were willing to pay triple the price after threelevelsofthirst.Wefoundthatatahighthirstlevel, happy primes than after angry primes, but nonthirsty priming reliably influenced selected beverage ratings participants were not. withoutinfluencingmood.Similarresultsholdwhenthe dataareanalyzedbydividingparticipantsintotwoinde- Wantingformorebeverage.Participantsratedhowmuch pendentgroupsusingamediansplitonthirst.Toillus- beveragetheywantedtodrinkona0to5scale(withval- trate,Figure6plotstheprimingeffectonbeveragerat- ueslabeledasnone,1-2sips,half-cup,1cup,1pint,and2 ingsandmoodforparticipantswhoscoredatorabove pints).Participantsratedtheirpostpriminglevelofwant- the thirst median. As shown, for those thirsty partici- ingmoreofthebeverageat1.27,orslightlymorethan1 pants, priming influenced beverage evaluation (left to2sips.ThebottompanelofFigure5plotstheeffectsof panel), but not subjective experience (right panel).
Description: