Topics in Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology With Key References Daniel B. Hier, M.D. Chairman, Department of Neurology, Michael Reese Hospital and Medical Center, and Associate Professor of Neurology, University of Chicago Pritzker School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois Philip B. Gorelick, M.D. Director, Stroke Service, Michael Reese Hospital and Medical Center, and Assistant Professor of Neurology, University of Chicago Pritzker School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois Andrea Gellin Shindler, M.A., CCC-SP Speech Pathologist and Research Associate, Department of Neurology, Michael Reese Hospital and Medical Center, Chicago, Illinois Butterworths Boston London Durban Singapore Sydney Toronto Wellington Copyright © 1987 by Butterworth Publishers. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or other- wise, without the prior written permission of the publisher. Every effort has been made to ensure that the drug dosage schedules within this text are accurate and conform to standards accepted at time of publication. However, as treatment recommendations vary in the light of continuing research and clinical experience, the reader is advised to verify drug dosage schedules herein with information found on product information sheets. This is especially true in cases of new or infrequently used drugs. Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Hier, Daniel B. Topics in behavioral neurology and neuropsychology. Includes bibliographies and index. 1. Clinical neuropsychology. 2. Cerebral cortex— Diseases. I. Gorelick, Philip B. II. Shindler, Andrea Gellin. III. Title. [DNLM: 1. Nervous System Diseases. 2. Neuropsychology. WL 100 H633t] RC386.2.H54 1987 616.8 87-11719 ISBN 0-409-95165-X Butterworth Publishers 80 Montvale Avenue Stoneham, MA 02180 10 9 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 21 Printed in the United States of America This book is dedicated to our spouses and children: Myra and Benjamin Bonnie, David, and Alissa Michael, Adam, and Carey Preface Several comprehensive texts on neuropsychology are now available, including Walsh's Neuropsychology (1978), Heilman and Valenstein's Clinical Neuro- psychology (1985), and Hécaen and Albert's Human Neuropsychology (1978). Mesulam's Principles of Behavioral Neurology (1985) and Kirshner's Behavioral Neurology (1986) are excellent introductions to the new discipline of behavioral neurology. These books should serve as a "core" library for anyone interested in neuropsychology and behavioral neurology. Our book is not intended to replace these standard texts. Rather, it is meant to supplement them by providing a ready source of concise definitions and brief discussions. In caring for patients with disorders of higher cortical function and in teaching house staff and fellows about these disturbances, we were struck by the need for a book that would permit rapid entry into the burgeoning literature devoted to neuropsychology and behavioral neurology. The book was written by developing a list of topics that we felt were central to neuropsychology and behavioral neurology. We have sought to provide concise discussions and abundant references for each topic, including a sampling of both current and "classic" references. The book is divided into broad chapters covering such subjects as De- mentia, Aphasia, Apraxia, Amnesia, Recovery, and Stroke and Other Brain Disorders. Some conditions defy easy classification (e.g., allesthesia, synkine- sia); these are arranged alphabetically in the chapter entitled Miscellaneous Disorders. Other disorders could appropriately have been assigned to more than one chapter (e.g., constructional apraxia could be classified as an apraxia, an agnosia, or a visual disorder; developmental dyslexia could be considered an alexia or a childhood disorder). In cases where more than one classification was possible, we have chosen what we felt was most appropriate. If in doubt, the reader is directed to the index to find a topic of interest. We hope this book will prove useful to neurology residents, psychiatrists, neurologists, neuropsychologists, speech pathologists, rehabilitation medicine specialists, neuroscientists, and all others who share an interest in disorders of higher cortical function. We are all indebted to our teachers, especially Drs. J.P. Mohr, Louis R. Caplan, and Norman Geschwind, for introducing us to the field of behavioral neurology. We thank Miss Merryl Billingsley for editorial assistance and manuscript preparation. XI Note to Readers Within each section, terms may appear in either bold type or italics. Bold type indicates that the referenced term is discussed elsewhere in the book; the reader may consult the table of contents or the index. Italics indicate that the term is not discussed further in other sections. xni 1 Cerebral Organization CLASSIFICATION OF DISORDERS OF HIGHER CORTICAL FUNCTION A large number of disorders of higher cortical function are now recognized, but no completely satisfactory schema exists for categorizing them. Tradition- ally, disorders of higher cortical function have been grouped according to major behavioral disturbance: Aphasias: disorders of language Apraxias: disorders of skilled movement Agnosias: nonperceptual disorders of recognition Alexias: disorders of reading Agraphias: disorders of writing Acalculias: disorders of calculating Dementias: global disorders of intellect Amnesias: disorders of memory Unfortunately this schema is not complete, and many disorders are difficult to classify (e.g., right-left confusion, confabulation). Disorders may also be grouped by the main modality affected (e.g., visual, auditory, or somesthetic), by whether they are motor (executive) or sensory (receptive), and by whether they are developmental or acquired. Unfortunately, none of these schémas permits easy classification of all disorders. Classification of disorders by major site of involvement (e.g., frontal, parietal) is useful (see below), but for many disorders (e.g., amusia, anosognosia) the exact site of the injury responsible for the disorder is uncertain. Within both hemispheres, certain general patterns of intrahemispheric organization exist. Brain mass anterior to the central sulcus (rolandic sulcus) subserves motor (executive) functions, whereas brain mass posterior to the central sulcus subserves sensory (receptive) functions. The frontal lobe is in- volved not only in the execution of motor acts but in generating much of the motivation that drives these acts. A variety of higher cortical deficits may follow frontal lobe injury (Table 1.1). 1 2 Topics in Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology Table 1.1 Frontal Lobe Disorders Left Right Bilateral Broca's aphasia Impersistence Abulia Oral apraxia Neglect Perseveration Dysprosody Extinction Confabulation Transcortical motor Reduplication Moria aphasia Rigidity Callosal apraxia Depression (?) Parietal cortex subserves tactile sensation (somatesthesis). The inferior parietal lobule (angular gyrus region) serves to integrate sensory information (thereby permitting visual, auditory, and tactile cross-modal associations). The left temporal-parietal-occipital junction, unlike the homologous area in the right hemisphere, must also integrate linguistic with sensory information. Hence, damage to this critical junction on the left produces a unique set of deficits that are both quasi-linguistic and quasi-perceptual (right-left confu- sion, alexia, agraphia, finger agnosia, and dyscalculia). Spatial and quasi-spa- tial mapping of sensory input occurs in both the left and right parietal lobes; however, the right parietal lobe appears more efficient for many of these operations. Deficits associated with parietal lobe injury are summarized in Table 1.2. The temporal lobes have prominent olfactory and auditory functions and, because of their proximity to the underlying limbic system, have impor- tant modulating effects on emotion. The dominant left temporal lobe is the site of much of the primary language cortex within the brain. Deficits associated with temporal-lobe injury are shown in Table 1.3, and visual disturbances that follow injury to the occipital lobes are listed in Table 1.4. Table 1.2 Parietal Lobe Disorders Left Right Bilateral Constructional apraxia Constructional apraxia Ideational apraxia (?) Dyscalculia Dressing apraxia Agraphia Aprosody (?) Alexia Neglect Semantic aphasia Extinction Anomic aphasia Topographical disorientation Gerstmann's syndrome Anosognosia Conduction aphasia Affective agnosia (?) Ideomotor apraxia Left astereognosis Right astereognosis Cerebral Organization 3 Table 1.3 Temporal Lobe Disorders Left Right Bilateral Amnesia (verbal) Amnesia (visual) Cortical deafness Amusia (?) Amusia (?) Auditory agnosia Jargonaphasia Agitation (?) Klüver-Bucy syndrome Wernicke's aphasia Confusion (?) Global amnesia Anomic aphasia Synesthesia (?) Catastrophic reaction Synesthesia (?) Agitation (?) The role of the insula remains largely a mystery. Conduction aphasia may follow injury to the left insula. Deep structures (especially the thalamus) subserve alerting functions for both hemispheres and act as a conduit for cortex-directed signals arising from the brain stem reticular-activating system. The thalamus also plays an important role in memory functions. The limbic system, which lies between the deep basal ganglionic nuclei and the surface cerebral cortex, is involved in memory, emotion, and possibly motivation. The pattern of déficits depends on the laterality of the brain injury. Memory deficits depend on the side of damage; left temporal lesions produce greater verbal memory deficits, and right temporal lesions produce greater nonverbal (visual- spatial) memory deficits. Similarly, emotional reactions also depend on the hemisphere damaged. Left-hemisphere damage is more likely to elicit depres- sion or catastrophic reactions, while right-hemisphere damage tends to pro- duce anosognosia (indifference reactions) and affective agnosia. Mesulam (1981) has suggested that the two cerebral hemispheres may use different but complementary organizational schemes. Higher cortical func- tions appear to be localized to specific brain areas in the left hemisphere (e.g., Broca's, Wernicke's). In contrast, the right hemisphere appears to be organized by a series of overlapping networks (e.g., one for attention, one for motiva- tion). The size of the various cortical areas devoted to higher cortical functions probably varies among individuals. For example, important differences may exist in the size of language zone. The work of Ojemann (1979) suggests Table 1.4 Occipital Lobe Disorders Left Right Bilateral Simultanagnosia Palinopsia Cortical blindness Alexia Metamorphopsia Visual agnosia Color anomia Left achromatopsia Prosopagnosia Optic aphasia Anton's syndrome Right achromatopsia Balint's syndrome 4 Topics in Behavioral Neurology and Neuropsychology considerable variability among individuals in intrahemispheric organization of higher cortical functions. Sex, handedness, and various factors (e.g., genetic, experiential) may influence the precise site and size of brain areas subserving higher cortical functions. References Brown JW. Aphasia, apraxia and agnosia. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas, 1972. Critchley M. The parietal lobes. New York: Hafner Press, 1953. Hécaen H, Albert ML. Human neuropsychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1978. Heilman KM, Valenstein E. Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press, 1985. Kertesz A. Localization in neuropsychology. New York: Academic Press, 1983. Marie P. Existe-t-il dans le cerveau humain des centres innés ou préformes de langage? La Press Médicale 1922;17:117-81. McGlone J, Davidson W. The relationship between cerebral speech laterality and spa- tial ability with special reference to sex and hand preference. Neuropsychologia 1973a;ll:105-13. McGlone J, Kertesz A. Sex differences in cerebral processing of visuospatial tasks. Cortex 1973b;9:313-20. Mesulam M-M. A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol 1981;10:309-25. Mesulam M-M. Principles of behavioral neurology. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis, 1985. Nielsen JM. Agnosia, apraxia, aphasia. New York: Hafner Press, 1965 (Reprint of 1946 edition). Ojemann GA. Individual variability in cortical localization of language. J Neurosurgery 1979;50:164-9. Semmes J. Hemispheric specialization: a possible clue to mechanism. Neuropsycholo- gia 1968;6:11-26. Whitaker HA, Ojemann GA. Graded localisation of naming from electrical stimulation mapping of left cerebral cortex. Nature 1977;270:50-1. CEREBRAL DOMINANCE The term cerebral dominance reflects the observation that language functions are usually lateralized to the left hemisphere and visual-spatial functions are usually lateralized to the right hemisphere. Although this propensity for lan- guage to lateralize to the left hemisphere is well documented, its biological basis is unknown. There is convincing evidence that left-handers are much less well lateralized than right-handers. Although right-hemisphere dominance for speech is rare in true right-handers, bilateral-hemisphere or right-hemisphere speech representation is common in left-handers. Aphasia after right-hemi- sphere injury in right-handers is rare (crossed aphasia). Transient aphasia with good recovery is common in left-handers, regardless of the hemisphere injured. As of 1976, Brown and Hécaen (1976) found only nine convincing cases of Cerebral Organization 5 crossed aphasia in dextrals in the world literature. On the other hand, Zangwill (1960) found that of 54 left-handers with left-hemisphere lesions, 24 (44%) had severe aphasia and 21 (39%) had transient aphasia. Of 39 left- handers with right-hemisphere lesions, 13 (33%) had severe aphasia and 17 (44%) had transient aphasia. Hemispheric dominance for language may be assessed by a variety of techniques, including the Wada test (intracarotid amobarbital [Amytal] injec- tion), dichotic listening tests, and the effects of unilateral electroshock therapy. Branch et al. (1964) examined dominance for language using the Wada test in 119 patients with intractable epilepsy. Of 48 right-handers, 43 (90%) were left-hemisphere dominant for speech and 5 (10%) were right-hemisphere dom- inant. The unusually high incidence of right-hemisphere speech dominance reflects the presence of brain injury in a high proportion of the epileptics. Among 71 left-handers, Branch et al. (1964) found 34 (48%) with left-hemi- sphere speech dominance, reflecting the presence of brain injury in a high proportion of the epileptics. Among the left-handers, evidence of prior left hemisphere injury dropped the frequency of left-hemisphere dominance for speech from 64% to 22%. Thus, early-life left-hemisphere injury strongly influences speech dominance to shift from the left to right hemisphere. How- ever, this injury must occur before six years of age and involve the central language zone for language dominance to reliably shift from left to right. Dichotic listening studies suggest that speech is less firmly lateralized to the left hemisphere in left-handers than in right-handers. Some evidence sug- gests that women are less lateralized than men. Based on available evidence, Roberts (1969) concludes that the left hemisphere is dominant for language in at least 95% of right-handers and 66% of left-handers. The right hemisphere is dominant for language in about 30% of left-handers. Bilateral speech repre- sentation probably occurs in a small percentage of left-handers and rarely in right-handers. Attempts have been made to relate functional asymmetries (i.e., lateralization of language) to morphological (anatomical) asymmetries of the brain, but such hypotheses remain controversial. References Annett M. Hand preference and the laterality of cerebral speech. Cortex 1975;11: 305-28. Benton AL. Historical notes on hemispheric dominance. Arch Neurol 1977;34:127-9. Branch D, Milner B, Rasmussen T. Intracarotid sodium Amytal for the lateralization of cerebral dominance for speech. J Neurosurg 1964;21:399-405. Briggs CG, Nebes RD. The effects of handedness, family history and sex on the perfor- mance of a dichotic listening task. Neuropsychologia 1976;14:129-33. Brown JW, Hécaen H. Lateralization and language representation. Neurology 1976;26:183-9. Bryden MP. Tachistoscopic recognition, handedness and cerebral dominance. Neuro- psychologia 1965;3:1-8. Dennis M, Whitaker HA. Language acquisition following hemidecortication: linguistic superiority of the left over the right hemisphere. Brain Lang 1976;3:404-33.