ebook img

Think-Aloud, Thought-Listing, and Video-Mediated Recall Procedures in the Assessment of ... PDF

20 Pages·2014·0.11 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Think-Aloud, Thought-Listing, and Video-Mediated Recall Procedures in the Assessment of ...

CognitiveTherapyandResearch,Vol.24,No.4,2000,pp.399–418 Think-Aloud, Thought-Listing, and Video-Mediated Recall Procedures in the Assessment of Children’s Self-Talk Jackie Lodge,1 Gail Tripp,1,2 and Diana Kim Harte1 Two studies examined the extent to which different cognitive assessment procedures yieldsimilardatainpre-adolescentchildren.InStudy1,situationallyanxiouschildren reported their self-talk via think-aloud and verbal thought-listing procedures (N ! 88).Halfofthechildrenreportedtheirself-talkusingboththink-aloudandthought- listing while the remainder engaged in thought-listing only. Prior participation in think-aloud did not influence the self-talk subsequently reported by children during verbalthought-listing.Comparedwiththought-listing,moreproblem-solving(analyti- cal) self-talk and less valenced self-talk was reported during think-aloud. In Study 2, 41 children reported their self-talk via both verbal thought-listing and video- mediated recall (own and other’s perspective) procedures. Video-mediated recall generated self-talk of a similar valence to that obtained by thought-listing. More self-talk was generated when the children viewed their own perspective videotape comparedwithobserverperspectivevideotapeandthought-listing.Whilepre-adoles- cent children were able to respond to all three cognitive assessment methods, the self-talk produced was not identical. In choosing a cognitive assessment method, researchers and clinicians should be guided by the purpose of the assessment and the setting in which it occurs. KEYWORDS:children;self-talk;think-aloud;thought-listing;video-mediatedrecall. Overthelast20years,interestintheroleofcognitionsinadultpsychopathology has increased dramatically. This has resulted in the development of a number of procedures for assessing cognitions. These procedures differ on four dimensions: structure,endorsementversusfreereport;timing,concurrentversusretrospective; responsemode,writtenversusverbal;andthenatureofthestimulus,invivositua- tion, viewed on videotape, or imagined (Glass, 1993). Glass and Merluzzi (1981) classify the available cognitive assessment procedures into six broad categories: 1UniversityofOtago,Dunedin,NewZealand. 2Correspondence should be sent to Dr. Gail Tripp, Department of Psychology, University of Otago, P.O.Box56,Dunedin,NewZealand.E-mail:[email protected] 399 0147-5916/00/0800-0399$18.00/02000PlenumPublishingCorporation 400 Lodge,Tripp,andHarte concurrentprocedures,recallprocedures,promptedrecallprocedures,recognition/ endorsementapproaches,projectivemethods,andnaturalisticapproaches.Recent interest in the importance of cognitive factors in child psychopathology has lead tothese proceduresbeing used,with fewmodifications,to assessthe cognitionsof children. It is unclear, however, which of these techniques is most suitable for use withchildren,andwhetherchildrenreportdifferentpatternsofself-talkinresponse to the different procedures. The present paper describes two studies that examined the extent to which different cognitive assessment procedures yield comparable self-talk data in pre- adolescentchildren.Study1comparedtheself-talkgeneratedbyconcurrent(think- aloud) and recall (verbal thought-listing) procedures in a sample of 9- to 10-year- oldschoolchildrenunderconditionsofmildanxiety.Thestudyalsobrieflyexamined therelationshipbetweenself-talktypeandanxiety.Study2comparedtheself-talk generated by recall (verbal thought-listing) and prompted recall (video-mediated recall) procedures in a sample of 9- to 11-year-old school children. Think-aloud and thought-listing techniques, including video-mediated recall, wereselectedforstudybecause(1)theycomecloserthanothercognitiveassessment methodstomeetingMeichenbaumandCameron’s(1981)guidelinesforthecollec- tionofvalidcognitiveself-reportdata,(2)lessisknownaboutthevalidityofthese production methods with children than endorsement procedures, and (3) in the caseofthink-aloudandthought-listing,bothtechniqueshavebeencomparedwith endorsement procedures, but to date have not been compared with one another (Houston,Fox,&Forbes,1984;Kendall&Chansky,1991;Prins&Hanewald,1997). STUDY 1 Think-aloudisaconcurrenttechniqueinwhichanindividualisaskedtoverbal- ize his or her self-talk aloud while simultaneously performing a pre-determined task(Arnkoff&Glass,1989;Genest&Turk,1981;Martzke,Andersen,&Cacioppo, 1987).Thought-listingisarecalltechniquethatrequiresanindividualtoretrospec- tivelyprovide(Kendall&Chansky,1991;Prins,1985,1986)verbalor,moreusually, writtenreportsoftheself-talkexperiencedduringaspecifictimeperiod(Blackwell, Galassi,Galassi,&Watson,1985;Halford&Sanders,1988;Sanders&Dadds,1992). Think-aloudhastheadvantageofreducingpotentialmemoryand/orreportingbias because the time between experiencing and reporting a self-statement is minimal (Blackwell et al., 1985). Furthermore, it typically yields a large sample of self-talk (Merluzzi & Boltwood, 1989). Thought-listing, on the other hand, is less likely to interfere with task performance (Blackwell et al., 1985). We are aware of only one published study that has directly compared think- aloud and thought-listing procedures and this was with adults (Blackwell et al., 1985).Think-aloudgeneratedapproximatelytwiceasmanyself-statementsaswrit- tenthought-listing.Whenboththefrequencyandproportionofself-talktypewere considered, think-aloud was found to produce significantly more attention-control andproblem-solvingself-statementsthanthought-listing.Althoughthink-aloudand thought-listing procedures have not been directly compared with children, a small AssessmentofChildren’sSelf-Talk 401 number of studies have shown that children can respond to these procedures, producing codeable self-talk (Fox, Houston, & Pittner, 1983; Houston et al, 1984; Kendall & Chansky, 1991; Prins, 1985, 1986). In the present study, think-aloud and verbal thought-listing procedures were used to elicit the self-talk of pre-adolescent children placed under conditions of mild anxiety. Verbal thought-listing was used to eliminate the risk that writing limitations would reduce or distort the children’s responses. Data were collected under conditions of mild anxiety to facilitate examination of the relationship be- tweenanxietyandself-talktype.Thespecificaimsofthestudywereto(1)compare the self-talk generated by children using think-aloud and verbal thought-listing procedures, (2) explore whether prior engagement in think-aloud influences the self-talkchildrensubsequentlyreportduringthought-listing,and(3)investigatethe relationships between self-talk type and anxiety in children. Basedonpreviousresearchusingthink-aloud orverbalthought-listingproce- dureswithchildren,itwasassumedthatthemajorityofchildrenwouldreportself- talk (Fox et al., 1983; Houston et al., 1984; Kendall & Chansky, 1991; Prins, 1985, 1986).Previousresearchwithadults(Blackwelletal.,1985)andourownpilotwork withchildrensuggestedthatthechildrenwouldreportmoreself-talkduringthink- aloudthanverbalthought-listing.AlthoughBlackwelletal.(1985)foundthatadults reported more ‘‘problem-solving’’ (analytical) self-statements under think-aloud than thought-listing, this finding was not supported by our preliminary results (Lodge,Harte,&Tripp,1998).Nospecificpredictionsweremadeaboutthecompa- rability of self-talk types produced under think-aloud and thought-listing proce- dures. Because there is currently no research examining the effect of think-aloud on children’s subsequent verbal thought-listing, no hypotheses were formulated regarding this issue. Thelimitedresearchontherelationshipbetweenanxietyandself-talktypein childrensuggestedthatnegativeself-talkwouldbepositivelycorrelatedwithanxiety (Prins, 1985, 1986; Prins, Groot, & Hanewald, 1994; Zatz & Chassin, 1983, 1985). Method Participants The participants were 88 children (46 females and 42 males) aged between 8 years 8 months and 10 years 9 months (M ! 9 years 8 months). Children 8 years andolderwereincludedinthestudybecausereportsintheliterature,andourown research, suggest that children younger than this cannot reliably report their self- talk.Thechildrenwererecruitedfromfoursuburbanprimaryschools.Oneschool served a low socioeconomic area, two serviced mid socioeconomic areas, and the fourth school serviced a high socioeconomic area. Most (83%) of the participating children were white. Letters of invitation explaining the study were distributed to the parents of childreninclassesequivalenttothirdandfourthgradeatthefourschools.Parents who were willing to allow their child to participate in the study returned a signed consentformtotheresearchers.Ninety(45.5%)ofthe198consentformsdistributed 402 Lodge,Tripp,andHarte were returned. Participation rates for the four schools ranged from 38.8% (mid socioeconomic area) to 68.2% (high socioeconomic area). Parents who did not return consent forms were not followed up. Materials State-TraitAnxietyInventoryforChildren(STAIC). Thechildren’sstate-and trait-anxietywereassessedwiththeSTAIC(Spielberger,1973).TheSTAICcontains two subscales—a 20-item State scale that assesses the child’s anxiety at the time theinventoryiscompleted,anda20-itemTraitscaledesignedtomeasurethechild’s generalized feelings of anxiety. The STAIC was designed for use with fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade children. Reliability and validity data are available for this measure(Kendall&Ronan,1990;Spielberger,1973).TheSTAICwasadministered verballybecausepreviousresearchindicatesNewZealandchildrenmayhavediffi- culty with some of the vocabulary used in the scale (Tripp & Lodge, 1993). Math Problems.3 Five math problems, to be completed within 5 minutes, served as the mildly anxiety-provoking task for the participating children. These mathproblemsweretakenfromthebook‘‘ShortMaths2’’(Lockyer,1990).They were selected in consultation with an experienced primary school principal, who considered them to be age appropriate. Given sufficient time, most 8- to 10-year- old children should be able to complete the math problems. Procedure Childrenwithparentalpermissiontoparticipateinthestudywereapproached at school by J.L., who described the study and invited them to take part. All of the children approached consented to participate. The children were individually interviewedtwiceovera2-weekperiod(1weekbetweensessions).Interviewswere conducted in a quiet room at the children’s schools. Duringthefirstsession,thechildrenwereverballyadministeredthetraitscale of the STAIC. After this session, pairs of children were matched for age, sex, and STAIC-trait anxiety percentile score. Children within the matched pair were then randomly assigned to one of two groups: think-aloud followed by verbal thought- listing (Group 1) or verbal thought-listing only (Group 2). Both groups included 23girlsand21boys.Thegroupsdidnotdiffersignificantlyontraitanxietypercentile scores (Group 1, M ! 39, SD ! 26; Group 2, M ! 39, SD ! 26) or age (Group 1, M ! 115.8 months, SD ! 6.9; Group 2, M ! 116.7 months, SD ! 5.9). At the beginning of the second session, the children were informed that they would be given 5 minutes in which to complete a series of math problems. They were told that the researcher was interested in how many of the problems they could get right within this time and that, although they might not get all of the problemsfinished,theyshouldtrytheirbesttoworkthemout.Afterashortpractice trial,thechildreninGroup1weregiven2minutestoreportaloudeverythingthat theywerethinkingandsayingtothemselves(think-aloud).Childrenwhoremained silentfor10secondsweregivenamaximumoftwostandardprompts:‘‘Remember 3Copiesofthefivemathproblemsareavailablefromthesecondauthor. AssessmentofChildren’sSelf-Talk 403 tosayoutloudallthethingsthatcomeintoyourhead’’and‘‘Whatareyouthinking and saying to yourself right now?’’ Children in both groups were then verbally administered the STAIC-state scale. Followingthis,allchildrenweregiventhemathproblemstoworkon.Children in Group 1 were asked to say out loud everything they were thinking and saying tothemselveswhiletheyworkedontheproblems(think-aloud).Asbefore,children who remained silent for 10 seconds were prompted (maximum of two standard prompts). The researcher also responded briefly to occasional questions raised by somechildreninbothgroups.Nootherverbalcontactwasmadewiththechildren while they worked on the math problems. The researcher was positioned at the back ofthe room, withinhearing distance butoutside the children’sline of vision, during this 5-minute period. Self-talk generated prior to and during the maths problems was combined for analysis. After5minutes,thechildrenwereaskedtostopworkingontheproblemsand were interviewed about what they were thinking prior to and while participating in the task (verbal thought-listing). The children were requested to: 1. ‘‘Tell me everything you thinking and saying to yourself when I first told you about the math problems?’’ 2. ‘‘Tell me everything you thinking and saying to yourself while you were working on the math problems?’’ Standardpromptsweregivenifchildrenfailedtorespondwithin10seconds(‘‘Were youthinkingorsayinganythingtoyourself?’’Ifanaffirmativeresponsewasgiven thechildwasasked‘‘whatwereyouthinkingorsayingtoyourself?’’)orresponded that they were thinking about the situation rather than reporting their actual self- talk (‘‘What were the thoughts that went through your mind?’’). The children were asked to indicate when they had finished responding to each thought-listing ‘‘question’’ by raising a hand. The children’s self-talk during think aloud and/or thought-listing was audiotaped and later transcribed verbatim. Results Parametric and nonparametric tests were used to analyze the data. Nonpara- metricprocedureswereusedwheredatawerecategoricalinnatureornon-normally distributed.Unlessotherwisestated,criticalalphawassetat.05andanalyseswere two-tailed. Coding of Dependent Variables The transcripts of the children’s self-talk were unitized by two trained raters working independently. A unit was defined as representing one main idea, rather than meaningless words. Sentence structure and content were used to help define units. Copies of the unitizing manual are available from the second author. One rater unitized all of the transcripts, while the second rater unitized self-statements fromrandomlyselectedpageswithinthetranscripts(45%ofthetranscribedmate- rial).Throughoutunitizing,theratershadaccesstotheaudiotapesofthechildren’s self-talk. 404 Lodge,Tripp,andHarte Agreementovertheexistenceofa‘‘unit’’wasdefinedasthetworatersmarking ‘‘unit boundaries’’ in exactly the same location on the transcripts. The two raters agreedon97.1%ofthecommonlyunitisedmaterial(372ofthe383unitsidentified). Therewere11omissiondisagreements(i.e.,‘‘unitboundaries’’thatonlyoneofthe tworatersidentified).Disagreementswereresolvedinfavoroftheraterwhounit- ized all of the transcripts. Following unitizing, the units of self-talk were coded into six theoretically meaningful categories: positive, negative, neutral, analytical, directive (sometimes referred to as coping), and questioning self-talk using a system developed by J.L. Questions are not usually identified separately but coded according to valence. A categoryof questioningself-talk wasincluded inlight ofthe frequencywith which questions appear in the self-talk of anxious adults (Kendall & Ingram, 1987). De- scriptions of all of the self-talk categories, together with examples, are given in Table I. TableI.CognitiveContentCategories:DefinitionsandExamples Cognitive category Definitions Examples Positivea Self-statement(s)likelytofacilitate ‘‘Icandoit.’’ controloffear/anxietyand/orachiev- ‘‘Ienjoymath.’’ ingafavorableoutcomeon(orcom- ‘‘Thisisachallenge.’’ pletionof)thetask.Excludesques- tions(codedasQuestioning)and self-instructions(codedasDi- rective). Negativea Self-statementslikelytohindercontrol ‘‘Thislookshard.’’ offearand/orhinderafavorable ‘‘Imustbestupid.’’ outcomeon(orcompletionof)the ‘‘I’mgettingthemallwrong.’’ task.Excludesquestions(codedas Questioning). Neutrala Self-statementsthathavenoclearin- ‘‘I’mthinkingaboutmybestfriend.’’ fluenceonthetaskathandandcan- ‘‘Therearefivequestions.’’ notbeclearlycategorizedelse- ‘‘Thisislikebeingintheclassroom.’’ where. Analytical On-taskverbalizationsinwhichthe ‘‘Theansweris10.’’ subjectisengaginginproblem-solv- ‘‘ABCDE.’’ ingstrategiestocompletethetask. ‘‘I’llworkthisoutbyaddingupthe numbers.’’ Directivea,b Self-statementsdirectingthoughtorbe- ‘‘Workatmyownpace.’’ havior,expressedintheformofa ‘‘Staycalm.’’ self-instruction.Unlikelytointerfere ‘‘Imustkeepconcentrating.’’ withachievingafavorableoutcome onthetask,taskcompletion,orcon- trollingfear/anxiety. Questioning Self-statementsexpressedintheform ‘‘WhatifIcan’tfinishtheseintime?’’ ofaquestion.Excludesreadingques- ‘‘WillIgetitdone?’’ tionsaloudfromthemathtaskor ‘‘WonderhowI’mdoing?’’ askingquestionsinordertosolvea mathproblem(e.g.,‘‘Whatcomes after10?’’)(bothcodedasAna- lytical). AdaptedfromLodgeetal.,1998. aDefinitionsbasedonKendall&Chansky(1991). b‘‘Directive’’self-talkismorecommonlyreferredtoas‘‘coping’’self-talkintheliterature. AssessmentofChildren’sSelf-Talk 405 A further two raters, blind to the experimental hypotheses, independently codedtheunitsofself-talk.Eachraterreceivedapproximately10hoursoftraining intheuseofthecodingsystemandbegancodingtheunitisedtranscriptsoncethey reachedmorethan95%agreementwithJ.L.Oneratercategorizedallunitsonthe transcripts,whiletheothercategorised388(47.5%)ofthe817unitsfromrandomly selectedpageswithinthetranscripts.Approximately7%oftheunitswerenotcoded (e.g., acknowledgements of prompts, descriptions of behavior, questions directed at the interviewer). Inter-rateragreementwascalculatedusingCohen’sKappa(Bakeman&Gott- man,1986;Cohen,1960).Kappasfortheindividualself-talkcategoriesrangedfrom 0.87to1.00,withanoverallkappaof0.96.Disagreementsincodingwereresolved in favor of the rater who coded all of the transcribed units. Anxiety Levels The participants’ raw scores on the state- and trait-scales of the STAIC were converted to percentile scores. Paired t tests were used to compare state and trait percentilescoreswithineachgroup.Childreninbothgroupsreportedexperiencing significantlymoreanxietyjustpriortocompletingthemathproblemsthaningeneral (Group 1: State M ! 53, Trait M ! 39, t[43] ! 2.89, p " .01; Group 2: State M ! 55, Trait M ! 39, t[43] ! 4.09, p " .001). Nature of Self-Talk Reported During Think-Aloud and Thought-Listing Allofthechildrenreportedself-talkduringthink-aloud(Group1).Onechild inGroup1andsixchildreninGroup2reportednoself-talkduringthought-listing. Chi-square analysis showed that the number of children failing to report self-talk duringthought-listingdidnotdiffersignificantlyacrossthegroups.TableIIpresents therange,mean,andmodalnumberofself-statementsreportedbyeachgroupfor each cognitive assessment method. The median amount of self-talk generated during think-aloud and thought- listing procedures by the children in Group 1 was compared with a Wilcoxon matched-pairs signed-ranks test. Significantly more self-talk was reported during think-aloud (M ! 9.5, SD ! 6.8) than during thought-listing (M ! 4.6, SD ! 2.7) Z ! #4.91, p " .0001.4 To compare the type of self-talk generated the responses in each self-talk category were converted to proportions of the total amount pro- duced.Thiswasdonetocontrolfordifferencesintheamountofself-talkgenerated acrossmethods.Onlydatafromchildrenwhoactuallyreportedself-talkwerecon- vertedtoproportions.Itisnotmeaningfultotalkabouttheproportionsofself-talk typesforchildrenwhoreportednoself-talk.Theseproportiondataarepresentedin Table III. The proportion data show a similar pattern to the frequency data. For think- aloud, analytical self-talk was most commonly reported, followed by negative and questioning self-talk. For thought-listing, negative and positive self-talk were the 4Thisanalysiswasrepeatedremovingthedatafromthechildwhoreportednoself-talkduringthought- listing: think aloud M ! 9.6, SD ! 6.8; thought-listing M ! 4.7, SD ! 2.7; Z ! #4.84, p " .0001, N!43. 406 Lodge,Tripp,andHarte TableII.FrequencyofSelf-StatementsAssignedtoEachCognitiveCategoryAccordingtoGroupand CognitiveAssessmentMethod No.(%) Numberofself-statementsreported reporting Cognition cognition category Mean (S.D.) Median Range type Group1(TA) N!44 N!44 N!44 N!44 Negative 1.8(2.1) 1.0 0–8 29(65.9%) Positive 1.5(1.9) 1.0 0–7 25(56.8%) Analytical 3.4(3.9) 2.5 0–15 30(68.2%) Questioning 1.6(1.8) 1.0 0–7 27(61.4%) Neutral 1.0(1.6) 0.0 0–6 18(40.9%) Directive 0.2(0.5) 0.0 0–2 8(18.2%) Total 9.5(6.8) 8.0 2–31 Group1(TL)a N!44 N!43 N!43 N!43 N!43 Negative 1.4(1.4) 1.4(1.4) 1.0 0–6 30(69.8%) Positive 1.2(1.3) 1.3(1.3) 1.0 0–6 32(74.4%) Analytical 0.7(1.1) 0.7(1.1) 0.0 0–4 17(39.5%) Questioning 0.8(1.3) 0.8(1.3) 0.0 0–6 15(34.9%) Neutral 0.4(0.8) 0.4(0.8) 0.0 0–3 12(27.9%) Directive 0.1(0.2) 0.0(0.2) 0.0 0–2 2(4.7%) Total 4.6(2.7) 4.7(2.7) 4.0 1–12 Group2(TL)a N!44 N!38 N!38 N!38 N!38 Negative 0.9(1.2) 1.0(1.2) 1.0 0–5 22(57.9%) Positive 0.8(1.2) 0.9(1.3) 1.0 0–5 20(52.6%) Analytical 0.7(0.9) 0.8(0.9) 1.0 0–3 21(55.3%) Questioning 0.5(1.0) 0.6(1.0) 0.0 0–3 10(26.3%) Neutral 0.2(0.5) 0.2(0.5) 0.0 0–2 6(15.8%) Directive 0.1(0.3) 0.1(0.3) 0.0 0–1 3(7.9%) Total 3.2(2.2) 3.7(1.9) 3.5 1–8 TA!think-aloud;TL!verbalthought-listing. aTLmeansandstandarddeviationsusingtheentiresampleandonlythosechildrenreportingself-talk arepresented. mostprevalent,followedbyhighratesofanalyticalself-talkforchildreninGroup 2. Because the majority of the self-talk was assigned to the negative, positive, analytical,andquestioningcategories,onlythesefourtypesareconsideredfurther. Theproportionofeachself-talktype(negative,positive,analytical,andques- tioning) reported during think-aloud and thought-listing was compared with Wil- coxon matched-pairs signed-ranks tests. Proportions of negative (Z ! #2.59, p " .01),positive(Z!#3.08,p".005),andanalytical(Z!#3.63,p".0005)self-talk, but not questioning self-talk, differed significantly across the cognitive assessment methods. Specifically, higher proportions of negative and positive self-talk were reportedduringthought-listing,whereasahigherproportionofanalyticalself-talk was reported during think-aloud. Considerationofthefrequencydatasuggestthatthedifferencesinthepropor- tionsofpositiveandnegativeself-talkobservedbetweenthink-aloudandthought- listing are an artifact of the very high rate of analytical self-talk reported during think-aloud.The think-aloud/thought-listinganalyseswererepeated usingthefre- quencydata(N!43).Wilcoxonmatched-pairssigned-ranktestsindicatedthatthe number of analytical (Z ! #4.37, p " .0001) and questioning (Z ! #2.68, p " AssessmentofChildren’sSelf-Talk 407 Table III. Proportion of Self-Statements Assigned to Each CognitiveCategoryAccordingtoGroupandCognitiveAssess- mentMethod Proportionofself-statementsreported Cognition category Meana(S.D.) Median Range Group1(TA) Negative 0.22(0.26) 0.13 0.00–1.00 Positive 0.15(0.19) 0.09 0.00–0.88 Analytical 0.33(0.30) 0.35 0.00–1.00 Questioning 0.21(0.28) 0.13 0.00–1.00 Neutral 0.08(0.12) 0.00 0.00–0.50 Directive 0.01(0.03) 0.00 0.00–0.13 Group1(TL) Negative 0.34(0.31) 0.29 0.00–1.00 Positive 0.27(0.24) 0.25 0.00–1.00 Analytical 0.13(0.19) 0.00 0.00–0.67 Questioning 0.17(0.27) 0.00 0.00–1.00 Neutral 0.08(0.14) 0.00 0.00–0.50 Directive 0.01(0.05) 0.00 0.00–0.33 Group2(TL) Negative 0.27(0.30) 0.23 0.00–1.00 Positive 0.24(0.30) 0.17 0.00–1.00 Analytical 0.24(0.28) 0.21 0.00–1.00 Questioning 0.15(0.28) 0.00 0.00–1.00 Neutral 0.07(0.19) 0.00 0.00–1.00 Directive 0.03(0.30) 0.00 0.00–0.33 TA!think-aloud;TL!verbalthought-listing. aMeans are basedon the number ofchildren reporting self- talk(Group1/TA!44;Group1/TL!43;Group2/TL!38) .01)self-statementsreporteddifferedsignificantlyacrossmethods.Moreanalytical and questioning self-talk were reported during think-aloud than during thought- listing.Frequenciesofpositiveandnegativeself-talkdidnotdifferacrosscognitive assessment procedures. Effect of Think-Aloud on Subsequent Thought-Listing To determine whether prior participation in think-aloud influenced the self- talk reported during thought-listing, both the total amount of self-talk and the proportions of self-talk types (negative, positive, analytical, and questioning) re- portedduringthought-listingwerecomparedacrossthegroupsusingMann-Whitney U tests. Children who reported no self-talk were excluded from the analyses of self-talk type. Whenonlythosechildrenwhoreportedself-talkwereincludedintheanalysis, therewasnosignificantgroupdifferenceinthetotalamountofself-talkgenerated during thought-listing. However, when the analysis was repeated to include all of the children, the group who had engaged in think-aloud prior to thought-listing (M ! 4.5, SD ! 2.7) reported significantly more self-statements than those who engaged in thought-listing only (M ! 3.2, SD ! 2.2) Z ! #2.3, p " .05. No significant group differences were found for the proportions of self-talk typesreported,althoughthedifferenceinproportionofanalyticalself-talkreported 408 Lodge,Tripp,andHarte TableIV.SpearmanRankCorrelationsBetweenAnxiety(STAIC-StateandTraitPercentileScores) andSelf-TalkType Think-aloud(N!44) Thought-listing(N!81) STAIC-trait STAIC-state STAIC-trait STAIC-state Frequencyofself-talk Negativea r !#.16ns r ! .15ns r ! .14ns r ! .37** s s s s Positive r ! .11ns r !#.08ns r ! .04ns r !#.08ns s s s s Questioninga r ! .04ns r !#.18ns r ! .01ns r !#.13ns s s s s Analytical r !#.05ns r !#.21ns r ! .03ns r ! .09ns s s s s Proportionofself-talk Negativea r !#.15ns r ! .25* r ! .10ns r ! .35** s s s s Positive r ! .06ns r ! .01ns r !#.01ns r !#.13ns s s s s Questioninga r !#.03ns r !#.13ns r ! .02ns r !#.12ns s s s s Analytical r !#.05ns r !#.25ns r ! .00ns r ! .06ns s s s s *p".05. **p".0005 ns!notsignificant. aWherethedirectionofarelationshipwaspredicted,one-tailedtestswereused. approached significance (Z ! #1.95, p ! .052).5,6 Children in Group 2 reported a higher proportion of analytical self-talk than those in Group 1. Self-Talk and Anxiety Spearmanrankcorrelationswereusedtoassessthedegreeoflinearassociation betweenboththefrequencyandproportionofpositive,negative,questioning,and analyticalself-talkandthechildren’slevelsofanxiety(TableIV).Separatecorrela- tionswerecarriedoutforthink-aloudandthought-listing,astheself-talkgenerated bythesecognitiveassessmentproceduresdiffered.Becausetherewerenosignificant group differences in the self-talk types reported during thought-listing, these data werecollapsedacrossthegroups.Alphawassetat.01toprotectagainsttypeIerrors. For thought-listing, both the proportion (r ! .35, p " .0005, one-tailed) and s thefrequency(r !.37,p".0005,one-tailed)ofnegativeself-talkweresignificantly s positively correlated with state anxiety levels. For think-aloud, the correlation be- tween state anxiety and the proportion of negative self-talk (r ! .25, p ! .026, s one-tailed)approached,butdidnotreach,statisticalsignificance.Allothercorrela- tions were nonsignificant. Discussion Theresultsofthepresentstudyindicatethattheself-talkreportedbychildren is influenced by the cognitive assessment method used. Think-aloud and thought- listing procedures do not produce identical self-talk. Although prior participation 5Theseanalyseswererepeatedtoincludethechildrenwhoreportednoself-talkduringverbalthought- listing.Mann-WhitneyUtestsshowednosignificantgroupdifferencesintheproportionsofnegative, positive,analytical,andquestioningself-talkreported. 6Anyteststhatapproachedstatisticalsignificancewerecheckedusingrandomizationtests(Manly,1992). Resultswereconsistentwiththenon-parametricanalysesreported.

Description:
Arnkoff, D. B., & Glass, C. R. (1989). Cognitive assessment in social anxiety and social phobia. Clinical Psychology Review, 9, 61-74.Google Scholar.
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.