The Social Dog Behaviour and Cognition Edited by Juliane Kaminski Psychology Department, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, UK Sarah Marshall-Pescini Dipartimento di Fisiopatologia Medico-Chirurgica e dei Trapianti, Sezione di Neuroscienze, Università degli Studi di Milano, Milan, Italy Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria Wolf Science Centre, Ernstbrunn, Austria AMSTERDAM • BOSTON • HEIDELBERG • LONDON NEW YORK • OXFORD • PARIS • SAN DIEGO SAN FRANCISCO • SINGAPORE • SYDNEY • TOKYO Academic Press is an imprint of Elsevier Academic Press is an imprint of Elsevier 525 B Street, Suite 1800, San Diego, CA 92101-4495, USA 32 Jamestown Road, London NW1 7BY, UK 225 Wyman Street, Waltham, MA 02451, USA Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system or transmitted in any form or by any means electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or otherwise without the prior written permission of the publisher Permissions may be sought directly from Elsevier’s Science & Technology Rights Department in Oxford, UK: phone (+44) (0) 1865 843830; fax (+44) (0) 1865 853333; email: [email protected]. Alternatively you can submit your request online by visiting the Elsevier web site at http://elsevier.com/locate/ permissions, and selecting Obtaining permission to use Elsevier material Notice No responsibility is assumed by the publisher for any injury and/or damage to persons or property as a matter of products liability, negligence or otherwise, or from any use or operation of any methods, products, instructions or ideas contained in the material herein. Because of rapid advances in the medical sciences, in particular, independent verification of diagnoses and drug dosages should be made British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data A catalog record for this book is available from the Library of Congress ISBN: 978-0-12-407818-5 For information on all Academic Press publications visit our web site at store.elsevier.com Printed and bound in the US 14 15 16 17 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Preface The domestic dog seems to be the new star in behavioural research. Forgotten for some time, it has been rising again in the past two decades. Research on dog behaviour and cognition has exploded, with so many papers being published that it is hard to keep track. There are probably many reasons why research with dogs has increased so substantially, one being that several researchers have raised the hypothesis that dogs might have evolved specialized social skills as a result of living with humans and adapting to the human environment. This hypothesis has sparked the interest of both the scientific community and a wider audience, reflected in many of the contributions to this volume. The main purpose of this book is to summarize the growing body of research on dogs’ social behaviour and cognition, as well as to highlight some of the con- troversial issues and open questions still needing answers. The intention is to make this exciting new field more accessible to scholars from both similar and different fields, to students, and to dog specialists whose practical work with dogs is informed by the research highlighted throughout the book. Our empha- sis, reflected in the organization of this volume, has been to draw the reader’s attention both to studies focusing on dog-to-dog social interactions as well as those directed at a deeper understanding of the dog-to-human relationship, since we think both are necessary to gain a more complete understanding of dogs. This project was endorsed enthusiastically by both the publishers and the many scientists who have contributed to the book. Indeed, we combine 13 original contributions, from 27 authors and co-authors, from many disci- plines including biology, veterinary science, and psychology. The diversity of the authors’ scientific backgrounds allows the reader to gain ‘a feel’ for how dogs are being studied, from many different perspectives. Each contribution is a review of a particular topic pertaining to dogs’ social behaviour and/or cogni- tion and is written to stand alone. However, each chapter is also a ‘slice of the cake’, and as a whole the result is an interdisciplinary book that provides an overview of progress in understanding many aspects of dogs’ social behaviour and cognition. Juliane Kaminski Sarah Marshall-Pescini xi Contributors Roberto Bonanni Department of Neuroscience, University of Parma, Italy Juliane Bräuer Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany Oliver Burman University of Lincoln, Riseholme Park, Lincoln, UK Simona Cafazzo Wolf Science Center, Ernstbrunn, Austria Juliane Kaminski Psychology Department, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, UK Anna Kis Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary; Department of Ethology, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary Enikő Kubinyi Department of Ethology, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary Sarah Marshall-Pescini Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria; Wolf Sci- ence Centre, Ernstbrunn, Austria; Dipartimento di Fisiopatologia Medico-C hirurgica e dei Trapianti, Sezione di Neuroscienze, Università degli Studi di Milano Karen McComb School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, UK Ádám Miklósi Department of Ethology, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary Daniel Mills Animal Behaviour Cognition and Welfare Group, School of Life Sciences, University of Lincoln, Riseholme Park, Lincoln, UK Katalin Oláh Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary; Department of Cognitive Psychology, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary Péter Pongrácz Department of Ethology, Biological Institute, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary Emanuela Prato Previde Dipartimento di Fisiopatologia Medico-Chirurgica e dei Trapianti, Sezione di Neuroscienze, Università degli Studi di Milano, Segrate, Italy Angelo Quaranta Department of Veterinary Medicine, Section of Behavioural Sciences and Animal Bioethics, University of Bari ‘Aldo Moro’, Italy Friederike Range Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, Austria; Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria; University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria; Wolf Science Centre, Ernstbrunn, Austria Victoria Frances Ratcliffe School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, UK David Reby School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, UK xiii xiv Contributors Marcello Siniscalchi Department of Veterinary Medicine, Section of Behavioural Sciences and Animal Bioethics, University of Bari ‘Aldo Moro’, Italy Barbara Smuts Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA Anna Magdalena Taylor School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, UK József Topál Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Psychology, Research Centre for Natural Sciences, Hungarian Academy of Sciences, Budapest, Hungary Borbála Turcsán Department of Ethology, Eötvös University, Budapest, Hungary Paola Valsecchi Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Università degli Studi di Parma, Parma, Italy Emile van der Zee School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Brayford Pool, Lincoln, UK Zsófia Virányi Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine, Vienna, Austria; Medical University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria; University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria; Wolf Science Centre, Ernstbrunn, Austria Helen Zulch Animal Behaviour Cognition and Welfare Group, School of Life Sciences, University of Lincoln, Riseholme Park, Lincoln, UK Chapter 1 The Social Dog: History and Evolution Sarah Marshall-Pescini1,2,3 and Juliane Kaminski4 1Dipartimento di Fisiopatologia Medico-Chirurgica e dei Trapianti, Sezione di Neuroscienze, Università degli Studi di Milano, Milan, Italy, 2Comparative Cognition, Messerli Research Institute, University of Veterinary Medicine, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria, 3Wolf Science Centre, Ernstbrunn, Austria, 4Psychology Department, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth, UK 1.1 WHERE DO DOGS’ SOCIALITY AND SOCIO-COGNITIVE ABILITIES COME FROM? THE CANID STORY The explosion of studies on dogs’ social behaviour and cognitive abilities since the turn of the twenty-first century has been impressive (see Bensky et al., 2013, for a comprehensive review), and the many hypotheses as to the causes behind dog’s remarkable socio-cognitive abilities have engendered lively debates in journals and at conferences. However, most debates revolve around the wolf– dog comparison (the wolf being dog’s closest living relative), neglecting the fact that the dog’s canine family is much larger and shows some unique and intrigu- ing features that may well have played a role in allowing dogs’ emergence as our favoured social companions. Hence, in the first part of this chapter, we intro- duce dogs’ canine family, presenting some of these intriguing social features and highlighting some of the characteristics that may have played a fundamen- tal role in allowing the emergence of one species’ unique history with humans. 1.1.1 Introducing Dogs’ ‘Canine’ Family The domestic dog belongs to the Canidae family, consisting of 35 related species that diverged within the last ten million years (Wayne et al., 1997; Ostrander & Wayne, 2005). In recent years, there has been considerable interest in the evolu- tionary relationships between canids that has resulted in analyses based on both morphological (Berta, 1987; Tedford et al., 1995; Lyras & Van Der Geer, 2003; Zrzavý & Řičánková, 2004) and molecular data (Wayne et al., 1987; Wayne et al., 1989), including, more recently, DNA sequencing (Wayne et al., 1997; Bardeleben et al., 2005; Linblad-Toh et al., 2005; Wong et al., 2010). The devel- opment of methodologies for the sequencing of DNA has allowed researchers to The Social Dog. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407818-5.00001-2 Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 3 4 SECTION | I Theoretical Aspects reconstruct the dog’s family tree, with a certain amount of accuracy (although a few grey areas still exist). Taken together, current results converge in showing three major groupings within the dog’s family: (1) the red fox–like canids, (2) South American canids, and (3) wolf-like canids. Together, these three clades contain 93% of all living canids. A separate lineage comprising the grey fox seems to be the most primi- tive and suggests a North American origin of the living canids about ten million years ago (Ostrander & Wayne, 2005; Bardeleben et al., 2005; Lindblad-Toh et al., 2005; Graphodatsky et al., 2008) (see Figure 1-1). When one looks more closely at the wolf-like canids, results place grey wolves as the closest living ‘cousins’ of domestic dogs, followed by a close affiliation with coyotes, golden jackals, and Ethiopian wolves. These phylo- genetic relationships imply that the dog has several close relatives within its genus, confirmed by results showing that all members of Canis can produce fertile hybrids, and several species may have genomes that reflect hybridisation in the wild (Wayne & Jenks, 1991; Gottelli et al., 1994; Roy et al., 1996; Adams et al., 2003). Closest to the Canis group are the dhole and African wild dog (thus completing the members of the wolf-like canids). Dhole and African wild dogs do not, however, form a monophyletic group, and their exact relationship to the Canis genus is still somewhat unclear (Bardeleben et al., 2005; Zhang & Chen, 2011). Finally, results from genetic analyses also appear to support an African origin for the wolf-like canids because the two African jackals are the most basal members of this clade (Lindblad-Toh et al., 2005). 1.1.2 Evolution of the Canid Brain: A Socially Driven Phenomenon? Studies on the evolution of canids show that this family separated from the other mammals around 40 million years ago. Interestingly, a shift in canid encepha- lisation and architectural reorganisation of the brain (i.e., expansion of the pro- rean gyrus at the anterior end of the neocortex, general increase in the amount of infolding of the frontal lobe, and expansion of the prefrontal cortex; Radinsky, 1969, 1973; Lyras & Van der Geer, 2003) appears to have occurred sometime in the late Miocene or early Pliocene period, roughly coinciding also with a sudden taxonomic diversification (Van Valkenburgh, 1991) and expansion of global grasslands (Cerling et al., 1997). Based on these data, authors have put forward a number of suggestions as to the possible causes driving these changes in the brain. According to some authors, they may simply have been a by-product of a rapid taxonomic diversification in the new environment (Andersson, 2005); however, considering the energetic expenditure of big brains, it would seem more probable that such an expensive adaptation would be driven by some major adaptive advantage. Work by Van Valkenburgh and colleagues puts forward the possibility that, in fact, the onset of cooperative pack hunting (Van Valkenburgh Chapter | 1 The Social Dog: History and Evolution 5 100 Arctic fox 100 Kit fox 100 100 Corsac fox 52 67 98 81 Ruppell’s fox 99 100 Red fox 100 Cape fox 100 (4) 96 Blanford’s fox 100 Fennec fox 75 100 Raccoon dog Bat-eared fox Short-eared fox Crab-eating fox 100 100 – Sechuran fox 80 81 – 99 Culpeo fox – 100 – 99 – Pampas fox 100 99 99 Chilla 100 93 100 Darwin’s fox Hoary fox 96 Maned wolf 100 100 Bush dog 100 100 Side-striped jackal 100 3–4 100 Black-backed jackal 100 9–10 Myr Golden jackal 100 Myr 75 100 93 Dog 100 99 78 100 Grey wolf 100 100 Coyote 92 (2) 100 Ethiopian wolf 6–7.4 78 (9) Myr 100 Dhole African wild dog 100 Grey fox 100 Island fox 100 Black bear 100 Giant panda 100 Northern elephant seal 100 Walrus FIGURE 1-1 Branch colours identify the red fox–like clade (red), the South American clade (green), the wolf-like clade (blue), and the grey and island fox clade (orange). (From Lindblad-Toh et al., 2005.) et al., 2003; Van Valkenburgh et al., 2004) may have driven this change. How- ever, Finarelli’s (2008) analysis, taking into account a larger sample of both extinct and living canids, suggests that encephalisation increased in the three major living clades (wolf-like, fox-like, and South American canids) at the same time, yet most of the smaller-bodied canids (except for the South American bush dog) are not cooperative hunters. The trait that most canids share, however, 6 SECTION | I Theoretical Aspects is ‘monogamy’ (defined as a single male and single female mating exclusively with each other over multiple reproductive cycles) and (to differing extents) the cooperative rearing of the young. Hence, these authors suggest that it may have been these traits driving encephalisation and that pack hunting would then have emerged as a second-order adaptation, facilitated by, but not directly caus- ally related to, the reorganisation of the canid brain. This hypothesis seems to be plausible given another study showing that, among mammals and birds in general, it is not the size of the social group that correlates with brain size (as in primates), but instead it is species that live in pair-bonded social systems that have the largest brains (when phylogeny and a range of life history and ecologi- cal variables are partialled out; Shultz & Dunbar, 2007; Dunbar, 2009). 1.1.3 The Sociality of Canids’ Mating System: Pair Bonding and Cooperative Pup Rearing Taken as a whole, the dog’s canine family is one of the most fascinating amongst carnivores. As a start, they are the most widespread, with at least one species inhabiting every continent except Antarctica (although dogs, of course, live there as well), and some spread over entire continents (Sillero-Zubiri et al., 2004). Perhaps more interestingly for the purpose of this book, canids have a number of interesting social adaptations, starting from their mating system. Most species in this family are monogamous, a rare trait amongst mammals, but a potentially characteristic trait for canids (Kleiman, 1977, 2011). However, there is considerable interspecific variation in mating systems among canids, and polygamy, polyandry, and monogamy have been documented (Bekoff et al., 1981; Moehlman, 1989; Geffen et al., 1996; Carmicheal et al., 2007): for exam- ple, red foxes, Arctic foxes, and coyotes appear to adopt a largely monogamous strategy (Sillero-Zubiri et al., 2004; Hennessy et al., 2012; but see Carmicheal et al., 2007), whereas a recent study using genetic analyses to determine parent- hood across multiple populations of African wild dogs found that there was, in fact, a greater than previously reported incidence of reproductive sharing in that both beta females and males (and not just the dominant pair as previously thought) played a significant role in producing young (Spiering et al., 2010), suggesting a more promiscuous strategy in this species. Canids are also unique in that intraspecific variation in mating systems may be as great as interspecific variation (Moehlman, 1989); for example, swift foxes, Arctic foxes, and urban red foxes may all live either in monogamous pairs or polygamous groups depending on their ecological context (Baker et al., 2004; Kamler et al., 2004; Carmicheal et al., 2007; and see Geffen et al., 1996 for a comprehensive review of canid social flexibility). Indeed, it appears that the choice between a monogamous or polygamous strategy may even change within the same population, most probably in relation to food availability (red foxes: von Shantz, 1984; Zabel & Taggart, 1989). Such flexibility is not con- fined to the fox-like canids; indeed, Ethiopian wolves show a similar level of Chapter | 1 The Social Dog: History and Evolution 7 flexibility depending on ecological factors affecting food availability (Sillero- Zubiri et al., 1996; Marino et al., 2012). Alongside pair bonding, canids also show a variety of parental care strate- gies. In some species, mostly the female appears to care for the young—the most common strategy in mammals (e.g., bat-eared foxes and swift foxes: Kam- ler et al., 2004; Poessel & Gese, 2013); however, in others, male provisioning of both pups and lactating females occurs (e.g., black-backed jackals, red foxes, coyotes: Moehlman, 1989; Zabel & Taggart, 1989; Gese, 1998); and in yet oth- ers, both breeding individuals and their older offspring (that delay dispersal: Emlen, 1991) are involved in pup rearing (e.g., grey wolves, African wild dogs, dhole: Fritts & Mech, 1981; Malcolm & Marten, 1982; Creel & Creel, 2002; Sillero-Zubiri et al., 2004; Venkataraman & Johnsingh, 2004). In fact, coop- erative breeding, involving also the non-breeding pack members and including ‘helping behaviour’ such as den-site attendance; provisioning (including regur- gitation); play; and grooming are perhaps among the defining characteristics of a number of canid species (Macdonald, 1979; Moehlman, 1986; Mech et al., 1999; Packard, 2003). Such helping behaviour has been shown to have an adaptive value in a num- ber of cooperative breeding species (banded mongoose: Hodge, 2005; meerkats: Russell et al., 2007), although evidence in the canid family has been harder to obtain (Gusset & MacDonald, 2010; but see Wright, 2006, for paternal invest- ment on pup rearing success in bat-eared foxes and helper effect in red wolves; Sparkman et al., 2011). However, sociality in many canid species is not just apparent in pup rearing but emerges strongly also in relation to hunting and territorial defence. 1.1.4 The Sociality of Canids’ Hunting and Defence Strategies Canid hunting strategies vary widely, ranging from species that feed largely solitarily on fish and insects, like the short-eared dog (Sillero-Zubiri et al., 2004), to hypercarnivore species that hunt in packs, such as the African hunt- ing dog, bush dog, wolf, and dhole (Venkataraman et al., 1995; Creel & Creel, 2002; Mech & Boitani, 2003; Sillero-Zubiri et al., 2004). Hunting techniques are intimately linked with sociality levels in canids, because species that hunt cooperatively are also described as the most social. Indeed, a number of authors have suggested that pack hunting may be the major force behind canid social- ity (Alexander, 1974; Pulliam & Caraco, 1984; Clark & Mangel, 1986). Group hunting, in theory, may allow the capture of larger prey. However, the cause and effect of such an argument may be difficult to tease apart: do some canid species live in larger groups because in so doing they can hunt larger prey, or does the availability of predominantly larger prey ‘oblige them’ to live in groups? In a chapter reviewing the topic, MacDonald et al. (2004) suggest that at present the evidence cannot tease these two alternatives apart, although novel methods of modelling hunting success seem to suggest that, at least in African wild dogs,