JournalofPersonality85:4,August2017 The Relation Between Valence and VC 2016WileyPeriodicals,Inc. DOI:10.1111/jopy.12258 Arousal in Subjective Experience Varies With Personality and Culture Peter Kuppens,1 Francis Tuerlinckx,1 Michelle Yik,2 Peter Koval,1,3 Joachim Coosemans,1 Kevin J. Zeng,2 and James A. Russell4 1KULeuven–UniversityofLeuven 2HongKongUniversityofScienceandTechnology 3AustralianCatholicUniversity 4BostonCollege Abstract Objective: Whileingeneralarousalincreaseswithpositiveornegativevalence(aso-calledV-shapedrelation),therearelarge differences among individuals in how these two fundamental dimensions of affect are related in people’s experience. In two studies,weexaminedtwopossiblesourcesofthisvariation:personalityandculture. Method: In Study 1, participants (Belgian university students) recalled a recent event that was characterized by high or low valence or arousal and reported on their feelings and their personality in terms of the Five-Factor Model. InStudy 2, partici- pants from Canada, China/Hong Kong, Japan, Korea, and Spain reported on their feelings in a thin slice of time and on their personality. Results: InStudy1,wereplicatedtheV-shapeascharacterizingtherelationbetweenvalenceandarousal,andidentifiedper- sonality correlates of experiencing particular valence–arousal combinations. In Study 2, we documented how the V-shaped relationvariedasafunctionofWesternversusEasternculturalbackgroundandpersonality. Conclusions: The results showed that the steepness of the V-shaped relation between valence and arousal increases with Extraversionwithincultures,andwithaWest-Eastdistinctionbetweencultures.Implicationsforthepersonality–emotionlink andresearchonculturaldifferencesinaffectarediscussed. Keywords: Valence, arousal, pleasure, activation, structure of affect, structure of emotion, personality, culture Subjectively experienced affect—how we feel—is a central encedaffect(Yik,Russell,& Barrett,1999).Thecurrentstudy aspectofthemind,playingafundamentalroleindiversepsycho- focusesonthesetwodimensionswithoutassumingvalenceand logical phenomena, including perception (Vuilleumier, 2005), arousalaretheonlydimensionsofaffect. categorization (Niedenthal, Halberstadt, & Innes-Ker, 1999), The second basic question then becomes how valence and decision making (Forgas, 1995), well-being, and psychopathol- arousal are related to each other. Answering this question is ogy(Watson,2000).Consequently,understandinghowaffectis importantfortheoretical(whatisthestructureofaffectiveexpe- rience?),empirical(whatistherelationofaffecttootherpsycho- structuredinsubjectiveexperienceisoneofthemostfundamental logical phenomena, from attention to well-being?), and and long-standing challenges to the science of psychology psychometricreasons(canvalenceandarousalbemeasuredor (Diener, 1999; Wundt, 1924). To address this challenge, two manipulated independently?). Several relationships have been basic questions need to be answered: What are the fundamental suggested,or,moreoften,assumed,including(a)independence dimensionsunderlyingaffect,andhowaretheyinterrelated? betweenthetwo(howpleasantorunpleasantoneisfeelingcon- Regardingthefirstquestion,differentanswershavebeenfor- veysnoinformationabouthowarousedoneisfeelingandvice mulated(Fontaine,Scherer,Roesch,&Ellsworth,2007;Larsen versa; e.g., Barrett & Russell, 1999; Carver & Scheier, 1990), &Diener,1992;Osgood,May,&Mirron,1975;Russell,1980; Thayer,1989;Watson&Tellegen,1985),andyettheyallcon- CorrespondenceconcerningthisarticleshouldbeaddressedtoPeter verge on identifying valence (i.e., pleasure-displeasure or Kuppens,FacultyofPsychologyandEducationalSciences,KULeuven– hedonictone)andarousal(i.e.,senseofenergyoractivation)as UniversityofLeuven,Tiensestraat102,3000Leuven,Belgium.Email:peter. amongthe mostfundamental properties of subjectivelyexperi- [email protected]. Relation of Valence to Arousal 531 Figure1 Overviewofpossiblerelationsandassociatedmodelequationsbetweenvalenceandarousal:(a)independence,(b)(positive)linearrelation,(d) symmetricV-shapedrelation,and(e)asymmetricV-shapedrelationincludingbothpositivityoffsetandnegativitybias. (b) a strictly linear relation between valence and arousal (how aresult,wedonotknowwhetheranyrelationfoundcharacter- pleasant one feels increases [or decreases] how aroused one izes the structure of affect universally. Second, Kuppens, feels), (c) the equation of arousal with the intensity of positive Tuerlinckx,Russell,andBarrett(2013)observedthatthesepro- andofnegativevalence(themorepositiveornegativeonefeels, posalsareallaimedatanomotheticlevel.Kuppensetal.(2013) themorearousedonefeels—graphicallyseenasaV-shapedpat- demonstrated that (a) any relationship between valence and ternofarousalasa function of valence;e.g.,Clore,Ortony, & arousalatthenomotheticlevelaccountsforonlyasmallpropor- Foss,1987;Lang1994),and(d)anasymmetricV-shapedrela- tionofthetotalvariance,and(b)thereissufficientvariationat tionshipwithapositivityoffset(feelingsofarousalarehigherat theidiographic(orwithin-person)levelthatvalenceandarousal lowlevelsofpositivevalenceascomparedtonegativevalence) caninprincipleshowanyrelationshipdependingontheperson and a negativity bias (negative feelings increase more strongly and the situation (e.g., the nomothetic relation accounted for thandopositivefeelingswithincreasingarousal;e.g.,Cacioppo only 1–18% of the total observed variance across studies). In &Gardner,1999).Figure1providesavisualdepictionofeach otherwords,howvalenceandarousalarerelatedcanbemark- oftheseproposedrelations. edly different from one individual toanother, ranging from V- Inadditiontotheexistenceofcompetingproposals,twofur- shaped, to linear, to inverted V-shaped relationships, or even ther limitations preclude us from accepting any such proposal. independence. This malleability means that for some individu- First,thefindingsandconclusionsfrompreviousresearchwere als, high pleasant and/or unpleasant affect is habitually experi- basedexclusivelyonWesternsamples.Moreover,toourknowl- encedincombinationwithhigherlevelsofarousal,whereasfor edge, no cross-cultural research has addressed these four com- others it may be experienced with lower levels of arousal, and peting modelsor indeed the general question ofthe relationof for some, valence and arousal are experienced relatively inde- valencetoarousal(e.g.,Perunovic,Heller,&Rafaeli,2007).As pendently. Large variation at the idiographic level underscores 532 Kuppens, Tuerlinckx, Yik, et al. the large malleability of the structure of affect. This variability predictthe habitualexperience of specific affective states(e.g., reflectsanimportantyetunderstudiedsourceofdiversityinsub- DeNeve & Cooper, 1998; Kuppens et al., 2007; Larsen & jective emotional experience and thus has important implica- Ketelaar, 1991; Watson & Clark, 1984; Yik, Russell, Ahn, tions for understanding the antecedents and consequences of Fern(cid:2)andezDols,&Suzuki,2002). affectivestates.Forexample,idiographicvariationinthestruc- Extraversion has consistently been found to be related to ture of affect would cast doubt on the existence of a universal increased experience of positive emotions (see Lucas, Le, & neuralarchitectureasthedeterminantoftherelationofvalence Dyrenforth,2008,forameta-analysis). to arousal. Moreover, idiographic variation in the valence– Perhaps because of its specific responsivity to appetitive arousalrelationmayimplythatvalenceandarousalmaynotuni- stimuli that involve the possibility of reward, Extraversion is formlyaffectperception,memory,well-being,andsoon(fora particularly related to the experience of high arousal positive more elaborate discussion, see Kuppens et al., 2013). Thus, affect(Smillie,Cooper,Wilt,&Revelle,2012).Inotherwords, explainingthisvariationisofcriticalimportanceforunderstand- when aroused or when feeling pleasant, it is more likely for ingindividualdifferencesinthesubjectiveexperienceofaffect extravertscomparedtointrovertstoalsoexperiencehighlevels andhowaffectimpactsotherphenomena.Furthermore,theexis- ofpositivefeelingsandarousal,respectively. tence of reliable and meaningful individual differences in the Neuroticism,inturn,isconsideredtoreflectthedispositional valence–arousal relationship would be further established by tendencytoexperiencenegativeemotions(Larsen&Buss,2015). showing that these differences are related to known sources of Unlike Extraversion, which seems to be particularly individual differences in order to explain part of the observed geared to the experience of high arousal states, Neuroticism idiographicvariation. has equally been related to the habitual experience of both Thepresentarticleaddressestwoissues.First,weprovidefur- low arousal negative feelings, such as sadness and depres- ther evidence on the relation between valence and arousal. Our sion, as to the experience of high arousal negative feelings, hypothesis is that the relationship is characterized by a weak, suchasstressandanxiety(Matthews&Deary,1998;Watson, asymmetric V-shape atthe nomothetic level but bylarge varia- Gamez,&Simms,2005). tionattheidiographiclevel(Kuppensetal.,2013).Second,we Based on this literature, we hypothesized that Extraversion aimtocontributetounderstandingthenatureoftheidiographic wouldbepositivelyrelatedtoindividualdifferencesinvalence variation by looking into how it correlates with personality when people experience high arousal (e.g., extraverts gravitate (Study 1) and culture (Studies 1 and 2). Indeed, personality is toward feeling pleasant when feeling active) and to higher assumed toreflect the mostbasic individual differences dimen- arousal when feeling positive (e.g., extraverts gravitate toward sions in how people behave, think, and feel (Larsen & Buss, feeling active when feeling pleasant). In other words, people 2014),andcultureisassumedtohaveaprofoundinfluenceon, who are high on Extraversion would experience more positive orisevenconstitutedof,thewayspeopleexperiencetheirworld feelings accompanied by higher levels of arousal. In addition, emotionally(e.g.,Mesquita,DeLeersnyder,&Boiger,inpress). we hypothesized that Neuroticism would be negatively related tovalencewhetherarousalisloworhigh(i.e.,neuroticsgravi- tatetowardfeelingunpleasant). STUDY 1:PERSONALITYCORRELATES Totestthesehypotheses,weconductedastudyinwhichpar- OF VARIATIONIN THE RELATION ticipants completed a personality questionnaire assessing the BETWEEN VALENCE ANDAROUSAL FFM dimensions, recalled four recent events (one highly acti- Thelargeindividualdifferencesinthevalence–arousalrelation vated,onelowinactivation,onehighlypleasant,andonehighly raise the important question of the extent to which this idio- unpleasant), and reported their experienced levels of valence graphicvariationismeaningful: Isthe variation inthe habitual andarousalinthoseevents.Thisevent-recallstudyallowedfor occurrence of particular valence–arousal combinations simply idiographic variability in experiencing valence in relation to random samplingerror,orcanitbeexplainedintermsofrela- highversuslowlevelsofarousal(Events1and2),andvariabili- tionships with well-established, independently defined sources tyinexperiencingarousalinrelationtopositiveversusnegative ofindividualdifferences?Ananswertothisquestionwouldcon- valence(Events3and4),anditenabledustoexaminehowthis tributetobuildinganomologicalnetworkaroundtheobserved variabilitywasrelatedtopersonality. idiographic variation in valence–arousal relations and would highlight the emotional concomitants of important established Method dimensionsofindividualdifferences. In this study, we examined the associations between idio- Participants. A total of 482 first-year Belgian, Dutch- graphic variability in experiencing valence–arousal combina- speakinguniversitystudents(398females;M 519years)par- age tions and individual differences in the personality dimensions ticipatedinreturnforcoursecredit. definedbytheFive-FactorModelofpersonality(FFM;McCrae &Costa,1987).Alargeliteraturehasdocumentedhowperson- Materials and Procedure. In a Dutch-language computer- ality dimensions, particularly Extraversion and Neuroticism, izedtask,participantsfirstreadshortdescriptionsofthepleasure Relation of Valence to Arousal 533 Figure2 Relationship between valence and arousal in the (a) high arousal (based on all data), (b) low arousal (based on all data), (c) positive valence (basedonpositivevalencedataonly),and(d)negativevalence(basedonnegativevalencedataonly)conditions.Forillustrativepurposes,thedashedline reflectsthenon-parametricrelationship.Thepointsaretheplottedrawdata(Study1). andarousaldimensions.Next,participantswereaskedtorecall .44 to –.58 per condition). The final score for valence was the four different events: the last time they had felt echt actief meanofthreescores:thebipolarSAMvalenceratings,thebipo- (“really active”; high arousal condition), echt inactief (“really lar verbal scale valence score, and the valence score based on inactive”;lowarousalcondition),echtgoed(“verygood”;posi- theunipolaritems.Thefinalarousalscorewassimilarlydefined. tive valence condition), and echt slecht (“very bad”; negative Thereliabilityofthesemultisourcecompoundscoreswasgood valencecondition). (Cronbach’sasforvalencerangedfrom.68to.95acrosscondi- Thefoureventdescriptionswerepresentedinaseparateran- tions;thoseforarousalrangedfrom.63to.79). domorderforeachparticipant.Whentryingtorecalleachevent, Finally,participantscompletedacomputerizedversionofthe participantswereinstructedtoclosetheireyes,relivetheexperi- Dutch translation of the Ten-Item Personality Inventory (TIPI; ence,andrememberexactlyhowtheyfeltatthetime.Theynext Hofmans,Kuppens,&Allik,2008),whichmeasureseachofthe typedashortdescriptionoftheeventintoatextbox.1 fivepersonalitydimensionsbymeansoftwo(opposite)itemsto Next, their feelings of arousal and pleasure were assessed bescoredona7-pointscalerangingfrom1(describesmenotat using multiple measures (while their description of the event all)to7(describesmeverywell). remainedatthetopofthescreen).Allratingsweremadeon9- point scales. The first measure was the Self-Assessment- Results Manikin(SAM;Bradley&Lang,2007),whichconsistsoftwo bipolarratingsscaleswithpictorialmanikinsrepresentingvary- Nomothetic Relationship. While the design of this study ingvaluesofpleasure(rangingfromunpleasanttopleasant)and was primarily intended to explain idiographic variation in the arousal(rangingfromlowtohigharousal),whichwererecoded experienceofparticularvalence–arousalcombinations,thedata torangefrom24to14.Thesecondmeasureconsistedoftwo canneverthelessbeusedtoverifywhethertheyconformtopre- semanticdifferentialitemsrangingfromverybadtoverygood dictions that follow from the hypothesis that a nomothetic V- (valence) and from very passive to very active (arousal), again shapedrelationbetweenvalenceandarousalfitsthedatabetter recodedtorangefrom24to14.Thethirdsetofmeasurescon- than other models suggested in the literature. To this end, we sistedof16unipolaradjectivescales,twoitemsforeachofeight examinedthe nomotheticrelationbetweenvalence andarousal combinations of arousal and valence (high arousal-positive: ineachcondition.Wefitdifferent statistical regression models enthusiastic, peppy; neutral arousal-positive: pleasant, good; inwhicharousalwaspredictedbyvalenceinwaysreflectingthe low arousal-positive: calm, relaxed; low arousal-neutral differenttheoreticalmodels.Forthehighandlowarousalcondi- valence:passive,sleepy;lowarousal-negative:bored,sad;neu- tions, we tested models involving (a) independence (Model 1: tral arousal-negative: bad, unpleasant; high arousal-negative: predictingarousalbyaninterceptonly;seeequationwithFigure nervous, stressed; high arousal-neutral valence: active, acti- 1a),(b)alinearrelation(Model2:predictingarousalbyvalence; vated).Onthe basis ofthese items,a valence scorewas calcu- seeequationwithFigure1b),(c)asymmetricalV-shapedrela- latedbysubtractingthemeannegativevalencecompositescore tion (Model 3: predicting arousal by the absolute value of from the mean positive valence composite score (intercorrela- valence;seeequationwithFigure1c),and(d)variousinstances tionbetweenboth5–.90,p<.001acrossconditions,andrang- of asymmetrical V-shaped relations (Models 4–6: predicting ing between –.48 to –.84 per condition); an arousal score was arousalbytheabsolutevalueofvalenceandallowingfordiffer- calculatedbysubtractingthemeanlowarousalcompositescore ences in intercept and/or slope for positive and negative from the mean high arousal score (intercorrelation between valence—models that reflect positivity offset and/or negativity both5–.77,p<.001acrossconditions,andrangingbetween– bias; see equation with Figure 1d). Finally, Model 7 tests a 534 Kuppens, Tuerlinckx, Yik, et al. 2aR .02 .05 .00 .03 .05.00 .04.02.03 aredis- el d c 00 00 01 00 0000 000001 mo Model7parametri PostP . . . . .. ... best-fitting n No BIC 459 611 867 022 870359 463111853 the 1 1 1 2 11 11 of es u Model6AsymmetricVPositivity1OffsetNegativ-ityBias BICPostP 456.00 608.02 —— —— 864.03358.00 462.01108.01855.00 enmodels.Theval 1 1 11 11 ev s of Model5mmetricVativityBias PostP .05 .07 — — .05.00 .05.09.06 mongtheset AsyNeg BIC 1451 1605 — — 18621358 14571102850 dataa chStudy Vset P .04 .33 — — .04.02 .09.10.07 giventhe ValenceforEa Model4AsymmetricPositivityOff BICPost 1452 1602 — — 18631355 14561102850 ofeachmodel y asaFunctionof Model3mmetricV PostP .78 .57 — — .88.05 .84.61.49 osteriorprobabilit odeled Sy BIC 1446 1601 — — 18571353 14521098846 5ostPp M P WhenArousalIs Model2LinearRelation BICPostP 453.02 623.00 866.08 001.99 881.00353.04 469.00106.01852.02 esreflectbetterfit); cs 1 1 1 2 11 11 alu atisti 1 0 1 1 07 084 werv SelectionSt Model1ependence PostP .1 .0 .9 .0 .0.8 .0.1.3 criterion(lo ofModel Ind BIC 1449 1618 1861 2010 18781347 14641101847 nformation model. Table1Summary Study Study1HigharousalconditionLowarousalconditionPositivevalenceconditionNegativevalenceconditionStudy2CanadaChina/HongKongJapanKoreaSpain 5Note.BICBayesianiplayedinboldface.a2Rofthebest-fitting Relation of Valence to Arousal 535 nonparametric relation between valence and arousal, where conditions),orinonelegoftheV-shape(thepositiveleginthe arousalismodeledasafreefunctionofvalenceinordertodetect positive valencecondition andthe negativeleg inthe negative possibleshapesnotseeninthepreviousmodels. valence condition) was moderated by personality. To this end, However, the positive and negative valence conditions we performed 20 separate regression analyses across partici- allowed us to test only part of the nomothetic V-shape predic- pantsinwhicharousalwaspredictedbyaV-shapedfunctionof tion, namely, the increase of arousal with increasing positive valence(inthecaseofthehighandlowarousalconditions)ora and negative valence, respectively. Indeed, due to the event- linearfunction(inthecaseofthepositiveandnegativevalence recall manipulation, valence scores were negatively skewed in conditions) in interaction with each of the five personality thepositivevalenceconditionandpositivelyskewedintheneg- dimensions(separatelyforeachpersonalitydimension).Innone ativevalencecondition.Asaresult,itdoesnotmakesensetofit of these analyses, however, was the V-shape or one leg of it V-shaped models in these conditions. Therefore, only models moderatedbypersonality(allps>.05). involving independence (Model 1), a linear relation (Model 2; Second, we examined the extent to which personality was whichwoulddetectonelegofaV-shape),andanonparametric related to the experience of each particular valence–arousal relation(Model7)fittedtothedatainvolvedpositiveandnega- combination. Specifically, we regressed the valence scores on tivevalencescoresundertheseinstructions,respectively. allfive personality dimensions separatelyfor the high and low Subsequently, we applied model-selection procedures to arousal conditions, and the arousal scores on the same five select the best-fitting model, relying on the Bayesian informa- dimensionsseparatelyforthehighandlowvalenceconditions. tion criterion (BIC), a model selection criterion that balances Theseanalysesinformushowindividualdifferencesintheper- modelfitwithparsimony,andposteriorprobabilityestimatesof sonality dimensions uniquely relate to idiographic variation in model fit, which provide a quantification of uncertainty as a valence when feeling highly active or passive, and to arousal probability statement (i.e., determining the probability that the whenfeelingverypositiveornegative.Byprovidingthisinfor- modelistrueafterhavingseenthedata;Raftery,1995). mationaboutthevalence–arousalcombinationspeoplewithcer- The results can be found in Table 1. In the high and low tain personality traits tend to experience, we can infer how arousal conditions, both the BIC and the posterior probability personalityshapestherelationbetweenvalenceandarousal. statisticsfavoredaV-shapedrelationship(Model3).Intheposi- In the high arousal condition, Extraversion predicted more tive valence condition, independence was favored, and in the positivevalence(b5.19,SE5.06,p<.01;theotherfourper- negative valence condition, a linear relationship was favored. sonality dimensions failed to make a significant contribution, Theselectedmodelsforthedifferentconditionsaregraphically ps>.05). In the low arousal condition, only Neuroticism pre- illustrated in Figure 2, along with the nonparametric model to dicted more negative valence (b5–.16, SE5.08, p<.05; all illustratetherelationbetweenvalenceandarousalwhennolin- otherps>.05).2Inthepositivevalencecondition,Extraversion earrestrictionsareimposed.Thesefiguresclearlyshowthatfeel- (b5.20, SE5.05, p<.001) and Agreeableness (b5.30, ing more positive or negative is on average accompanied by SE5.08, p<.001) each predicted higher arousal (all other feeling more arousal in almost all conditions, essentially con- ps>.05). In the negative valence condition, Neuroticism firmingtheV-shapedrelationbetweenvalenceandarousal.Fig- (b5.15, SE5.06, p<.05) and Extraversion (b5.15, ure 2c, reflecting the positive valence condition, in which an SE5.06,p<.05)predictedhigherarousal,whereasAgreeable- independence model was favored, is an exception to this rela- ness (b5–.21, SE5.09, p<.05) predicted lower arousal (all tion, although the nonparametric function again goes in the otherps>.05). directionofa(one-armed)V-shape. Idiographic Variation. Thewidescatteringoftheplotteddata Discussion in the Figure 2 panels clearly illustrates, however, that partici- pantsstronglydifferedinhowarousedtheyfeltwhentheyfelt As expected, valence and arousal were found to be related to verygoodorverybad,andinhowpositiveornegativetheyfelt oneanotheratthenomotheticlevel.Therewerereliabletenden- when they felt very high or low arousal, showcasing the large cies for certain combinations of valence and arousal to occur idiographicvariationinvalence–arousalcombinations.Thesize morefrequentlythanothercombinations.Thisvalence–arousal of the idiographic deviation from the overall relation is also relationship was not simply linear. On average, as people felt illustrated bythe very low R-squared values (ranging from .00 more intensely positive or more intensely negative, they felt to.05)reportedinTable1(seealsoKuppensetal.,2013,report- morearoused: Intensityineithervalence directionwas accom- ingvaluesbetween.01and.34,withamedianof.08)thatindi- paniedbymorearousal. cate howmuchofthe varianceinthe data ofeachconditionis However,ascanbeobservedfromthedatainFigure2,this explainedbytheselectednomotheticmodel. relationshipwasbynomeanstrueforallindividuals.Themag- Toexaminehowmuchofthevariationcouldbeexplainedby nitudeoftherelationwassmallatthenomotheticlevel,andany personality, we took two approaches. First, we examined to combinationofvalenceandarousalispossible,andindeedfre- what extent variation in the slope of the entire V-shape or the quent. Contrary to the overall V-shaped relation at the nomo- positiveornegativelegoftheV-shapeseparately(inthearousal theticlevel,someindividualsreportedfeelinglowarousallevels 536 Kuppens, Tuerlinckx, Yik, et al. incombinationwithhighlypositiveorhighlynegativevalence. (e.g., relaxed and peaceful feelings). Such differences in ideal Similarly, when participants were instructed to remember affectarethoughttooriginatefromculturaldifferencesininter- instances when they felt high or low arousal, they reported a personalgoals.InWesternsocieties,theemphasisliesoninde- widerangeofvalencescores.However,thiswaslessthecasein pendence, influencing the environment, and personal thehigharousalcondition,inwhichparticipantspredominantly achievement, whereas in Eastern societies, the focus lies more reportedpositiveexperiences(seeFigure2a).Thus,theinstruc- on interdependence, adjustment to the environment, and inter- tion to remember a highly active experience seemed to prime personalharmony(Markus&Kitayama,1991;Tsai,2007). primarilypositivememories,atleastinaWesternEuropeancul- Althoughidealaffectisnotalwaysachievedinactualexperi- turalcontext. encedaffect,culturaldifferencesinthetwocertainlyalign(Tsai, The observed idiographic variation in scores was related to 2007).Asa consequence,people fromdifferent cultures might established dimensions of personality. Extraverts tended to differintheirnomotheticexperienceofvalence–arousalcombi- experiencehigherarousalcomparedtointrovertsincombination nations:Peoplemightsucceedinachievingtheirideal.InWest- withpositiveandnegativevalence,andhigherpositivevalence ern societies, the preference for high arousal positive affect in combination with high arousal. Individuals scoring high on wouldcreateahigherprevalenceofhigh(vs.low)arousalposi- Neuroticism tended to experience high arousal in combination tive states; in Eastern societies, the preference for low arousal withnegativevalenceandnegativevalenceincombinationwith positive affect would create a higher prevalence for low (vs. lowarousal.Inaddition,agreeableindividualstendedtoexperi- high)arousalpositivestates.Combinedwithareasonablyantici- encehigharousalincombinationwithpositivevalence,butlow pated preponderance of average arousal levels experienced at arousal in combination with negative valence. These results lowpositiveandnegativevalence,thesepreferencescouldtrans- showed that individual differences in experiencing particular lateintomoreorlesssteep,orevennegative,positivevalence– valence–arousalcombinationsaremeaningfullyrelatedtobroad arousalrelationsacrosscultures. personality dimensions in ways consistent with previous find- Predictionsregardingnegativeaffectarelessstraightforward, ings on these personality traits. Regarding Extraversion, early as ideal affect is understandably situated on the positive rather theoriesalreadylinkedthispersonalitydimensionwiththeseek- than negative side, and less research on ideal negative affect ing of high arousal states (Eysenck, 1967); Neuroticism is a exists.Still,peopledoonoccasionseekoutandachievenegative strong predictor of negative emotionality both in the high and affect(Tamir,2009).Anintriguingquestion,therefore,iswhether lowarousalrealm(Barlow,Sauer-Zavala,Carl,Bullis,&Ellard, a cultural proneness for experiencing high or low arousal affect 2014); the low anger-proneness associated with Agreeableness on the positive side also generalizes to the negatively valenced (e.g.,Kuppens,2005)maypredisposeindividualstoexperience realmoffeelingstates.Thereareseveraltheoreticalreasonswhy this may be so. High arousal negative states such as anger and lowratherthanhigharousalnegativestates. anxietyarise,inpart,tomobilizeenergyforinfluencingtheenvi- Thecurrentstudydesignfocusedonparticularregionsofthe ronment,andthemotivationtoexertinfluenceoneventsiscon- affectivespace.Hence,itwasnotdesignedtoestablishpersonal- sideredtobemoredominantinWesternthaninEasterncultures, itymoderationofglobalpatternsofvalenceandarousalacross which are more concerned with adjustment (Tsai, 2007). The theentireaffectivespace(suchasaV-shape).Forthispurpose, oppositeholdsforlowarousalnegativestatessuchassadnessand dataareneededthatpotentiallyrangeacrosstheentireaffective depression, which reflect passive compliance rather than active realm (such as those based on uninstructed self-reported affect interactionwithongoingevents.Empirically,culturaldifferences inathinsliceoftime). inthevalence–arousalrelationremainunexplored. InStudy2,we examinedpossible culturaldifferences inthe STUDY 2:CULTURALCORRELATESOF nomothetic relation between valence and arousal by analyzing VARIATIONINTHE RELATION existingdatacollectedbyYiketal.(2002)infivedistinctcultural contexts:Canada,China/HongKong,Japan,Korea,andSpain.3 BETWEEN VALENCE ANDAROUSAL Thesecountrieswereselectedbecausetheyrepresentfivedif- Most data reported in the literature (including our Study 1) on ferentlanguagegroups(seeYiketal.,2002)andshowsignifi- therelationbetweenvalenceandarousaloriginatedfromWest- cant variation on basic cross-cultural dimensions (e.g., with ernsamples.Asaresult,thequestionremainswhetherthecon- Spain being more collectivistic thanmostother Western coun- clusions drawn could be generalized to non-Western samples. tries,butmoreindividualisticthanEasterncountries;Hofstede, Perhapstherelationsfound,suchastheshallowV-shape,reflect 2001). Participants recalled a moment from the preceding day, a culture-specific feature of the structure of affect stemming basedonwhichtheyprovidedaffectratingsthatwerethenused fromaWesternlikingofhigharousalaffectivestates. tocalculatecompoundscoresofvalenceandarousal.Thesedata Therearegoodreasonstocontemplatethislatterpossibility. also contained scores on the FFM personality dimensions, Tsai,Knutson,andFung(2006)convincinglydemonstratedthat therebyallowingustoexaminefurtherthepersonalitycorrelates members from Western cultures prefer high arousal positive ofthevalence–arousalrelationshipbasedondataforwhichpar- affect(e.g.,feelingexcitedandenthusiastic),whereasmembers ticipants were not instructed to report on particular restricted of Eastern cultures instead prefer low arousal positive states regionsoftheaffectivespace. Relation of Valence to Arousal 537 Figure4 Relationship between valence and arousal for people scoring Figure3 Relationshipbetweenvalenceandarousalpercountryaccording highversuslow(basedonmediansplit)onExtraversion(E)(Study2). toamodelthatassumesasymmetricV-shaperelation(Study2). aroused, hyperactivated, activated), and deactivation (adjec- Method tives: sleepy,still,quiet).Next,the study authorsidentifiedthe Participants. Participants were undergraduate student volun- items thatwereculturally and theoreticallyappropriatetoindi- teers.TheCanadian(Vancouver)datacamefrom535participants catepositiveandnegativevalenceandactivatedanddeactivated (294 females; M 520 years); the Chinese/Hong Kong data arousalineachculture.Measurementinvarianceoftheresulting age from487participants(323females;M 520years);theJapa- item groups was tested using multigroup confirmatory factor age nese (Kyoto) data from 450 participants (222 females; analyses (Brown, 2015), with corresponding item parcels for M 520 years); the Korean (Busan) data from 365 partici- eachresponseformatservingasindicatorsforpositivevalence, age pants (189 females; M 521 years); and the Spanish (Mad- negative valence, activation, and deactivation. Model selection age rid)data from233participants (37females;M 520years). (based on BIC) indicated weak invariance (CFI5.946, age Theyparticipatedinthestudyinreturnforcoursecredit. RMSEA5.094),suggestingthatthefactorloadingsofthepar- celscanbeconsideredtobeequalacrossgroups.Becauseposi- Materials and Procedure. To measure momentary affect, tive and negative valence scores were strongly negatively wereliedonthe“rememberedmomentsmethod”inwhichpar- correlated (intercorrelation between both5–.81, p<.001 ticipantswereaskedtodescribetheiraffectinamomentduring acrosscountries,andrangingbetween–.73to–.87percountry), theprecedingdaythattheyrememberedwell.4Theywereasked wecreatedavalencescorebysubtractingnegativefrompositive toreporttheiraffectusingfourquestionnaires,eachinadifferent valence scores for each of the three formats. Similarly, an format.The rationale for the inclusion of multipleformatswas arousalscorewascalculatedbysubtractingdeactivedfromacti- toobtainscoresthatarenotdependentonaparticularscalefor- vated scores (intercorrelation between both5–.51, p<.001 mat,andtocaptureemotionalresponsesindependentofassess- acrosscountries,andrangingbetween–.35to–.63percountry). ment method (see also Yik et al., 2002). A first format used Next,valencearousalscoreswereaveragedacrossthethreefor- visual analogue scales with opposing emotion terms at either matstoobtainafinalvalenceandarousalscoreforeachsubject. end, a second format involved indicating to what extent they Cronbach’salphasfromthefivesamplesrangedfrom.95to.97 experienced a number of emotions termed as adjectives on a for valence,and from.75 to.91 for arousal,indicatingreliable scalerangingfrom1(notatall)to5(extremely),athirdformat scales. involved indicating their agreement on a scale ranging from 1 In addition, personality was measured with the NEO Five- (stronglydisagree)to5(stronglyagree)withanumberofstate- FactorInventory(NEO-FFI;Costa&McCrae,1992),a60-item ments regarding their feeling state, and a fourth format asked questionnairedesignedtomeasuretheFFMdimensionsofper- how well a number of statements describe their feeling at the sonality. Each of the five factors is represented by 12 items. timeonascalerangingfrom1(notatall)to4(verywell).Each Responses are made on a 5-point rating scale ranging from formatexceptforthefirstonecontaineditemsthatcouldbeused stronglydisagreethroughneutraltostronglyagree.Cronbach’s tocomputescalescoresforpositivevalence(adjectives:happy, alphasfromthefivesamplesrangedfrom.62to.83,indicating pleasant,content,satisfied),negativevalence(adjectives:miser- thatthescaleswereinternallyconsistent,similartotheoriginal able, troubled, unhappy, dissatisfied), activation (adjectives: Englishscales(McCraeetal.,1998).Modelselection(basedon 538 Kuppens, Tuerlinckx, Yik, et al. BIC) testing different types of measurement invariance using p5.038),butnotofthenegativevalence–arousalslope(B5– multigroupconfirmatoryfactoranalysespointedtoweakinvari- .07,SE5.08,p5.398).TovisualizethemoderationbyExtra- ance, indicating that factor loadings can be considered to be version, Figure 4 displays the V-shape relationship between equal across groups (both for models involving 12 individual valenceandarousalforparticipantswithhighandlowlevelsof itemsperscale,CFI5.602,RMSEA5.063,orparcelsconsist- Extraversion. While in the model underlying this figure Extra- ingofthreeorfouritemsperscale,CFI5.832,RMSEA5.094, versionisallowedtomoderatetheinterceptandslopeofthepos- andCFI5.825,RMSEA5.080,respectively). itive and negative leg of the V-shape separately (based on median split), it is important to point out that again only the moderationofthepositiveslopereachesstatisticalsignificance.6 Results Second, although no clear-cut predictions could be formu- First,weexaminedthenomotheticrelationshipbetweenvalence lated based on the results from Study 1, we examined in two andarousalpercountry.AsinStudy1,thebest-fittingofseven similar models how Neuroticism (again country-centered) modelswasselectedbasedontheassociatedBICandposterior related to the intercept and slope(s) of the V-shape. The first probabilityestimates,separatelyforeachcountry.Theresultsof model showed that Neuroticism predicted a higher level of thismodelselectioncanbefoundinTable1.Inallculturesbut arousal at neutral valence (intercept: B5.20, SE5.09, one (China/Hong Kong), a symmetric V-shape (Model 3) was p5.027) and a slightly less steep slope overall (B5–.08, the most dominant pattern in the data. The order of countries, SE5.04,p5.046).However,the secondmodel didnotshow fromlargesttosmallest,basedonthesizeoftheslope(steepness that Neuroticism moderated the steepness of the separate posi- oftheV-shape)inthosemodelswasCanada(B5.32,SE5.06, tive and negative valence–arousal slopes (B5–.06, SE5.05, p<.001),Spain(B5.27,SE5.11,p5.013),Japan(B5.25, p5.283;B5.10,SE5.07,p5.189). SE5.06, p<.001), and Korea (B5.18, SE5.06, p 5 .004). Agreeableness,OpennesstoExperience,andConscientious- For the data from China/Hong Kong, independence between ness (country-centered) were not related to the intercept, V- valenceandarousal(Model1)fitthedatabest. shape,orseparateslopesinsimilarmodels(allps>.10). Wenextfitamultipleregressionmodelassumingasymmet- Inacombinedmodelinwhichallfivepersonalitydimensions ricV-shapeonalldatacombined,allowingforseparateintercept wereincludedininteractionwiththeinterceptandslopeofthe and slope values per country. As seen in Figure 3, Canada, V-shape,onlyNeuroticismsignificantlypredictedtheintercept, Spain,andJapanwerecharacterizedbyasteeperV-shapedrela- with higher Neuroticism being related to a higher level of tionship,whereastheV-shapewasweakerinKoreaandalmost arousal at neutral valence (B5.21, SE5.10, p5.033), and absentinChina/HongKong.Theinteractionbetweenslopeand Extraversion remained the only predictor of the slope, with country was significant, F(4)53.58, p5.006, indicating that higher Extraversion being related to a steeper slope of the V- the country differences in steepness of the V-shape were reli- shape(B5.12,SE5.06,p5.036;allotherps>.05).Finally, able. A comparison of the BIC of a joint model with inacombinedmodelinwhichallpersonalitydimensionswere (BIC56710)andwithout(BIC56694)thisinteractionfavored entered as predictors of the intercept and separate positive and amodelwithouttheinteraction,however. negative slopes, the only significant moderation that emerged Finally,weexaminedhowtheshapeoftheV-shapeisafunc- was Extraversion being positively related to the positive tionofpersonalityacrossdatacombinedfromallcultures.The valence–arousalslope(B5.15,SE5.07,p5.027). results from Study 1 provide the basisfor a clear predictionin thisrespect.AsExtraversionpredictedhigherarousalinthecase of both positive and negative valence, we expected that the Discussion steepness of the V-shape would be predicted by Extraversion. Totestthishypothesis,weestimatedaregressionmodelacross The findings from this study again replicated the previously all data predicting arousal from valence in which country- documentedrelationbetweenvalenceandarousal,whileatthe centered Extraversion interacted with the intercept (level of sametimedemonstratingthatculturalandindividualvariability arousalatneutralvalence)andabsolutevalueoftheslope(steep- in that relationship are predictable and therefore meaningful. ness of the V-shape).5 The results indeed showed that higher Specifically, all but one country showed a V-shaped relation scores for Extraversion were related to a more steep V-shape between valence and arousal, with the exception being China/ (slope;(B5.12,SE5.05,p5.016;Extraversiondidnotrelate Hong Kong, in which valence and arousal were independent. totheintercept,however,B5.01,SE5.10,p5.943).Inaddi- Moreover,thesteepnessoftheV-shapevariedbycountry.The tion, we also examined how country-centered Extraversion V-shapewassteepestinCanada,thenSpain,thenJapan,moder- moderatedthesteepnessoftheslopeseparatelyforpositiveand ateinKorea,andalmostabsent(i.e.,flat)inChina/HongKong. negative valence (to examine whether Extraversion indeed Thesefindingsattesttothemalleabilityofthestructureofaffect relatestoasteeperV-shapeoverall,orperhapsparticularlytoa (seealso,e.g.,Feldman,1995;Kuppens,2008). steeper slope for positive or negative valence). The result Inaddition,theystronglyresonatewiththedocumentedpref- showed that Extraversion only positively moderated the steep- erence for high arousal positive affect in Western cultures and ness of the positive valence–arousal slope (B5.13, SE5.06, lower arousal positive affect in Eastern cultures (Tsai et al., Relation of Valence to Arousal 539 2006). Indeed, Figure 3 and the accompanying reported V- Extraversionhasbeenassociatedwiththehabitualexperienceof shape slopes illustrate that the five samples can be ordered in positivehigharousalaffectivestates,butnotmuchwithpositive termsoftheirarousallevelsatbothhighpositiveandhighnega- low arousal or negative states (Yik, 2009; Yik, Russell, & tivevalenceinlinewithanEast-Westculturaldistinction.This Steiger,2011).Assuch,Extraversioncanberegardedasagen- indicates that differences in ideal affect extend to experienced eral arousal-infusing factor for positive feelings. The approach affect(see also Tsai et al.,2006),and italsodemonstrates that motivationandrewardsensitivitythatisconsideredtounderlie cultural differences in preference for higher or lower levels of Extraversion (e.g., Smillie, 2013) may be the driving force arousal extend to negative affect. The largest exception to this behindthelargersurgeinarousalinreactiontopositiveevents. wasJapan,however,whichwascharacterizedbylowerlevelsof Otherpersonalitydimensionswerealsorelatedtomorespe- arousalatneutralvalence,butdidshowcasearelativelystronger cific aspects of idiographic variation in the structure of affect. V-shapecompared to the other Eastern countries (although we Neuroticism was generally related to higher levels of arousal, note thattheaverage arousal level atneutralvalencewas quite particularly of negative affect. Neuroticism has indeed been lowinJapan).Apossibleexplanationforthismaybedifferences associated with negative emotionality in numerous studies, in scale use when reporting emotions between cultures. Also, although it must be said that this dimension is considered to the data from Hong Kong, while displaying a flat relation makeindividualspronetoexperiencingbothhigharousalnega- between valence and arousal, were characterized by relatively tivestates(e.g.,stress,anxiety)aswellaslowarousalnegative states (e.g., sadness, depression). This duality is perhaps also high arousal levels overall. Especially at neutral valence, the reflectedinthefindingsfromStudy1that,ontheonehand,indi- arousallevelofHongKongparticipantsexceededthatofpartici- viduals high in Neuroticism tend to feel more negative when pants from other nations. More research is needed to pinpoint experiencinglowarousal(correspondingtosadnessanddepres- theexactinterpretationofthesefindings,however. sion), while at the same time also reporting increased arousal Inaddition,thecurrentresultsallowedustofine-tuneinsights when feeling negative (corresponding to stress and anxiety). from the first study in that Extraversion was found to be the Thecombinationofthesefindingsmayhelponetounderstand most consistent independent personality predictor of the theroleofNeuroticismin,forinstance,thereportedhighcomor- arousal–valence relationship. Specifically, the findings showed bidity between depression and anxiety (Barlow et al., 2014; that Extraversionisrelated to a steeper V-shapeinthe relation Mineka,Watson,&Clark,1998). between valence and arousal, which is particularly driven by In terms of culture, the first important aspect to highlight is Extraversion being related to increasing positive valence with thatdespitetheobservedculturalvariation,theV-shapebetween increasingarousal. valence and arousal still remains the dominant pattern across Anotablelimitationofthisstudy,however,isthatitisdiffi- cultures, albeit to varying degrees, ranging from positive to culttoruleoutthepossibilitythatpersonalityand/orculturemay totally flat.Our data didnot indicatea culture inwhich the V- partlyhavedriventhespecificeventspeoplerecallwhenasked shape was reversed, or made thoroughly asymmetric, as could to report on a particularly memorable event from the previous perhaps be expected on the basis of research on ideal affect day (cf. Robinson & Clore, 2002), and that this may partly (which would predict decreasing arousal with positive valence accountforthefindings.Whilethiswouldreflectadifferentway inEasterncultures).The observeddifferences moreor lessdid personalityorculturemaybiaspeopleaffectively,experimental align,however,withthedistinctionbetweenWestern/independ- methods are needed to dissociate this possibility from the entversusEastern/interdependentculturesthatareconsideredto hypothesis that personality and culture predispose people to lieatthebaseofdifferencesinidealaffect.Also,whileexperi- respondaffectivelytoeventsinparticularways. encedandidealaffectfolloweachother’scourse,theyarecer- tainly not identical. One way to interpret thisset of findingsis that although experienced affect does not coincide with ideal GENERALDISCUSSION affect,culturalpreferencesforhighorlowarousalstatesseemto The present studiesreplicatedKuppens andcolleagues’(2013) pull at the overall nomothetic V-shape in one direction or findings, in six new samples in both Western and Eastern cul- another,creatingculturaldifferencesinhowvalenceandarousal tures, regarding an overall nomothetic V-shaped association relatetooneanother.Anadditionalfindingwasthatthecultural betweenvalenceandarousal,witharousalincreasingaspositive ordering in experienced arousal levels at higher levels of posi- ornegativevalenceincreases.Yet,atthesametime,thecurrent tive and negative valence was similar, suggesting that habitual studies highlight significant and meaningful variation in this experience of high versus low arousal feeling states generalize association across individuals and cultures. One of the main acrossvalence. objectivesofthetworeportedstudieswastoidentifycorrelates Combining the findings from both studies, it is striking to ofthesedifferences. note that cultural differences in Extraversion (e.g., McCrae, Intermsofpersonality,themostconsistentfindingwasthat 2004; McCrae & Terracciano, 2005) seem to align with the Extraversion was associated with how much arousal increases observed cultural differences in the V-shaped relation between with increasing valence, particularly positive valence, under- valence and arousal, which showed a steeper slope in Western scoringthecrucialroleofExtraversioninemotionalexperience. comparedtoEasternsamples.Thisobservationtiesthefindings
Description: