ebook img

The Phonetics of Fingerspelling PDF

114 Pages·1992·24.052 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview The Phonetics of Fingerspelling

THE PHONETICS OF FINGERSPELLING STUDIES IN SPEECH PATHOLOGY AND CLINICAL LINGUISTICS SERIES EDITORS Martin J. Ball Raymond D. Kent Dept. of Communication Dept. of Communicative Univ. of Ulster at Jordanstown Disorders Shore Road University of Wisconsin Newtownabbey BT37 OQB 1975 Willow Drive UK - Northern Ireland Madison Wisconsin 53706, USA EDITORIAL BOARD Christopher Code (University of Sydney, Australia) Alvirda Farmer (San Jose State University, USA) John H.V. Gilbert (University of British Columbia, Can.) Yvan Lebrun (Vrije Universiteit, Belgium) Lise Menn (University of Colorado at Boulder, USA) AIMS AND SCOPE The establishment of this series reflects the growth of both interest and research into disorders of speech and language. It is intended that the series will provide a plat form for the development of academic debate and enquiry into the related fields of speech pathology and clinical linguistics. To this end, the series will publish book length studies or collections of papers on aspects of disordered communication, and the relation between language theory and language pathology. Volume 4 Sherman Wilcox The Phonetics of Fingerspelling THE PHONETICS OF FINGERSPELLING SHERMAN WILCOX University of New Mexico JOHN BENJAMINS PUBLISHING COMPANY AMSTERDAM/PHILADELPHIA 1992 Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Wilcox, Sherman. The phonetics of fingerspelling / Sherman Wilcox. p. cm. -- (Studies in speech pathology and clinical linguistics, ISSN 0927-1813; v. 4) Includes bibliographical references and index. 1. Finger spelling. 2. Grammar, General and comparative-Phonetics. I. Title. II. Series. HV2477.W55 1992 419--dc20 92-8926 ISBN 90 272 4334 4 (Eur.)/l-55619-390-4 (US)(alk. paper) CIP ® Copyright 1992 - John Benjamins B.V. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by print, photoprint, microfilm, or any other means, without written permission from the publisher. John Benjamins Publishing Co. · P.O. Box 75577 · 1070 AN Amsterdam · The Netherlands John Benjamins North America · 821 Bethlehem Pike · Philadelphia, PA 19118 · USA TABLE OF CONTENTS Chapter 1: A Linguistic Approach to Fingerspelling 1 The Structure of Signed Languages 1 Signs as Unsegmentable Wholes 2 Signs as Simultaneously Segmentable 3 Signs as Sequentially Segmentable 4 Fingerspelling 9 Lexical Borrowing 13 Research on Fingerspelling 14 Fingerspelling as a Research Tool 15 Models of Fingerspelling 16 The Production and Perception of Fingerspelling 19 Learning to Fingerspell 21 Fingerspelling Fluency 23 Coarticulation in Fingerspelling 24 Kinematics, Dynamics, and Articulatory Movements 25 Organization of the Studies 27 Learning to Fingerspell 27 The Phonetics of Fingerspelling 27 Chapter 2: Learning to Fingerspell 29 Adult Acquisition of Fingerspelling 29 Method 31 Subjects 31 Materials 31 Procedures 31 Results 31 Chapter 3: The Measurement of Fingerspelling Movement 37 Background 37 Hardware 37 Software 39 Sample Data Analysis 43 Methods and Procedures for Motion Analysis Studies 48 Subjects 48 Stimulus Materials 48 Room and Camera Arrangement 49 vi PHONETICS OF FINGERSPELLING Marker Placement and Attachment 52 Sampling Times and Rates 52 Calibration 53 Chapter 4: Targets and Transitions 55 Models Revisited 55 The Salience of Targets and Transitions 59 Dynamic Modeling of Phonetic Structure 62 Chapter 5: Towards a Dynamics of Fingerspelling 65 From Kinematics to Dynamics 65 Fingerspelling Speed 65 Cooperativity in Fingerspelling Production 69 Discussion 74 Chapter 6: Future Directions in Signed Language Research 79 The Linguistic Study of Fingerspelling 79 Limitations and Suggestions for Further Study 81 Motion Analysis of Signed Language 81 Learning to Fingerspell 82 Models of Fingerspelling 83 Sign Science and Speech Science: The Search for Unity 85 Speech as Gestures 86 Signing as Gestures 88 Is Speech Special? 90 Notes 93 References 97 Index 107 Chapter 1 A Linguistic Approach to Fingerspelling The Structure of Signed Languages "A language is a set of principles relating meanings and phonetic sequences" (Lan- gacker, 1972:1). One type of phonetic medium is sound; one way of conveying lan guage is through speech. Until recently, however, most linguists and researchers in the language sciences would have gone beyond the characterization of sound as a possible phonetic medium and would have insisted that more than being "merely one way of conveying language, the sounds of speech are, instead, its common and privileged carriers" (Liberman, Cooper, Shankweiler, and Studdert-Kennedy, 1967:431). It is certainly true that in approaching the study of language, the data most readily at hand come from speech. Spoken languages are all around us; sounds are clearly a common way of representing meanings. But are they privileged? We must be careful about making claims concerning language in general on the basis of a par ticular language. If we examined only English, for example, we might conclude that relatively strict word order is a property of all languages. Cross-linguistic data are necessary in order to get a full picture of the structure of human language. Just as important are cross-modal data. Linguists now recognize another medi um through which languages can be conveyed. Numerous natural languages exist which are represented in the visual-gestural modality. Research has established that these signed languages1 are fully independent of spoken languages. The most exten sive research to date has examined American Sign Language (ASL), the signed lan guage used by deaf people in America (Bellugi and Studdert-Kennedy, 1980; Fischer & Siple, 1990; Grosjean and Lane, 1980; Klima and Bellugi, 1979; Siple, 1978; Wilbur, 1987). 2 PHONETICS OF FINGERSPELLING For many years, signed languages were thought to be dependent on spoken lan guages. In particular, ASL was considered to be merely a derivative of English. One explanation for this misconception can be found in the distinction between languag es and modalities (Table 1). Table 1: The Language/Mode Distinction Language Mode English Speech (Spoken Language) Spanish Cochiti French Writing (Written Language) American Sign Language French Sign Language Signing (Signed Language) British Sign Language Speech is the primary means of representation for most natural languages. Written languages are usually characterized as secondary systems used to represent spoken languages in a written form, for particular purposes such as overcoming the temporal or distance limitations of speech (Gelb, 1963). Table 1 can be used to explore the relationships among languages and their modes of representation. English, Spanish, and French, for example, are both spoken and written. Cochiti, like many of the world's languages, has no conventional written form. American Sign Language has no conventional spoken or written form; its only means of expression is through the signed modality.2 Since speech — spoken language — was for so many years viewed as primary and the written modality as secondary, it was perhaps natural for linguists to suppose that the signed modality was likewise used as only a secondary medium for the rep resentation of spoken languages. What must be recognized is that the signed modal ity can serve as the primary medium for a set of natural languages. Signs as Unsegmentable Wholes As for our understanding of the phonological structure of signed languages, the com mon belief for many years was that the words of signed languages, unlike the words LINGUISTIC APPROACH 3 of spoken languages, had no internal structure. Signs (words) were regarded as unan- alyzable wholes. Thus, a word such as LIKE (Figure 1) was regarded as a wholistic item, an unanalyzable gesture, with no internal structure. Figure 1. The ASL Word LIKE Signs as Simultaneously Segmentahle It was not until the publication of the classic monograph, "Sign Language Structure" (Stokoe, 1960), that this conception was refuted. Stokoe noted that signs, like words, do have an internal structure by demonstrating that signs must be described in terms of three parameters: dez (hand configuration), tab (location), and sig (movement). Stokoe originally coined the term chereme for these sublexical units, but later re searchers pointed out that what was being described was the phonological structure of ASL and preferred to call them phonemes. Working within Stokoe's framework, researchers began in earnest to study the phonology of signed languages (Battison, 1973,1974; Battison, Markowicz, and Woodward, 1975; Friedman, 1976; McIntire, 1977;Wilbur, 1985). As an example of how Stokoe's three parameters can be used to describe the phonology of ASL, it is instructive to consider minimal pairs. Varying only the hand- shape parameter, it is possible to sign the minimal pair ONION and CHINESE. Varying only the location parameter, we can sign the minimal pair CHINESE and BORING. Varying only the movement parameter yields the minimal pair NAME and SHORT. A fourth parameter, orientation, was suggested by Battison (1973; Bat tison, Markowicz, and Woodward, 1975). Varying only the orientation parameter yields the minimal pair TRAIN and SHORT. As research proceeded under this view of sign structure, linguists began to note that the structure of signed and spoken words differed in one remarkable way. Unlike 4 PHONETICS OF FINGERSPELLING spoken words, in which phonemes are arranged sequentially, the phonemes of signs are simultaneously present. As Stokoe noted (1960:37): The sign morpheme, however, unlike the morpheme or word of a spo ken language, is seen as simultaneously, not sequentially, produced. Analy sis of the sign morpheme then cannot be segmentation in time but must be aspectual. The different sublexical structure of signs and words — simultaneous versus se quential — was taken as evidence for how languages are differentially structured by their modality (Bellugi and Studdert-Kennedy, 1980). Klima and Bellugi (1979:39) succinctly present the argument: Thus the lexical items of ASL and all other primary sign languages we know of appear to be constituted in a different way from those of spoken languages: the organization of signs is primarily simultaneous rather than sequential. ASL uses a spatial medium; and this may crucially influence its organization. Signs as Sequentially Segmentahle Recent phonological research on American Sign Language is suggesting that signs are not only analyzable simultaneously but also sequentially (Liddell, 1984a, 1984b, 1990; Liddell and Johnson, 1986, 1989; Padden & Perlmutter, 1987; Perlmutter, 1990; Sandler, 1986, 1989, 1990; Wilbur, 1990). Liddell, for example, argues that signs can be sequentially analyzed into Movements (M) and Holds (H). Under this analysis, the sign LIKE consists of three segments (HMH), as in Figure 2. The initial H segment is defined by several features; for the sake of simplicity, only a few are depicted in Figure 2 and only the handshape parameter is given values. In Liddell's analysis, autosegmental feature spreading would account for the M-seg- ment features. LiddelTs model of ASL phonology has been developed in great detail. Present ing evidence from word formation and compounds, and drawing on work in spoken language autosegmental phonology, he convincingly demonstrates that the phono logical structure of signs is more similar to that of spoken words than was previously thought. Sandler (1986) has proposed an alternative autosegmental model of ASL pho nology. In her model, the opposition is not between movements and holds but be tween movements and locations, with handshape on a separate autosegmental tier;

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.