ebook img

The Pathologic Physiology of Dementia: With Indications for Diagnosis and Treatment PDF

163 Pages·1978·4.987 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview The Pathologic Physiology of Dementia: With Indications for Diagnosis and Treatment

Monographien aus dem Gesamtgebiete der Psychiatrie 20 Psychiatry Series Herausgegeben von H. Hippius, Munchen . W. Janzarik, Heidelberg C. Muller, Prilly-Lausanne Richard M. Torack The Pathologic Physiology of Dementia With Indications for Diagnosis and Treatment Springer -Verlag Berlin Heidelberg New York 1978 Professor Dr. RICHARD M. TORACK, Washington University, School of Medicine, Department of Pathology, 660 South Euclid Avenue, St. Louis, MO 63110/USA ISBN-13: 978-3-642-88439-9 e-ISBN-13: 978-3-642-88437-5 DOl: 10.1007/978-3-642-88437-5 Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication Data. Torack. Richard M .• 1927 -. The patholo gic physiology of dementia. with indications for diagnosis and treatment. (Monographien aus dem Gesamtgebiete der Psychiatrie; v. 20). Bibliography: p. Includes index. I. Dementia. I. Title. II. Series. RC386.2.T67. 616.8'9. 78-15102. This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved, whether the whole or part of the mate rial is concerned, specifically those of translation, reprinting. re-use of illustrations, broad casting, reproduction by photocopying machine or similar means, and storage in data banks. Under § 54 of the German Copyright Law, where copies are made for other than private use, a fee is payable to the publisher, the amount of the fee to be determined by a~reement with the publisher. © by Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 1978. Softcover reprint of the hardcover 1st edition 1978 The use of registered names, trademarks, etc. in this publication does not imply. even in the ab sence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use. 2123/3130-543210 Contents 1. Historical Overview of Dementia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .... Prelude to the Modem Era . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 Age of Alzheimer and Kraepelin. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 5 History of Alzheimer's Disease. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 12 References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 15 2. Clinical Manifestations as a Determinant of Dementia. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 17 General Considerations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 17 Functional Mental Syndromes as a Manifestation of Brain Disease . . . . . . .. 19 Acute Confusion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 23 Specialized Syndromes in Dementia . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 26 Creutzfeldt-lakob Disease. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 26 Kluver-Bucy Syndrome (Limbic Encephalitis) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 28 Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 32 Summary of Specialized Syndromes . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 34 Summary of Clinical Manifestations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 34 References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 35 3. Diagnostic Tests of Dementia as Research Tools. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 39 Psychological Tests of Intelligence . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 39 Electroencephalography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 43 Radiographic Technique. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 46 Morphologic and Chemical Assay of Brain Tissue. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 47 References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 51 4. Treatment of Senile Dementia. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 56 Acute Confusion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 57 Circulatory Agents. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 57 Metabolic Stimulants. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 58 Psychoactive Drug Therapy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 59 Antiviral Therapy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 59 Surgical Management. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 60 Shunt Therapy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 60 Carotid Surgery. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 60 Conclusions. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 61 References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 61 VI 5. Epidemiology of Dementia. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 64 General Considerations. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 64 Previous Surveys of Senile Dementia. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 65 Cross-Sectional Survey . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 65 Larsson Survey . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 68 Autopsy Studies. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 68 The GM Survey. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 70 Characteristics of the Population . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 70 Methods. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 70 Diagnosis of Dementia. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 70 Results. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 71 Etiologic Implications of Epidemiology. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 73 Aging. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 73 Heredity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 73 Slow Virus Infection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 74 Concluding Remarks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 75 References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 75 6. Current Evaluation of Pathologic Correlates of Dementia . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 77 Congophilic Angiopathy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 77 Senile Plaques. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 85 Endogenous Plaque Formation. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 87 Exogenous Origin From Circulating Immunoproteins. . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 88 Neurofibrillary Tangles. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 88 Creu tzfeld t -J akob Disease. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ., 91 Other Pathologic Correlates. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 93 Aluminum. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 93 Lipofuscin. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 94 Granulovacuolar Degeneration. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 95 Pathogenetic Implications of Pathology. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 95 References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 95 7. Arteriosclerotic Dementia. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 101 Dementia Following Multiple Strokes .......................... , 102 Little Strokes .......................................... , 104 Diffuse Cerebral Ischemia. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 105 Ischemia Due to Macrovascular Disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 105 Carotid Artery Occlusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 105 Vertebrobasilarinsufficiency. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 105 Ischemia Due to Microvascular Disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 106 Cerebral Blood Flow Study of Dementia ........................ , 106 Summary. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 108 References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 108 VII 8. Critical Evaluation of the Concept of Mental Aging . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 110 Functional Aspects of the Elderly Brain. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 112 Mental Behavior in the Aged . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 112 Memory ........................................... 112 Cognitive Function . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 113 Psychoses After 65 . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . ., 113 Electroencephalography . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 114 Routine EEG . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 114 Visual Evoked Response (VER) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 115 Cerebral Blood Flow . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 116 Immunologic Indices of Aging. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 116 Pathology of the Aged Brain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 118 Brain Weight as a Determinant of Atrophy. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 118 Neuron Loss With Aging. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 120 Senile Plaques, Neurofibrillary Tangles, Granulovacuolar Degeneration . .. 121 Lipofuscin. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 122 Abnormalities of the Peripheral Neuron. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 123 Epidemiology of Mental Aging. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 124 Cross-Sectional Survey of Effectiveness. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 124 Longitudinal Survey of Aging . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 127 Summary of Longitudinal Surveys . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 128 Psychological Test of In telligence. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 128 Psychometrics as Determinants of Longevity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 129 Biologic Factors of Longevity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 130 Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 130 Summary of Mental Aging. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 131 References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 132 9. Nature and Cause of Alzheimer's Disease: The Case for Slow Virus Infection. 138 Nature of AD. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 138 Clinical Indicators. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 138 Diagnostic Criteria. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 139 Epidemiology. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 140 Pathologic Data. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 140 Etiologic Potential for AD. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 142 Metabolic Disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 142 Autoimmune Disease. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 142 Metallic In toxication . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 143 Slow Virus Infection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 145 Concept of Slow Virus Infection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 145 Comparison of AD and Slow Virus Infection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 146 References. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 148 Subject Index. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . .. 151 1. Historical Overview of Dementia The evolution of senile dementia has traditionally been considered to represent an aspect of senescence which, in turn, is the normal final phase of human performance that occurs as a prelude to death. Yet t}1ere has always been vast disagreement regarding the mean ing of this statement. Let us begin with senescence itself (Tibbitts, 1957): The character of elderly men - men who are past their prime may be said to be formed for the most part of elements that are the contrary of all those in youth. They have lived many years; they have often been taken in, and often made mistakes; and life on the whole is a bad business. (Aristotle, 384-322 B.C.). Aristotle's negataive opinion is exactly opposite that of his mentor Plato as he pre sents his plan for gerontocracy (Tibbitts, 1957): And he who at every age, as boy and youth and in mature life, has come out of the trial victo rious and pure, shall be appointed a ruler and guardian of the State; he shall be honored in life and death. There can be no doubt that the elder must rule to younger. (Plato, 427 -34 7 B. C.). A very astute appraisal of aging was that of Cicero in his famous essay "De Senectute" (Couch,1959): The blame seems to me misdirected. If the faults are due to old age, the same misfortunes would have befallen me and other men, but I have known many men who have no complaints. (Cicero, 106-43 B.C.). The controversy regarding aging inevitably transfer to that condition we call senile dementia, since as senility, it personifies the ultimate force of aging. There have always been those who advocate the concept that senility, like senescence, is that bad product of brain aging and decay to which all of us will succumb if we live long enough. Their adversaries argue that this idea is untenable because there is no age by which everyone has been shown to become senile. The complexity of this problem is evidenced by the fact that this issue has not been resolved despite the efforts of modern medical science. The history of dementia affords an opportunity to understand how the present situation evolved. There is another benefit that accrues from a study of history, one which was described very well by Harry Truman in his memoirs (Truman, 1956): I learned of the unique problems of Andrew Johnson, whose destiny it was to be thrust suddenly into the presidency to fill the shoes of one of history's great leaders. When the same thing happened to me, I knew just how Johnson had coped with his problems, and I did not make the mistakes he made. 2 Prelude to the Modern Era One initial impact of mental problems like dementia was the result of the fact that chan ges in the brain accompanied the disorder. This indicated a relationship between mind and brain, which certainly was appreciated by Hippocrates, who discussed the nature of consciousness in his book, Sacred Disease (Jones, 1923): Wherefore I assert that the brain is the interpreter of consciousness. Some people say that the heart is the organ with which we think, and that it feels pain and anxiety. But is not so ... Men ought to know that from the brain, and from the brain only, arise our pleasures, joys, laughter and jests, as well as our sorrows, pains, griefs and tears. Through it, in particular, we think, see, hear, and dis tinguish the ugly from the beautiful, the bad from the good, the pleasant from the unpleasant ... It is the same thing which makes us mad or delirious, inspires us with dread and fear, whether by night or by day, brings sleeplessness, inopportune mistakes, aimless anxieties, absentmindedness, and acts that are contrary to habit. These things that we suffer all come from the brain, when it is not healthy. Hippocrates seems to be concerned with the notion that the heart, rather than the brain, is the organ of the mind, so we can assume that many people believed this to be true. His concept of mind and brain was perpetuated by Galen, who not only affirmed this view, but added a belief that dementia was due to a "coldness in the brain". How ever, the collapse of Rome and Greco-Roman culture ushered in an era of intellectual confusion in which the fears of Hippocrates were confirmed. The mind moved from the brain to the heart. Presumably, the nature of this heroic age required an association between consciousness, strength, and heart. At any rate, this alien habitat seems like a good explanation for the intellectual confusion. Our heritage from this era is best mani fest in the rich endowment of our vocabulary linking heart with all those things that Hippocrates talked about: hearty, heartfelt, hartless, heart strings, heartache, etc. The whole idea culminates in the sentiment expressed on Valentines Day. For those who only believe the dictionary, one current definition of heart is: "the vital center of one's being, emotions and sensibilities; the seat or repository of emotions." Hippocrates and Galen were resurrected during the renaissance along with Socrates and Plato and, at this time, the relationship between mind and brain was renewed. Although renaissance man adopted the classics, he also fel t compelled to enlarge and improve this culture by his own hunger for knowledge. Among other things, brains were studied avidly and one of the points of greatest interest was the ventricular system with its fluid content. This is a direct outgrowth of the concern with body humors in the medical classics. This interest expanded when Harvey published his theory on the circula tion of blood in 1628. The circulation of a fluid peculiar to the brain was considered by some to have comparable importance for mental performance. Some astonishing ideas of brain function were formulated and those of Rene Descartes achived greatest notoriety. In Descartes, we find that renaissance ideal of "universality" expressed in a complete ness comparable to Da Vinci. Primarily a mathematician, he produced essays on music, on the development of the fetus, and on legal doctrine. Late in life, he became obsessed with the function of the mind; his "Je pense donc je suis" is still a milestone in the history of philosophy. He was also engrossed in the topic of matter and mind (or soul). Complicating his science was a strong religious conviction, which embodied God as an infmite source, a fountainhead. He conceived of the brain as another spiritual system, needing a similar initiator of all activity. He ascribed this power to the pineal gland which has been ridiculed ever since Descartes' folly (Haldane and Ross, 1911): 3 It is, "the principal seat of the soul, and the place where all its thoughts occur." As the "animal spirits, which are like a tenuous wind, or rather like a pure or vital flame", pass along "the passage from the anterior to the posterior cavities of the brain", it takes advantage of its strategic position (it is "suspended above the passage") to exert itself upon them, and is reciprocally affected by the changes they undergo." (Descartes, 1596-1650.) Within a short time, Gall and those who followed him showed that the neocortex was the seat of consciousness and any similar function of the pea-sized pineal was considered absurd. However within the past decade, we have learned that the pineal gland synthe sizes powerful neurohormones and that these "animal spirits" do move through the third ventricle to affect the pituitary gland and probably vital nuclei in the hypothalamus and medulla as well. And Descartes did it all with deductive reasoning and lyric prose. The first investigator to localize consciousness to the cortex was Joseph Gall (Acker knecht, 1958). By 1810, he had formalized a new concept of brain function that was based mainly on 12 proofs he derived from comparative anatomy of the brain. His initial conclusions were fourfold: 1. Faculties are innate. 2. Faculties depend on organic structures. 3. The brain is the organ of all faculties, tendencies, and feelings. 4. The brain is composed of as many organs as the individual has faculties, tendencies, and feelings. Gall observed that the development of these faculties paralleled the evolution of the neocortex, so he localized these organs in the cortex but, while his anatomy was revolu tionary, his physiology was his downfall. He believed whenever a certain faculty like memory was highly developed, an appropriate part of the cortex was enlarged. This in turn led to certain protuberances of the skull. Finally he conceived that he could palpate and influence these organs through an intact skull. This was the beginning of phrenology and the ending of science. The real impact of evolution occurred in the last half of the nineteenth century, in large measure this was inspired by Darwin. At this time the neocortex was precisely defined anatomically and Sherrington was the unquestioned leader in localizing different levels of brain function. However, his methodology was not well suited for a study of consciousness; this remained a task for Hughlings Jackson, using human disease as the experimental model. Jackson related consciousness to his highest centers, which he iden tified in various parts of the cortex but in which he recognizes several levels (Taylor, 1931). In epilepsy, he says: There are three degrees of negative affection of consciousness; in ciinicallanguage they are mental confusion, insensibility and coma. His conception of insanity was even more important for he believed this to be the result of a dissolution of the various levels of consciousness: In every insanity, with one obvious exception (complete dementia), there is a double symptomatic condition, a condition of two opposite mental elements, one negative and one positive (or superposi tive). Repeating what has just been said, there is in every insanity (1) negatively, defect of conscious ness (loss of some consciousness) and there is (2) the consciousness remaining. In complete dementia, the situation becomes quite different: Here the negative mental affection is greatest, is indeed total; there is dementia. There are no positive symptoms; there is no mind or consciousness. There is complete dissolution, and thus no 4 lower range of evolution remains. Here is what I called "the obvious exception" to the statement that the mental condition in insanities is a double condition, one of a negative and a positive element. In this depth there is no person but only a living creature. (Hughlings Jackson, 1834-1911.) Following the stimulus of Jackson's ideas, insanity was recognized as brain disease and not the result of an indwelling evil spirit, or a just tribute exacted by an intemperate and dissolute life. This was an abnormality of the neocortex and the primary brain change was cortical atrophy. During this same interval the clinical manifestations of dementia were being re-evalu ated and reclassified. Prior to this time mental illness was distinguished on the basis of activity (mania, melancholia) and on the time of life (adolescence, menopause, senium, etc.). Now Maudsley (1868) and Clouston (1874) in England, Charcot (1868) in France, and Kraepelin (1883) in Germany were furiously characterizing the various forms of insanity. Probably the best example of this new psychiatry is the evolution of dementia praecox (schizophrenia), which occurred in the eight editions of Kraepelin's textbook Psychiatrie that appeared between 1883 and 1910. A major advance occurred when it became clear that only certain forms of insanity were associated with cortical atrophy. These chiefly occurred after age 65 and, because this was believed to be due to brain aging, the entity was called senile dementia. Conver sely most of the insanities occurring at an earlier age had no change in the brain organ. We must appreciate that although the new entities were considered to represent abnor mal brain function, no one had any idea of the basis of the problem or of a rational form of treatment. Indeed the situation had been unchanged since Pinel took the chains off the mental patients in the Bicetre in 1793. The insanity related to degeneration of the brain was chronic, progressive, and fatal. By 1868 Maudsley was able to characterize several categories of chronic insanity: They represent in undistinguishable varieties the shattered wrecks of the mental organization. Three main groups of them may perhaps be made. The first will consist of those who exhibit a few striking delusions which seem to be automatically expressed. In a second group of cases there is a more general incoherence or craziness, without any parti cular delusion, but with greater external activity. Lastly, there is a group of demented patients in whom the mind is almost extinguished: who have to be fed, moved, clothed, and cared for; who evince little or no sensibility; whose only utterance is a grunt, a whine, or a cry; and whose only movements are to rub their heads or hands. These chronic progressive disorders were even less treatable than the functional prob lems but some idea of cause was derived from the brain changes. In 1874, Clouston could write about the cause of senile insanity on the basis of 203 cases occurring in sub jects over the age of 60: The three great dangers to normal mental senility are hereditary brain weakness, a diseased vas cular system, and the effects of over exertion or toxic irritations of brain structures either at the time or at former periods of life which have left the convolutions weakened. It is impossible to fix an age at which physiological senility begins, and therefore we cannot fix an age for senile insanity. Premature age in look characterizes many of those who become insane after 4S. By the end of the century, sophistication but no fundamental change in Maudley's concept had occurred. Senile dementia had become a disease entity comprising two forms. Simple senile dementia appeared to be the comparatively benign form, beginning with episodes of confusion that gradually became complicated by loss of memory and

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.