ebook img

The Linguistic Cerebellum PDF

427 Pages·2016·9.929 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview The Linguistic Cerebellum

THE LINGUISTIC CEREBELLUM Edited by PETER MARIËN Department of Neurology and Memory Clinic, ZNA Middelheim General Hospital, Antwerp, Belgium; and Clinical and Experimental Neurolinguistics (CLIN), Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium and MARIO MANTO Unité d’Etude du Mouvement Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium; and Université de Mons, Mons, Belgium Amsterdam • Boston • Heidelberg • London New York • Oxford • Paris • San Diego San Francisco • Singapore • Sydney • Tokyo Academic Press is an imprint of Elsevier Academic Press is an imprint of Elsevier 125 London Wall, London EC2Y 5AS, UK 525 B Street, Suite 1800, San Diego, CA 92101-4495, USA 225 Wyman Street, Waltham, MA 02451, USA The Boulevard, Langford Lane, Kidlington, Oxford OX5 1GB, UK Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher. Details on how to seek permission, further information about the Publisher’s permissions policies and our arrangements with organizations such as the Copyright Clearance Center and the Copyright Licensing Agency, can be found at our website: www.elsevier.com/permissions. This book and the individual contributions contained in it are protected under copyright by the Publisher (other than as may be noted herein). Notices Knowledge and best practice in this field are constantly changing. As new research and experience broaden our understanding, changes in research methods, professional practices, or medical treatment may become necessary. Practitioners and researchers must always rely on their own experience and knowledge in evaluating and using any information, methods, compounds, or experiments described herein. In using such information or methods they should be mindful of their own safety and the safety of others, including parties for whom they have a professional responsibility. To the fullest extent of the law, neither the Publisher nor the authors, contributors, or editors, assume any liability for any injury and/or damage to persons or property as a matter of products liability, negligence or otherwise, or from any use or operation of any methods, products, instructions, or ideas contained in the material herein. ISBN: 978-0-12-801608-4 British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data A catalog record for this book is available from the Library of Congress For information on all Academic Press publications visit our website at http://store.elsevier.com/ Publisher: Nikki Levy Acquisition Editor: Nikki Levy Editorial Project Manager: Barbara Makinster Production Project Manager: Julia Haynes Designer: Matt Limbert Typeset by TNQ Books and Journals www.tnq.co.in Printed and bound in the United States of America CONTRIBUTORS Hermann Ackermann Department of Neurology and Stroke, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany Michael Adamaszek Department of Neurologic and Cognitive Rehabilitation, Bavaria Clinic Kreischa, Kreischa, Germany Louise Allen-Walker School of Psychology, Bangor University, Wales, UK Georgios P.D. Argyropoulos Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuropsychiatry Section, Developmental Neurosciences Programme, Institute of Child Health, University College London, London, UK Lauren A. Barker The Chicago School of Professional Psychology, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA Lisa Bartha-Doering Department of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, Medical University Vienna, Vienna, Austria Alan A. Beaton Department of Psychology, Aberystwyth University, Wales, UK; Department of Psychology, Swansea University, Wales, UK Florian Bodranghien Laboratoire de Neurologie Expérimentale–ULB, Brussels, Belgium R. Martyn Bracewell School of Psychology, Bangor University, Wales, UK; School of Medical Sciences, Bangor University, Wales, UK Hyo-Jung De Smet Clinical and Experimental Neurolinguistics (CLIN), Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium John E. Desmond Departments of Neurology, Neuroscience and Cognitive Science, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA Thora Gudrunardottir Oncological Department, Hilleroed Hospital, Copenhagen, Denmark; Posterior Fossa Society Christophe Habas Service de NeuroImagerie, Hôpital des Quinze-Vingts, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Paris, France Ingo Hertrich Department of Neurology and Stroke, Hertie Institute for Clinical Brain Research, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany xi xii Contributors Laura Jansons Private Practice, Arlington Heights, IL, USA Kenneth C. Kirkby Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Tasmania, Hobart, Australia Leonard F. Koziol Private Practice, Park Ridge, IL, USA Maria Leggio I.R.C.C.S. Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy; Department of Psychology Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy Mario Manto Unité d’Etude du Mouvement, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium; Université de Mons, Mons, Belgium Peter Mariën Department of Neurology and Memory Clinic, ZNA Middelheim General Hospital, Antwerp, Belgium; Clinical and Experimental Neurolinguistics (CLIN), Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium Cherie L. Marvel Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA Klaus Mathiak Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, University Hospital Aachen, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany; Jülich-Aachen Research Alliance, JARA-Brain, Jülich, Germany Marco Molinari I.R.C.C.S. Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy Philippe Paquier Clinical and Experimental Neurolinguistics (CLIN), Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium; Department of Neurology and Neuropsychology, University Hospital Erasme, ULB, Brussels, Belgium; Unit of Translational Neurosciences, School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Universiteit Antwerpen, Antwerp, Belgium Jeremy D. Schmahmann Ataxia Unit, Cognitive Behavioral Neurology Unit, Laboratory for Neuroanatomy and Cerebellar Neurobiology, Department of Neurology, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA Catherine J. Stoodley Department of Psychology and Center for Behavioral Neuroscience, American University, Washington, DC, USA Kim van Dun Clinical and Experimental Neurolinguistics (CLIN), Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium Contributors xiii Dorien Vandenborre Cepos, Rehabilitation Centre, Rooienberg, Duffel, Belgium; Clinical and Experimental Neurolinguistics (CLIN), Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium Jo Verhoeven Language and Communication Science, City University London, Northampton Square, London, UK; Computational Linguistics & Psycholinguistics Research Center (CLiPS), University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium Wolfram Ziegler EKN—Clinical Neuropsychology Research Group, Clinic for Neuropsychology, City Hospital Munich, Munich, Germany; Institute for Phonetics and Speech Processing, Ludwig-Maximilians-University Munich, Munich, Germany INTRODUCTION Peter Mariën1,2, Mario Manto3,4 1Department of Neurology and Memory Clinic, ZNA General Hospital, Middelheim, Antwerp, Belgium; 2Clinical and Experimental Neurolinguistics (CLIN), Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Brussels, Belgium; 3Unité d’Etude du Mouvement, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Brussels, Belgium; 4Université de Mons, Mons, Belgium In less than three decades, the concept of “cerebellar neurocognition” has evolved from a mere afterthought to an entirely new and multifac- eted area of neuroscientific research. A close interplay among three main strands of contemporary neuroscience has induced a substantial modifi- cation of the traditional view of the cerebellum as a simple coordinator of autonomic and somatic motor functions. Indeed, the wealth of cur- rently available evidence derived from (1) detailed neuroanatomical investigations, (2) functional neuroimaging studies with healthy subjects and patients, and (3) in-depth neuropsychological assessment of patients with cerebellar disorders shows that the cerebellum plays also a cardinal role in affective regulation, cognitive processing, and linguistic functions. However, although considerable progress has been made in models of cerebellar function, controversy remains regarding the exact role of the “linguistic cerebellum” in a broad variety of nonmotor language processes. This volume brings together a range of different viewpoints and opin- ions regarding the contribution of the cerebellum to language function. Recent developments and insights in the nonmotor modulatory role of the cerebellum in language and some related disorders are discussed by experts in the field. The role of the cerebellum in speech and language perception, in motor speech planning including apraxia of speech, in ver- bal working memory, in phonological and semantic verbal fluency, in syntax processing, in the dynamics of language production, in reading, and in writing will be addressed. In addition, the functional topography of the linguistic cerebellum and the contribution of the deep nuclei to linguistic function will be discussed. As such, with this volume, we hope to offer a framework for debate and discussion. The reader interested in the neuroscientific mysteries of this organ situated at the bottom of the brain will find not only state-of-the-art contributions, but also novel ideas that are being investigated in a growing number of laboratories worldwide. The fact that the cerebellum contains more neurons than any xv xvi Introduction other region of the brain and is characterized by a geometrical structure makes it an excellent candidate for investigation of novel concepts in neuroscience. DEVELOPMENT OF CONCEPTS Two centuries of research on cerebellar function have been dominated by the role of the cerebellum in motor control (see Manto et al., 2012 for a review). However, from time to time, clinical case descriptions and experi- mental evidence from animal studies dating back to the early part of the nineteenth century, already suggested an association between cerebellar pathology and a variety of nonmotor cognitive as well as affective dysfunc- tions (see Schmahmann, 1991, 1997). As early as 1831, Combettes described in the Bulletins de la Société Anatomique de Paris, the postmortem findings of an 11-year-old girl, Alexandrine Labrosse, who presented with neurode- velopmental disorders including a range of cognitive, affective, and motor impairments resulting from a complete absence of the cerebellum (Figure 1). Nevertheless, a causal connection between cerebellar disease and cogni- tive and affective disturbances was dismissed for decades. In the mid-1900s, investigators started to examine a possible link between the cerebellum and cognition and emotion, exemplified by the work of Snider and Eldred (1948), Snider (1950), Snider and Maiti (1976), Dow (1974), Heath (1977, 1997), Heath, Franklin, and Shraberg (1979), Cooper, Riklan, Amin, and Cullinan (1978) and others (see Schmahmann, 1991 for a review). This laid a foundation for the rediscovery of this concept by Leiner, Leiner, and Dow (1986, 1991), who hypothesized that more recently evolved parts of the cerebellum contribute to learning, cognition, and language, and by Schmahmann (1991) and Schmahmann and Pandya (1987, 1989), who introduced the dysmetria of thought hypothesis (Schmahmann, 1998). These authors provided a historical, clinical, neuroanatomical, and theoreti- cal framework within which a cerebellar role in higher cognitive and affec- tive processes could be considered. That there may be a correlation between the size of the cerebellum and aspects of general intelligence has been known for some time (e.g., Allin et al., 2001; Ciesielski, Harris, Hart, & Pabst, 1997; Mostofsky et al., 1998; Paradiso, Andreasen, O’Leary, Arndt, & Robinson, 1997). From an evolutionary perspective, MacLeod, Zilles, Schleicher, Rilling, and Gibson (2003) demonstrated a reliable linear regres- sion contrast between volumes of whole brain, cerebellum, vermis, and hemisphere of hominoids and monkeys and a striking increase in the lateral cerebellum in hominoids (Beaton & Mariën, 2010). After controlling Introduction xvii statistically for age and sex, Pangelinan et al. (2011) showed with school- aged children that total cerebellar volume correlates significantly with cog- nitive ability (as measured by overall intelligence quotient) (but see Parker et al. (2008) for negative findings). Posthuma et al. (2003) reported that Figure 1 Early description by Combettes (1831) of an 11-year-old girl with a complex of cognitive, affective and motor developmental disturbances due to agenesis of the cerebel- lum. cerebellar volume in healthy adults (as well as total cerebral grey and white matter volumes) correlates with working memory performance. Such find- ings make it difficult to deny that the cerebellum is “an organ of cognition” (Justus & Ivry, 2001). xviii Introduction SCHMAHMANN SYNDROME Only a few years after the introduction of the dysmetria of thought theory, Schmahmann and Sherman (1998) described in a seminal study of patients with focal cerebellar lesions a consistent pattern of cognitive and affective deficits and coined the term “cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome” to describe this condition. Schmahmann syndrome, the eponym of cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome (Manto & Mariën, 2015), was characterized as a cluster of multimodal disturbances including: (1) executive deficits (deficient planning, set-shifting, abstract reasoning, working memory, and decreased ver- bal fluency), (2) disruption of visuospatial cognition (visuospatial disorganiza- tion and impaired visuospatial memory), (3) personality changes (flattening or blunting of affect, and disinhibited or inappropriate behavior), and (4) a range of linguistic impairments among which were dysprosodia, agrammatism, and mild anomia. However, analysis of the clinical data revealed that not all deficits occurred in each patient, but that certain symptoms were particularly promi- nent. Decreased verbal fluency, which did not relate to dysarthria, was said to be present in 18 of the 20 patients. Visuospatial disintegration, mainly consist- ing of disruption of the sequential approach to drawing and conceptualization of figures was found in 19 cases. Eighteen of the 20 patients presented with executive dysfunctions involving working memory, motor, and mental set- shifting and perseverations of actions and drawing. In 15 patients, frontal-like behavioral and affective changes were evident. Flattening of affect or disinhi- bition occurred, taking the form of overfamiliarity, flamboyant and impulsive actions, and humorous but inappropriate comments. Behavior was character- ized as regressive and child-like in some cases and obsessive–compulsive traits were occasionally observed. Deficits in mental arithmetic were evident in 14 patients. Visual confrontation naming was impaired in 13 patients. Eight patients developed abnormal prosody characterized by high-pitched, whin- ing, and a hypophonic speech quality. Mnestic deficits (verbal and visual learn- ing and recall) were observed in some cases. The cluster of symptoms defining Schmahmann syndrome was associated with a decrease of general intellectual capacity. From an anatomoclinical perspective, cognitive and affective impair- ments were more prominent and generalized in patients with large, bilateral, or pancerebellar disorders, especially in a context of an acute onset of cerebel- lar disease. Posterior lobe damage was particularly important in the genesis of this novel syndrome. Damage of the vermal regions was consistently present in patients with disruption of affect. Anterior lobe damage was found to be less important to cause cognitive and behavioral deficits. Schmahmann Introduction xix syndrome in patients with stroke improved over time, but executive function remained abnormal. Schmahmann and Sherman (1998) pointed out that on the basis of their observations, it was not possible to distinguish the contribu- tion of the lesioned cerebellum to these abnormal behaviors from that of the cerebral regions newly deprived of their connections with the cerebellum. Indeed, the clinical features of the cognitive and affective impairments consti- tuting Schmahmann syndrome are identical to those usually identified in patients with supratentorial lesions affecting the cortical association areas and paralimbic regions and their interconnections. Reciprocal connections link- ing the cerebral association areas and paralimbic regions with the cerebellum constitute the neuroanatomical basis to explain the pathophysiological mech- anisms of the cerebellar induced cognitive and affective deficits. As pointed out by Schmahmann and coworkers in an influential series of neuroanatomi- cal studies, the cerebrocerebellar anatomical circuitry consists of a feedforward limb (the corticopontine and pontocerebellar pathways) and a feedback limb (the cerebellothalamic and thalamocortical systems) reciprocally connecting the cerebellum with the supratentorial regions crucially implicated in cogni- tive and affective processing. Since then, cerebellar involvement in linguistic processes has been studied by advanced neuroimaging methods in healthy subjects and several studies have been published focusing on a variety of lin- guistic dysfunctions following cerebellar lesions of different etiologies in chil- dren as well as adults. Reviews of the role of the cerebellum in nonmotor language functions are provided by Gordon (1996), Mariën, Engelborghs, Fabbro, and De Deyn (2001), Mariën et al. (2014), Paquier and Mariën (2005), De Smet, Baillieux, De Deyn, Mariën, and Paquier (2007), Beaton and Mariën (2010), Murdoch (2010), Highnam and Bleile (2011) and De Smet, Paquier, Verhoeven and Mariën (2013). With the introduction of Schmahmann syndrome, neuroscientists deal- ing with the cerebellum have now a better idea of the topography of cere- bellar deficits. Maps of lesion symptoms have been identified, clarifying the roles of cerebellar lobules in motor, cognitive, or behavioral operations. THE SUBSERVING NEURAL NETWORK The neuroanatomical substrate of the recently acknowledged nonmotor role of the cerebellum in cognitive and affective processing is a dense and reciprocal network of crossed cerebrocerebellar pathways consisting of corticopontocerebellar and cerebellothalamocortical loops that establish a close connection between the cerebellum and the supratentorial motor,

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.