ebook img

The Global Structure Of Visual Space PDF

227 Pages·2004·6.456 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview The Global Structure Of Visual Space

Advanced Series on Mathematical Psychology VOl Global Structure Tarow Indow •The Global Structure Visual Space ADVANCED SERIES ON MATHEMATICAL PSYCHOLOGY Series Editors: H. Colonius (University of Oldenburg, Germany) E. N. Dzhafarov (Purdue University, USA) Vol. 1: The Global Structure of Visual Space by T. Indow Advanced Series on Mathematical Psychology The Global Structure of Visual Space Tarow Indow University of California, Irvine, USA Y|g5 World Scientific NEW JERSEY • LONDON • SINGAPORE • BEIJING- SHANGHAI • HONGKONG • TAIPEI • CHENNAI Published by World Scientific Publishing Co. Pte. Ltd. 5 Toh Tuck Link, Singapore 596224 USA office: Suite 202,1060 Main Street, River Edge, NJ 07661 UK office: 57 Shelton Street, Covent Garden, London WC2H 9HE British Library Cataloguing-in-Publication Data A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library. THE GLOBAL STRUCTURE OF VISUAL SPACE Advanced Series on Mathematical Psychology — Vol. 1 Copyright © 2004 by World Scientific Publishing Co. Pte. Ltd. All rights reserved. This book, or parts thereof, may not be reproduced in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording or any information storage and retrieval system now known or to be invented, without written permission from the Publisher. For photocopying of material in this volume, please pay a copying fee through the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc., 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, USA. In this case permission to photocopy is not required from the publisher. ISBN 981-238-842-7 Printed in Singapore by World Scientific Printers (S) Pte Ltd Foreword In moving around, most animals guide the body in accordance with the information obtainable through vision. Humans are not exceptional. For humans, however, perceived space has played a more important role. Our ability to see the highly structured surrounds as they are has provided us with another ability to grasp the geometrical structure within a figure or between figures. This is a starting point of logical thinking in humans. As will be discussed in this book, "null or void" and "infinity" are beyond our direct perceptual experience. This may have something to do with the fact that humans have had difficulty in achieving these concepts. The use of the positional notation corresponding to 0 was an invention in India, perhaps around 6th century. Medieval abacus had the column corresponding to 0 but mathematicians had difficulty to conceptualize it. "The zero is something that must be there in order to say that nothing is there" (Menninger, 1969, p.400). Conceptualizing infinity is even subtler. Often people imagine a condition of "countless" as the image for infinity because this condition can occur as a phenomenon of our visual experience. The space we see extends in three directions. Gardner (1990) vividly describes how difficult it is to visualize something that is not in our perceived space. According to him, until the 19th century, mathematicians did not realize the possibility of extending Euclidean geometry to dimensions higher than three. Investigating visual perception has been a main subject of experimental psychology. However, most studies have been concerned with local phenomena in the space we see. It is especially true since modern technology was introduced in experiments of visual perception. Generating visual patterns on a CRT by computer and the availability of fMRI (functional magnetic resonance imaging) have revolutionized the experimentation, and most investigators are more and more interested in detailed study of phenomena in a small area in the visual space. Consequently, studying the global structure inherent in what we see escapes the attention it deserves. This is the problem to be taken up in this book. The publication of experimental results referred to in this book began in 1962. Many of my students at Keio University in Tokyo made contributions. The following names deserve explicit mention: Emiko v vi Global Structure of Visual Space Inoue, Nobuko Momomi, Mitsuho Ohta, Mitsuko Shimada, Keiko Matsushima, Yasuo Nishikawa, Hisako Miyauchi, Noriko Yamashita, Akira Nakada, and Toshio Watanabe. I taught at Keio University until 1979, then moved to the University of California Irvine (UCI). Watanabe was accepted to UCI as a graduate student and received a Ph.D. with the study cited in Sec.5.1.1. Experiments numbered as 5 in Table 2.2 were performed by a student Kevin Wright. While I was in Japan, I was given an opportunity to work as a research fellow at the Laboratory of Psychophysics in Harvard, 1963 - 1966. S.S. Stevens was the director. During that period, I met R.D. Luce, a professor of University of Pennsylvania at that time. Later, he invited me to the Institute for Advanced Study in Princeton as a visiting member, 1971-1972, and recommended me to UCI when he moved from UCI to Harvard. I am particularly indebted to these two distinguished scholars in the United States. My study on visual space at UCI was partially supported by the National Science Foundation Grant, IST80-23893. As to the global structure of visual space, I have exchanged opinions with many scientists all over the world. Discussions with Patrick Suppes of Stanford University and Jan Drosler of University of Regensburg in Germany were especially helpful to me. The writing of this book is due to strong recommendation by two colleagues in the Society for Mathematical Psychology, E.N. Dzhafarov, a professor at Purdue University, and H. Colonius, a professor at Universitat Oldenburg, Germany. Irvine, California Tarow Indow December, 2003 Menninger, K Number words and number symbols:A cultural history of numbers (MIT Press, Cambridge, 1958). Gardner, M The new ambidextrous universe: Symmetry and asymmetry from mirror reflections to superstrings (W.F.Freeman, New York, 1990). CONTENTS Foreword v Abbreviations and Symbols ix 1. Visual Space 1 1.1 Global Structure of Visual Space 1 1.1.1 Features of VS 2 1.2 Binocular Vision 8 1.2.1 Cyclopean Vision in the Horizontal Plane of Eye-level 9 1.2.2 3-D Cyclopean Vision 11 1.2.3 Spatial Behavior 14 2. Luneburg Model 17 2.1 P- and D-alleys, H-curves in the Horizontal Plane 17 2.1.1 Experiments with Stationary Points 17 2.1.2 Discrepancy between {Q/}p and {Qj} 20 D 2.2 VS as a Riemannian Space of Constant Curvature 22 2.2.1 Riemannian Space of Constant Curvature 22 2.2.2 Eudlidean Map (EM) 26 2.2.3 Equations of P-and D-alleys, H-curves in EM2 29 2.3 Theoretical Curves in X2 33 2.3.1 Luneburg's Mapping Functions 33 2.3.2 Equations of P-and D-alleys and H-curves in X2 37 2.3.3 Comments on Results of Alley Experiments 39 2.3.4 Comments on Values of-K and a 44 2.4 Derivations and Explanations 47 2.4.1 Supplementary Explanations to Sec.2.2.1 47 2.4.2 Derivations of Equations in Secs.2.2.2 and 2.2.3 51 3. Two Extensions of Luneburg Model 67 3.1 Alleys on a Frontoparallel Plane 68 3.1.1 Theoretical Equations 68 3.1.2 Experimental Results 75 3.2 Direct Mapping according to Riemannian Metric 79 3.2.1 Multidimensional Mapping according to Riemannian Metric 80 3.2.2 Experimental Results 91 vii viii Global Structure of Visual Space 3.2.3 Concluding Remarks to Sec.3.2 97 4. Visual Space under Natural Conditions 103 4.1 The Perceived Sky and Ground 103 4.1.1 Bisection of the Sky 104 4.1.2 The Moon Illusion 109 4.1.3 Multidimensional Construction of the Night Sky 113 4.1.4 Horizon 117 4.2 Scaling of Radial Distance 8 122 0 4.2.1 Scaling based on Difference Judgment 122 4.2.2 Scaling based on Ratio Judgment 129 4.2.3 Discussion on the Form of d(x) 133 4.3 Perceived Spatial Layouts under Full Cue Conditions 137 4.3.1 Three Experiments 137 4.3.2 General Discussion 143 4.3.3 Regular Triangles with S at the Barycenter 145 5. Related Experiments and Theoretical Considerations 149 5.1 Spatial Layouts in Frameless Visual Space 149 5.1.1 An Experiment with Circles 149 5.1.2 Experiments using Triangles 152 5.1.3 General Comments and Derivations 159 5.2 Mapping Functions 163 5.2.1 Experimental Data 163 5.2.2 Theoretical Considerations 167 5.2.3 Roles of Mapping Functions 173 5.3 Experimental Tests of Properties of VS as an R 175 5.3.1 Two Experiments of Foley 175 5.3.2 Bottom-up Experimental Approaches 180 5.4 Discussion on the Postulate that VS is anR 186 5.4.1 Helmholtz-Lie Problem 186 5.4.2 Congruence and Similarity in VS 189 5.4.3 Linear Perspective 193 5.4.4 Concluding Remarks 197 References 201 Author and Subject Indexes 211 Credits 216 Abbreviations and Symbols S subject (observer) in experiment VS visual space X physical space E Euclidean space R Riemannian space of constant curvature K Gaussian (totoal) curvature r curvature radius EM Euclidean map to represent R in E (Poncare) BS, BC basic sphere, basic circle in EM 2 inSec.2.4.2 Klein's model for R2 of K<0 Q in Sec.2.4.2 sphere representing R2 of K > 0 Q, Q(x,y^\ Q(y,<|>,9) stimulus point in X, see Fig.2.3 y, <>| , 9 convergence, lateral, elevation angles Pv(£v,Tlv£v)> Pv($o, <Pv, $v) perceived point in VS, see Fig.2.3 P(£,TI,Q, P(p0, (p, $) point representing Pv in EM, see Fig.2.3 O or 0 origin of coordinates, body in X, self in VS,EM DP(8) X2 extending in depth along x with an elevation angle 9 or its conterpart in VS and EM horizontal DP DP with 9 = 0 HP frontoparallel plane in VS or its conterpart in X, EM e, e distance, radial distance (Euclidean) 0 in "X" 111 _/V P, Po distance, radial distance (Euclidean) in EM 8, 8 distance, radial distace (perceptual) 0 in VS, latent variable d,d scaled value of 8, 8 0 0 IX

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.