Western Kentucky University TopSCHOLAR® Masters Theses & Specialist Projects Graduate School 5-2015 The Effects of Anticipated Feedback Proximity on Performance: Exploring the Moderating Role of Self-Efficacy and Task Type Xingya Xu Western Kentucky University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at:http://digitalcommons.wku.edu/theses Part of theExperimental Analysis of Behavior Commons Recommended Citation Xu, Xingya, "The Effects of Anticipated Feedback Proximity on Performance: Exploring the Moderating Role of Self-Efficacy and Task Type" (2015).Masters Theses & Specialist Projects.Paper 1502. http://digitalcommons.wku.edu/theses/1502 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by TopSCHOLAR®. It has been accepted for inclusion in Masters Theses & Specialist Projects by an authorized administrator of TopSCHOLAR®. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE EFFECTS OF ANTICIPATED FEEDBACK PROXIMITY ON PERFORMANCE: EXPLORING THE MODERATING ROLE OF SELF-EFFICACY AND TASK TYPE A Thesis Presented to The Faculty of the Department of Psychological Sciences Western Kentucky University Bowling Green, Kentucky In Partial Fulfillment Of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Science By Xingya Xu May 2015 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS First and foremost, I would like to offer my sincerest gratitude to my advisor, Dr. Qin Zhao for her continuous assistance and dedicated involvement in my research and my Master’s study. I would like to thank her very much for the support, patience and encouragement. Further gratitude is extended to Dr. Jenni Redifer and Dr. Andrew Mienaltowski for serving as thesis committee members, as well as their insightful comments, professional suggestions and encouragement. I would also have to acknowledge Dr. Elizabeth Lemerise and Dr. Aaron Wichman, who motivated and supported me to chase my academic dream. To other faculty, Dr. Steve R. Wininger, Dr. Anthony Paquin and Dr. Reagan Brown, I would like to express gratitude to their enlightenment and assistance. Thanks also goes out to Battogtokh Zagdsuren for her help in data grading, and to Casey Fortney for his assistance in programming Media Lab software. A special thanks to my family for the support they provided me and in particular, I must acknowledge my husband and best friend, Lianqi Wang, without whose love, encouragement and assistance, I would not have accomplished this thesis. iii CONTENTS INTRODUCTION ................................ .............................................................................1 METHOD ......................................................................................................................... .8 RESULTS ........................................................................................................................ 13 DISCUSSION .................................................................................................................. 16 REFERENCES ................................................................................................................ 20 APPENDIX A: REMOTE ASSOCIATE TEST (RAT)...................................................25 APPENDIX B: THE ABBREVIATED TORRANCE TEST FOR ADULTS (ATTA)...26 APPENDIX C: MATH TEST...........................................................................................27 APPENDIX D: VERBAL TEST.......................................................................................29 APPENDIX E: PROBLEM-SOLVING SELF-EFFICACY.............................................31 APPENDIX F: TASK INVOLVEMENT, EFFORT AND ENJOYMENT..................... 32 iv THE EFFECTS OF ANTICIPATED FEEDBACK PROXIMITY ON PERFORMANCE: EXPLORING THE MODERATING ROLE OF SELF-EFFICACY AND TASK TYPE Xingya Xu May 2015 33 Pages Directed by: Qin Zhao, Jenni Redifer, and Andrew Mienaltowski Department of Psychological Sciences Western Kentucky University The present study investigated the effect of anticipated feedback proximity (immediately after completing the task or one week later) on performance and the moderating role of self-efficacy and task types (analytical or creative). I hypothesized that expecting rapid feedback should yield better performance than expecting delayed feedback, for people with high self-efficacy or those who receive analytical tasks. For those who receive creative tasks or have low self-efficacy, expecting rapid feedback may produce negative impact on performance. The results indicated a trend of main effect of anticipated feedback proximity. Specifically, participants performed better when they expected immediate feedback relative to expecting delayed feedback, regardless of the task type. There was also a main effect of task type. Performance on the analytical tasks was better than performance on the creative tasks. However, neither self-efficacy nor task type moderated the effects of anticipated feedback proximity. The implications for these findings are discussed herein. v Introduction In learning settings, people often receive feedback on their performance, and this feedback plays an important role in helping people to reflect and improve. Feedback can be understood as a process which provides information in response to one’s behaviour while completing tasks. The extensive study of feedback in research indicates its important role in learning (Schmidt & Bjork, 1992). Researchers have also started to study some extended features of feedback, such as anticipated feedback proximity. Anticipated feedback proximity refers to how quickly people expect to receive feedback on their performance. Anticipated Feedback Proximity The earliest evidence for a relationship between anticipated feedback proximity and performance is from the research conducted by Nisan (1976), who reported that the expectation of immediate feedback led participants to have lower task performance than those who expected delayed feedback (1 week delay). Ninety-six boys in three seventh- grade classes from a school in Jerusalem were the participants. Two tasks in this study included constructing a word from six letters and an instruction test (Burt, 1927). The author gave example of the instruction test: “If you have already eaten supper today write here M, and if not write S.” Surprisingly, no research has been published about anticipated feedback proximity since Nisan (1976) until recently. Kettle and Häubl (2010) conducted a field experiment to explore the effects of anticipated feedback proximity on both actual and predicted performance on an individual oral presentation task. They obtained the opposite results to Nisan’s (1976) experiment. Kettle and Häubl (2010) randomly assigned 1 participants to a presentation date, varying the date at which participants expected to receive their presentation grades (i.e., delay ranging from 0 to 17 days). Presentations were peer graded on a scale from 0 (poor) to 10 (excellent), and participants also made rank predictions (1 to 10) of their own performance. The results showed that anticipating more rapid feedback improved performance on the oral presentation task, and yet lowered performance prediction. The reported effects of anticipated feedback proximity were attributed to the desire to avoid disappointment (Kettle & Häubl, 2010). van Dijk, Zeelenberg, and Van der Pligt (2003) have suggested that the more proximate the anticipated feedback is, the more easily people feel the threat of disappointment. One way to avoid the negative feelings of disappointment is for the individual who is receiving feedback to lower their expectations while also working hard to perform well (Shepperd & McNulty, 2002). A recent lab experiment involving a verbal aptitude task replicated the main findings of Kettle and Häubl (2010): anticipated feedback proximity had a significant effect on actual and judged performance, and expecting more proximate feedback caused people to perform better (Fajfar, Campitelli, & Labollita, 2012). One important difference between the two studies was that Kettle and Häubl (2010) examined predictions of future performance, whereas Fajfar et al. (2012) studied post-test estimation of performance. Fajfar et al. (2012) randomly assigned participants to one of two feedback proximity conditions: immediate versus delayed (after a week). They were asked to finish the BAIRES test of verbal aptitude (Cortada de Kohan, 2003), which asked participants to judge the definitions and synonyms of nouns. One dollar was rewarded for every correct answer, providing an incentive to motivate thoughtful and accurate performance. In 2 addition to actual test performance, the researchers examined participants’ post-test estimation of performance (how many correct items they think they achieved), estimation of others’ performance (the average number of others’ correct responses), etc. The main findings were that the group expecting immediate feedback had better performance than the group expecting delayed feedback. The immediate feedback group underestimated their performance, whereas the delayed feedback group was fairly accurate and showed no significant bias in their judgment. Explanation for this finding was defensive pessimism: when the feedback was close, people were apt to have a pessimistic attitude about performance, and they worked hard to avoid negative feedback. Therefore, performance would be improved by working harder. In addition to the main effects of anticipated feedback proximity on actual and judged performance, researchers have recently explored individual differences in response to anticipated feedback proximity, for example, the moderating role of metacognitive beliefs about intelligence (Zhao, Zhang, & Vance, 2013). Research suggests that there are two main types of metacognitive beliefs (or implicit theories) about intelligence: incremental theory and entity theory (Dweck, 1999; Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Molden & Dweck, 2006). Incremental theorists believe that intelligence is malleable and can be developed with effort. People with this belief tend to perceive feedback as useful information to improve their abilities (Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Molden & Dweck, 2006). However, entity theorists believe that intelligence is largely stable or fixed. People with this belief tend to perceive feedback as evaluative judgments on their abilities and are more likely to admit they have reached their ability’s limits (Dweck & Leggett, 1988). Zhao et al. (2013) hypothesized that expecting rapid (versus 3 delayed) feedback would motivate better performance, particularly for people with an incremental view of intelligence who embrace feedback. Participants recruited from an introductory psychology course were randomly assigned to one of the four conditions resulting from a 2 x 2 between-subjects design: (anticipated feedback proximity: same day vs. 3 days later; beliefs about intelligence: incremental vs. entity). Prior to the exam, participants read one of the two versions of the article named “The Origins of Intelligence: Is the Nature-Nurture Controversy Resolved,” which was adopted to manipulate incremental or entity beliefs about intelligence (Miele, Finn, & Molden, 2011). Participants also received instructions about when they would likely know their test grades, on the same day or 3 days later. The main results showed that anticipation of rapid feedback improved performance in the incremental group but reduced performance in the entity group. It thus seems that anticipating rapid feedback can be motivating or detrimental, depending on individuals’ beliefs about ability (Zhao et al., 2013). In summary, research on the effects of anticipated feedback proximity on judged and actual performance has important practical implications but is surprisingly scarce. The available evidence thus far demonstrates the significant effect of anticipated feedback proximity (rapid versus delayed) on people’s performance. In addition, individual differences may exist in response to anticipated feedback proximity. Overall, the main purpose of the current research is to extend past work by exploring additional moderating variables, including individual and task factors. Self-Efficacy Self-efficacy is a kind of strong personal belief about oneself. In social cognitive theory, Albert Bandura first introduced self-efficacy as an important concept in 1977. 4
Description: