ebook img

The Developmental Costs of High Self-Esteem for Antisocial Children PDF

13 Pages·2010·0.25 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview The Developmental Costs of High Self-Esteem for Antisocial Children

ChildDevelopment, November/December2007,Volume78,Number6,Pages1627–1639 The Developmental Costs of High Self-Esteem for Antisocial Children MadhaviMenonandDesireeD.Tobin BrookeC.Corby FloridaAtlanticUniversity LynnUniversity MeenakshiMenon ErnestV.E.Hodges FloridaAtlanticUniversity St.John’sUniversity DavidG.Perry FloridaAtlanticUniversity Two hypotheses—high self-esteem leads children to act on antisocial cognitions (disposition-activating hypothesis) and high self-esteem leads children to rationalize antisocial conduct (disposition-rationalizing hypothesis)—wereinvestigatedintwolongitudinalstudies.InStudy1(N5189;meanage511.1years),antisocial behaviorwasaggression;inStudy2(N5407;meanage510.8years)itwasavoidanceofthemother.Inboth studies, there was little evidence for the disposition-activating hypothesis but considerable support for the disposition-rationalizinghypothesis.Overtime,aggressivechildrenwithhighself-esteemincreasinglyvaluedthe rewards that aggression offers and belittled their victims, and avoidant children with high self-esteem increasinglyviewedtheirmotherasharassinganduninvolved.Forantisocialchildren,highself-esteemcarries costs. Introduction Onecentralthemethatemergesisthatpeoplewith high self-esteem are intolerant of threats to the self, High self-esteem is so widely considered a boon to andtheyvigorouslystrivetomaintainorrestoretheir children’s development that it is commonly found highsenseofself-worthwhenitisthreatened.Inone among a list of ‘‘positive adjustment outcomes’’ longitudinal investigation, Egan and Perry (1998) included in developmental investigations. Indeed, foundthathighself-esteemprotectedpreadolescents highself-esteemdoesoftengowithgoodthings,such fromvictimizationbypeers,apparentlybecausehigh- as school success, athletic competence, physical self-esteem children refused to submit to bullies’ attractiveness, positive relationships with parents coerciveovertures.Adultswithhighself-esteemstick and peers, absence of internalizing symptoms (e.g., up for themselves too, and in a number of ways, anxiety, depression) and eating disorders, and low including speaking up and expressing their views, participationinriskbehavior(Baumeister,Campbell, cultivatingtheirmostpromisingtraits(asopposedto Krueger, & Vohs, 2003; Harter, 1998). For many of focusing on remedying their deficiencies), taking these associations, however, self-esteem may be the credit for successes and denying blame for failures, consequenceratherthanthedeterminant(Baumeister, reacting to setbacks by increasing their expectations 1998;Leary&Baumeister,2000).Longitudinalstudies for success, and persisting in the face of failure showing that self-esteem promotes advantageous (Baumeister, 1998; Baumeister et al., 2003; Blaine & outcomes over time (or buffers against the ill effects Crocker,1993;LePine&VanDyne,1998). of risk factors) are rare (Baumeister et al., 2003). Theconfident,assertive,ego-servingself-systemof Nevertheless, a few conclusions about the effects of high-self-esteem people is often likely to promote self-esteem are possible based mainly on the scant adaptive outcomes for these individuals. However, longitudinal evidence and laboratory research with itisnowclearthat,atleastforasubsetofpeoplewith adults. highself-esteem,suchaself-systemmayalsoleadto harmful outcomes, especially for the persons’ inter- action partners but also for the high-self-esteem ThisresearchwassupportedbyGrant1R01HD38280fromthe persons themselves. Several investigators have pro- NationalInstituteofChildHealthandHumanDevelopment.This posed that it is necessary to distinguish secure high article was written while D.G.P. was Visiting Professor of PsychologyattheUniversityofHongKong. Correspondenceconcerningthisarticleshouldbeaddressedto David G. Perry, Department of Psychology, Florida Atlantic University,BocaRaton,FL33431.Electronicmailmaybesentto #2007bytheSocietyforResearchinChildDevelopment,Inc. [email protected]. Allrightsreserved.0009-3920/2007/7806-0001 1628 Menonetal. self-esteem from insecure high self-esteem and have children to rationalize antisocial conduct, thereby stressed that persons with high but insecure self- solidifying an antisocial value system. Confirmation esteem are at risk for problematic development, of either possibility would require further amend- especiallyantisocialconduct(e.g.,Baumeister,Smart, menttoaconceptualizationofhighself-esteemasan &Boden,1996;Kernis,Grannemann,&Barclay,1989). unmitigatedblessingandsuggestcertaincautionsto Individuals have been said to have insecure, or investigators planning interventions to boost child- fragile, high self-esteem if their high self-appraisals ren’s self-esteem. Each of the two possibilities is appear to be inauthentic (at variance with other consideredinturn. people’sevaluationsofthem;Salmivalli,Kaukiainen, Kaistaniemi, & Lagerspetz, 1999; Zakriski & Coie, DoesHighSelf-EsteemEncourageChildrentoActon 1996),narcissistic(reflectfeelingsofsuperiority,infal- AntisocialCognitions? libility, and entitlement; Bushman & Baumeister, 1998), coupled with implicit (unconscious) low self- The first hypothesis of this research is that high esteem(e.g.,asindexedbytheImplicitAttitudeTest; self-esteem causes children to translate antisocial Greenwaldetal.,2002),unstable(showingshort-term thought into antisocial behavior. Antisocial conduct fluctuations;Kernis,2003;Kernisetal.,1989),contin- takes different forms, and different social–cognitive gent(dependentonspecificqualities,suchasphysical factorsunderlieeachform.Aggression—thequintes- attractiveness or outperforming others; Crocker & sentialantisocialbehavior—isspurredbyanumberof Wolfe, 2001; Deci & Ryan, 1995), or coupled with cognitivemotivators,includingahostileattributional the sense that others harbor ill will toward them bias, aggressive goals, the belief that aggression is (Salmivalli,Ojanen,Haanpaa,&Peets,2005).Children normative and expected, expectations of reward, (Salmivalli et al., 1999, 2005; Waschull & Kernis, expectations of victim suffering, and perceptions of 1996) and adults (Bushman & Baumeister, 1998; self-efficacy (Bandura, 1986; Crick & Dodge, 1994; Kernis et al., 1989; Twenge & Campbell, 2003) who Dodge,1986;Egan,Monson,&Perry,1998;Huesmann havehighbutinsecureself-esteemarepronetoanger, &Guerra,1997;Slaby&Guerra,1988;Weiss,Dodge, hostility,andaggression,especiallyinresponsetoego Bates, & Pettit, 1992). Another type of antisocial threats(e.g.,challengestotheiradequacyinadomain conduct is the active avoidance or dismissal of a in which they stake their self-esteem). Presumably, relationship partner evidenced by persons with an personswithhighbutinsecureself-esteemlashoutat insecure/avoidantattachment.Avoidant attachment people who criticize or disrespect them as a way of hasbeenstudiedmainlyininfants’relationshipswith avoiding painful downward revisions of their lofty their mother (Ainsworth, 1979) but also in adults’ self-concepts(Baumeister,1998).Incontrast,persons conceptions of their relationships with their parents with secure high self-esteem tend to be nonviolent (Crowell, Fraley, & Shaver, 1999) and their romantic andaresometimesexceptionallyprosocial(e.g.,pro- partners (Feeney, 1999). However, some preadoles- tect and defend victimized peers; Salmivalli et al., cents self-report an avoidant attachment to their 1999, 2005). Given the heterogeneity of high-self- mother by denying affection toward her, shunning esteempeoplewithrespecttoaggressivetendencies, herwhenupset,avoidingherduringexplorationand itisperhapsunsurprisingthat,asagroup,high-self- reunion, and refusing to use her as a task-relevant esteem persons are not consistently more or less resource (Finnegan, Hodges, & Perry, 1996). Such aggressive than low-self-esteem persons (though avoidance forecasts aggression toward peers when a difference is found, high-self-esteem people (Hodges, Finnegan, & Perry, 1999). Presumably, the usuallyarelessaggressivethanlow-self-esteempeo- cognitions motivating avoidant attachment are per- ple;e.g.,Donnellan,Trzesniewski,Robins,Moffitt,& ceptions of the mother as unloving, unavailable if Caspi,2005;Sprott&Doob,2000). needed, intrusive, disinterested, harsh, or rejecting Clearly, then, high self-esteem has a dark side, (Ainsworth,1979).Indeed,preadolescentswhoreport revealed in the violence of persons whose high self- anavoidantattachmentdoperceivetheirmothersin esteem is insecure. However, there may exist other, theseways(Kerns,Tomich,Aspelmeier,&Contreras, previously unrecognized, developmental risks asso- 2000;Yunger,Corby,&Perry,2005). ciated with having high self-esteem. The present Our prediction is that children with high self- research explores two such additional possibilities. esteemaremorelikelythanlow-self-esteemchildren The first is that high self-esteem causes children to toactonanyantisocialcognitionsthattheyhappento act on social cognitions (e.g., expectations, values, possess.Inotherwords,thecognitionsthatencourage efficacy beliefs) that encourage antisocial conduct. aggression (e.g., expectation of reward) or avoidant A second possibility is that high self-esteem causes attachment (e.g., perception of the mother as CostsofHighSelf-Esteem 1629 rejecting) should promote the behavior in question adversaries (peer victims, mothers) by increasingly mainly for high-self-esteem children. This disposition- derogatingthemovertime. activating function of high self-esteem is expected Ourviewthathighself-esteemworsensthedevel- because high-self-esteem persons, compared with opmentofchildrenwhoholdantisocialcognitions(by low-self-esteem people, place greater confidence causingthemtocarryouttheirinjuriousthoughts)or and trust in the beliefs that they hold, are actually whoengageinantisocialconduct(byreinforcingtheir more accurate in their perceptions of people and antisocialworldviews)standsincontrasttoaconcep- situations, possess greater self-concept clarity, are tion of high self-esteem as a panacea-like buffer moreconfidentoftheirstrengths,actmoreassertively against all things bad. A critic of our position might on their beliefs and values, select more efficacious argue that children who have achieved high self- strategies for pursuing goals, and are disinclined to esteemshouldfeeltheleastneedeithertoactontheir give up easily when thwarted (Baumeister, 1998; antisocial cognitions or to rationalize theirantisocial Baumgardner,1990;Campbell,1990;Campbelletal., conduct, because their strong sense of self should 1996;Kernis,2003;Murray,Rose,Bellavia,Holmes,& provide a sense of security and well-being that Kusche, 2002; Sandelands, Brockner, & Glynn, 1988; obviates the need for hurting others or rationalizing Swann & Pelham, 2002; Tedeschi & Norman, 1985). hurtful conduct. However, the considerations we These qualities should conspire to cause high-self- haveraisedsuggestthatitismorereasonabletothink esteemindividualstoactonanyantisocialcognitions that high self-esteem, when combined with either theyharbor. antisocial behavior or antisocial cognitions, consti- tutesriskfor,ratherthanprotectionagainst,develop- mentaladversity. DoesHighSelf-EsteemCauseAntisocialChildrento Thisarticle reportstwolongitudinalstudies,each JustifyTheirConduct? ofwhichtestsboththedisposition-activatingandthe Oursecondhypothesis,whichisnotincompatible disposition-rationalizing hypotheses as they pertain withthefirst,isthathighself-esteemcauseschildren toantisocialconduct.Thefirststudy,whichisareanal- torationalizeantisocialbehaviorafterperformingit. ysisofdatapublishedbyEganetal.(1998),teststhe That is, high self-esteem should lead aggressive hypotheses with respect to aggression. The second children to develop cognitions that justify (and sus- study presents new data and tests the hypotheses tain) aggression, such as the conviction that aggres- withrespecttoavoidantattachmenttothemother. sion is rewarding; it should also lead avoidantly attached children to develop cognitions that justify (and sustain) avoidant behavior, especially percep- Study1:Aggression tionsofthemotherasharassingandunloving. Thisdisposition-rationalizinghypothesisisbasedon This short-term longitudinal study evaluates two severalconsiderations.Asnoted,thememorybiases hypotheses. The first is that high self-esteem trans- ofhigh-self-esteemindividualsleadthemtoexagger- forms aggressive cognitions into aggressive action ateandtakecreditfortheirsuccessesandtominimize (thedisposition-activatinghypothesis).Thatis,Time and deny responsibility for their failures (Blaine & 1 self-esteem should interact with Time 1 aggressive Crocker,1993;Campbell,Reeder,Sedikides,&Elliot, cognitions to predict change in aggressive behavior 2000; John & Robins, 1994). Also, antisocial children over time, with aggressive cognitions forecasting with high self-esteem may reason about the self in increased aggression mainly for children with high awaythatyieldsantisocialvalues:‘‘IamgoodandI self-esteem. This hypothesis was tested for each of pushothersaround;therefore,pushingothersaround fiveaggression-encouragingcognitionsfoundinpre- is good’’ (see e.g., Rudman, 2004). Finally, high-self- vious work to be associated with aggression: expec- esteem people tend to engage in self-enhancing tation of reward, expectation of victim suffering, strategies when threatened. One self-enhancement value placed on reward, value placed on victim strategy particularly favored by persons with high suffering,andself-efficacyforaggression. self-esteem(especiallyiftheirself-esteemisinsecure) The second hypothesis is that high self-esteem isderogationofanadversary(Baumeisteretal.,2003; encourages aggressive children to rationalize their Kernis & Sun, 1994; Kernis et al., 1989; Morf & aggressive conduct—to develop cognitions that jus- Rhodewalt, 1993). It is likely that aggressive and tifyaggression(thedisposition-rationalizinghypoth- avoidantchildrenencounternumerousinterpersonal esis). Thus, Time 1 self-esteem should interact with threats(Dodge,1986;Yungeretal.,2005),andthus,it Time 1 aggressive behavior to predict change in is reasonable to expect that they might react to their aggressive cognitions over time, with aggressive 1630 Menonetal. behaviorportendingincreasedaggressivecognitions Cronbachasforthesemeasures,respectively,forthe mainlyforchildrenwithhighself-esteem. fall(spring)were.63(.69);.60(.70);.76(.83);.85(.83); and.87(.90).Sampleitemsfromthescalesaregivenin Eganetal.(1998).Scalescorescouldrangefrom1to4, Method with 4 indicating greater endorsement of the social Because details of this study are published (Egan cognition. Finally, Harter’s (1985) 6-item global self- et al., 1998), only highlights of the method are pro- worthscalewasadministeredateachtesting,butonly vided. Participants were 189, predominantly White Time1scoreswereused(fallCronbacha5.73);scores third-throughseventh-gradeboys(n592)andgirls could range from 1 to 4, with 4 indicating greater (n 5 97). Children were tested in the fall and again self-esteem. inthespringofaschoolyear.Theyaveraged10years 10monthsofageinthefall.Ateachtesting,children’s Results aggressionwasassessedwitha3-itempeernomination scale(e.g.,‘‘He/Shemakesfunofpeople.’’).Achild’s Intercorrelations of measures. Table 1 displays the score on aggression was determined by calculating associationsamongthemeasureswithchildsexand thepercentageofclassmateswhocheckedthechild’s agecontrolled(becausethemeansofsomemeasures name on each item and then totaling these percen- variedwithchildsexorage).Severalfeaturesofthese tagesacrosstheitems;thus,scorescouldrangefrom correlationsarenoteworthy.First,aggressionand,to 0to300(fallandspringCronbachas5.85and.87).At alesserdegree,thefiveaggression-encouragingcog- each testing, children also responded to a 40-item nitions were moderately stable over the school year. questionnaire assessing five aggression-encouraging Second, although there were some significant corre- cognitions(eightitemseach):expectationofreward(the lations among the cognitions at each time of testing, belief that aggression yields tangible and status the associations were not consistent or strong, sug- rewards),expectationofvictimsuffering(thebeliefthat gesting that it is worthwhile to retain the five cog- aggressioncausesphysicalorpsychologicalharmand nitions as separate variables. Third, surprisingly, suffering), value of reward (the importance placed on self-esteem was positively correlated with expecta- tangible and status rewards), value of victim suffering tionofvictimsufferingateachtimeoftesting. (feeling little empathy for one’s victims, e.g., not The disposition-activating hypothesis: does high self- caring whether a victim suffers), and self-efficacy for esteemmagnifythecontributionofcognitionstoaggressive aggression (feeling capable of enacting aggression). behavior? This hypothesis was evaluated in five Table1 CorrelationsAmongtheMeasuresofStudy1 Measure Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 1.Self-esteem(Time1) 1.00 .12 .05 .20* .10 .08 .05 .00 .02 .17* .10 .03 .03 ! ! ! ! ! ! 2.Aggression(Time1) 1.00 .11 .05 .15 .06 .14 .75** .08 .13 .20** .12 .11 3.Expectationofreward(Time1) 1.00 .25** .46** .44** .41** .13 .42** .35** .17* .16* .28** 4.Expectationofvictim 1.00 .25** .13 .10 .04 .09 .34** .05 .08 .03 ! suffering(Time1) 5.Valueofreward(Time1) 1.00 .34** .25** .18* .29** .27** .37** .25** .21** 6.Valueofvictimsuffering(Time1) 1.00 .44** .11 .17* .08 .10 .49** .31** 7.Self-efficacyforaggression(Time1) 1.00 .14 .38** .10 .20* .34** .60** 8.Aggression(Time2) 1.00 .09 .02 .15* .10 .18* 9.Expectationofreward(Time2) 1.00 .23** .28** .28** .47** 10.Expectationofvictim 1.00 .04 .07 .02 ! suffering(Time2) 11.Valueofreward(Time2) 1.00 .18* .22** 12.Valueofvictimsuffering(Time2) 1.00 .53** 13.Self-efficacyforaggression(Time2) 1.00 Note.TableentriesarefortheentiresampleofStudy1(N5189)andarepartialcorrelationscontrollingforageandsex. *p,.05.**p,.01. CostsofHighSelf-Esteem 1631 hierarchicalregressionanalyses,oneforeachaggres- In each case, the interaction conformed to the sive cognition. The dependent variable was always predicted pattern: high self-esteem exacerbated the Time 2 aggression. In each analysis, child sex, child contribution of aggression to the aggressive cogni- age,andTime1aggressionwereenteredonthefirst tion. The nature of each significant interaction was step. On the second step, Time 1 self-esteem and a examined using the procedure recommended by Time1cognitionmeasure(e.g.,expectationofreward) Aiken and West (1991), which estimates the relation wereentered.Onthethirdstep,thefocalinteractionof of a predictor (e.g., Time 1 aggression) to a criterion self-esteemandcognitionwastested. Supplementary (e.g.,aTime2cognition)ateachofthethreelevels( 1, ! analyses were also conducted to see whether child 0,and+1SD)ofamoderator(e.g.,Time1self-esteem); ageorsexmightmoderateafocaltwo-wayinteraction, relationsareestimatedintheformofunstandardized thatis,toseewhetherathree-wayinteractionofAge betacoefficients.Table 2displaystheresultsofthese " Self-Esteem Cognition or of Sex Self-Esteem follow-upanalyses.NoticethattheimpactofTime1 " " " Cognition was significant (with all relevant main aggression on each cognition outcome becomes effectsandtwo-wayinteractionsinthemodel). increasinglypositiveaschildren’sself-esteemmoves In no analysis was the focal two-way interaction from low to medium to high. These results are significant.Furthermore,innosupplementaryanaly- consistent with the idea that high-self-esteem chil- sis was the three-way interaction significant. Thus, drenrationalizetheirantisocialconduct. Study1failedtoyieldanysupportforthedisposition- Althoughthedisposition-rationalizinghypothesis activatinghypothesis. specifies that self-esteem moderates the impact of Thedisposition-rationalizinghypothesis:doeshighself- aggressiononcognition,andthereforeitisappropri- esteemmagnifythecontributionofaggressivebehaviorto atetoevaluatethehypothesisbytreatingself-esteem aggressive cognition? This hypothesis was evaluated asthemoderatorinthefollow-upresultsreportedin in five regression analyses, with the Time 2 level of Table 2, it is also possible to unpack the significant each cognition measure taking a turn as the depen- interactions by examining the impact of Time 1 self- dent variable. In each analysis, child sex, child age, esteemonTime2cognitionatdifferentlevelsofTime andtheTime1cognitionvariablewereenteredonthe 1aggression(i.e.,bytreatingTime1aggressionasthe first step; Time 1 self-esteem and Time 1 aggression moderator). Results of such analyses are given in were entered on the second step; and the focal Table 3. Noteworthy is that the beta coefficients in interaction of self-esteem and aggression was tested the first column are uniformly negative, with two on the third step. Supplementary analyses tested reaching significance. These data suggest that for whether child sex or age moderated any focal two- children who are nonaggressive, high self-esteem is way interaction. In four analyses, the focal two-way notproblematic but infactinhibitsthedevelopment interaction was significant, or nearly so, and in no ofaggression-encouragingcognitions. supplementary analysis was the three-way interac- tion significant (i.e., no significant two-way interac- Discussion tion was moderated by age or sex). The interaction was evident for expectation of reward (F 5 8.45, Results support the view that aggressive children p , .004), value of reward (F 5 5.69, p , .02), value who have high self-esteem react to their aggressive ofvictimsuffering(F53.51,p,.07),andself-efficacy conduct with a variety of self-enhancing and foraggression(F54.37,p,.04). aggression-justifying strategies, including viewing Table2 RelationofTime1AggressiontoTime2CognitionasaFunctionofTime1Self-Esteem(Study1) Time1self-esteem Time2cognition Low( 1SD) Medium(0SD) High(+1SD) ! Expectationofreward .20y .01 .19* ! ! Valueofreward .02 .15* .33*** ! Valueofvictimsuffering .04 .08 .20* ! Self-efficacyforaggression .10 .02 .15y ! Note.TableentriesareunstandardizedbetacoefficientsfromtheAiken&West(1991)procedure.Withineachrow,thereissignificant variabilityamongthebetas. yp,.10.*p,.05.***p,.001. 1632 Menonetal. Table3 RelationofTime1Self-EsteemtoTime2CognitionasaFunctionofTime1Aggression(Study1) Time1aggression Time2cognition Low( 1SD) Medium(0SD) High(+1SD) ! Expectationofreward .14 .06 .25* ! Valueofreward .22* .05 .13 ! ! Valueofvictimsuffering .11 .01 .14 ! Self-efficacyforaggression .12* .03 .06 ! ! Note.TableentriesareunstandardizedbetacoefficientsfromtheAiken&West(1991)procedure.Withineachrow,thereissignificant variabilityamongthebetas. *p,.05. theselfasmorepowerfulandsuccessful,viewingthe Third,perhapsthecognitivedimensionsunderstudy rewardsofaggressionasmoredesirable,andviewing were not ones that are the most relevant to an theinflictingofharmandsufferingonone’svictimsas aggression-activatingmechanism.Perhapshighself- less objectionable. Low-self-esteem children are not esteem is more likely to transform hostile attribu- inclined to react to their aggressive actions in these tional bias or aggressivegoals, neitherof which was self-serving,aggression-encouragingways. investigated here,into aggressive action. Finally, the Althoughresultsareconsistentwithadisposition- relatively high overtime stability of the aggression rationalizing mechanism, other factors may have measure may have made it difficult to identify contributed. Aggressive children who have high predictorsofchange. self-esteem may enact their aggression more confi- dently or with greater success than aggressive chil- dren with less self-esteem (see Perry, Perry, & Study2:AvoidantAttachment Kennedy, 1992, for a discussion of effectual vs. inef- fectual aggressors), and these qualities may have This 1-year longitudinal study also evaluated the mediated the effects. Also, it is likely that the high disposition-activating and disposition-rationalizing self-esteem of aggressive children is insecure (e.g., hypotheses but with avoidant behavior toward the unstable,inauthentic),andtheinsecurityofthehigh mother serving as the antisocial behavior. Here, the self-esteemmayaccountfortheresults.Directassess- disposition-activating hypothesis is that high self- ments of the qualities of children’s aggression (e.g., esteem causes children to act on cognitions that efficacy)andofthesecurityoftheirself-esteemwould encourage avoidance of the mother. Presumably, bedesirableinfutureresearch. avoidantchildrenexperience themother as aversive Regardless of the mechanism(s), it is clear that and are trying to exit the relationship. Thus, we high-self-esteem aggressors are inclined to increase examined whether high self-esteem spurs avoidant theiraggression-encouragingbeliefsovertime.Thisis behavior in reaction to five qualities of perceived likely to sustain their antisocial behavior bringing mothering—harassment, unreliable support, low griefnotonlyto their victimsbut also, ultimately,to monitoring, low affectionate contact, and intrusive themselves (e.g., via peer rejection and academic overprotectiveness. The first four of these perceived difficulties;Coie&Dodge,1998). maternalqualitieshavebeenshowntobeassociated No evidence was found for the disposition- (concurrently) with preadolescents’ avoidant stance activating hypothesis that high self-esteem encour- towardthemother(Yungeretal.,2005). ages children to act on their aggressive cognitions. The disposition-rationalizing hypothesis is that Severalfactorsmayhavecontributedtothefailureto high self-esteem leads avoidant children to justify confirmthishypothesis.First,childrenwithhighself- theiravoidanceoftheirmotherbystrengtheningtheir esteem who harbor aggressive cognitions may retal- perceptions of her as an inept, hostile, uncaring, iateimmediatelytoegothreatsbutmaynotincrease blameworthy parent who deserves the avoidant in trait level of aggressionover a school year; future treatment she is receiving. Adults with avoidant research might include assessment of aggression as romantic attachments, compared with those with animmediateresponsetoprovocation.Second,high more secure relationships, tend to self-enhance by self-esteem may cause children to act on aggressive psychologically distancing themselves from their cognitions only when their self-esteem is insecure. partners—by seeing them as bad and as different CostsofHighSelf-Esteem 1633 from themselves and by projecting unwanted traits and sensitive when needed for help or communica- onto them (Hart, Shaver, & Goldenberg, 2005; tion. The second dimension was perceived maternal Mikulincer & Horesh, 1999; Mikulincer, Dolev, & overprotectiveness (12 items) or children’s perception Shaver,2004).Suchstrategiespresumablyallowavoi- that the mother discourages exploration and other dant persons to cast themselves in a favorable and exciting activities (e.g., from fear that the child will superior light relative to their partner, thereby getsickorinjured).Thethirddimension(eightitems) protecting their self-esteem. We are suggesting that wasperceivedharassmentorchildren’sperceptionof high self-esteem intensifies these efforts of avoidant angry, rejecting, and humiliating behavior by the personstokeeptheirloftysenseofselfafloat. mother. The fourth dimension (six items) was per- ceivedaffectionatecontact(e.g.,themotherisviewedas participating with the child in joint recreational Method activities). The fifth dimension (six items) was per- Participants. Participants were 407 children (213 ceivedmaternalmonitoring(i.e.,thechildreportsthe girls,194boys)whowereinthefourthgradeatinitial mother as knowing the child’s whereabouts, activi- testing (mean age 5 11 years 1 month). Children ties, and companions). These last five scales were attended five relatively small elementary schools drawn from Finnegan et al. (1996) and Yunger et al. serving middle- and lower middle–class neighbor- (2005). Cronbach as for the foregoing measures, hoods in southeast Florida. Participating children respectively, for Time 1 (Time 2) were .72 (.77); .73 represented 67% of all fourth graders at the schools. (.78);.77(.76);.76(.80);and.67(.68). Thesampleincluded113Blackchildren,83Hispanic children, and 211 White children. The sample com- Results prisedtwocohorts,withCohort1(n5156)testedin thewintersof2001and2002andCohort 2(n5251) Intercorrelations of measures. Table 4 displays the testedinthewintersof2002and2003. associations among the measures with child sex, Procedure. Wheninthefourthgrade(Time1)and cohort, and ethnicity/race controlled (because the againinthefifthgrade(Time2),childrenresponded means of some measures differed with these varia- to self-report scales assessing self-esteem, avoidant bles).Avoidantattachmentandtheperceivedparent- attachment, and perceptions of the mother. Addi- ingvariablesshowedmoderate(andsimilar)degrees tional measures not relevant to the present report of stability over the 1-year period. Correlations werealsocollected.Childrenwereindividuallytested among the perceived parenting measures were gen- inaquietroomattheirschoolbyoneofseveralfemale erally modest to moderate. Self-esteem was associ- graduateassistantswhoreadtheitemstothechild. ated with other variables in ways that might be Measures. Self-esteem was assessed as in Study 1; expectedbasedonpreviousstudies. the Time 1 Cronbach a was .72. Avoidant attachment The disposition-activating hypothesis: does high self- wasassessedwitha10-itemscaleadaptedbyYunger esteem energize avoidance by children who perceive their etal.(2005)fromFinneganetal.’s(1996)originalscale. mother negatively? This hypothesis was evaluated in TheformatoftheitemswasthatdevelopedbyHarter five regression analyses, one for each perceived (1982)tominimizetheinfluenceofsocialdesirability maternal variable. The dependent variable was response bias. Each item described two kinds of alwaysTime2avoidance.Ineachanalysis,childsex, children—those engaging in avoidant behavior and cohort, ethnicity/race, and Time 1 avoidance were those engaging in a nonavoidant way. Children first entered on thefirst step. On the second step, Time 1 decidedwhichkindofchildrentheyresembledmore self-esteem and a Time 1 perceived parenting mea- and then indicated whether this choice was ‘‘sort of sure (e.g., harassment) were entered. On the third true’’or‘‘verytrue’’forthem.Theavoidantbehaviors step, the focal interaction of self-esteem and the capturedbytheseitemsweredescribedearlier.Scores perceivedparentingmeasurewastested.Supplemen- couldrangefrom1to4.Time1andTime2Cronbach tary analyses were run to see whether child sex or aswere.84and.85. cohortmoderatedanyfocaltwo-wayinteraction. Five dimensions of perceived maternal behavior Therewaslimitedsupportforthehypothesisthat were assessed in a separate questionnaire. The first self-esteem motivates avoidance by children who dimension was the mother’s provision of reliable view their mother negatively. In one of the supple- support(eightitems).Thiswasassessedusingashort- mentary analyses, the three-way interaction of Self- ened form of the Kerns’s Security Scale (Kerns, Esteem PerceivedMaternalMonitoring Sexwas " " Klepac, & Cole, 1996), which captures the degree to significant, F 5 8.04, p , .005. When a separate whichthecaregiverisperceivedasloving,accessible, regressionanalysiswasrunforeachsex,onlyforgirls 1634 Menonetal. Table4 CorrelationsAmongtheMeasuresofStudy2 Measure Measure 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 1.Self-esteem(Time1) 1.00 .26** .32** .14** .25** .33** .34** .09 .23** .08 .18** .21** .23** ! ! ! ! ! ! 2.Avoidance(Time1) 1.00 .49** .14** .32** .50** .38** .45** .31** .02 .26** .38** .35** ! ! ! ! ! ! ! 3.Reliablesupport(Time1) 1.00 .07 .51** .62** .39** .26** .41** .12* .31** .38** .31** ! ! ! ! ! 4.Overprotectiveness(Time1) 1.00 .22** .18** .11* .06 .04 .46** .06 .03 .04 ! ! ! ! 5.Harassment(Time1) 1.00 .54** .36** .19** .31** .28** .41** .33** .24** ! ! ! ! ! 6.Affectionatecontact(Time1) 1.00 .35** .29** .38** .19** .33** .43** .30** ! ! ! 7.Monitoring(Time1) 1.00 .24** .30** .06 .24** .29** .51** ! ! ! 8.Avoidance(Time2) 1.00 .57** .01 .51** .65** .52** ! ! ! ! 9.Reliablesupport(Time2) 1.00 .15** .58** .65** .50** ! ! 10.Overprotectiveness(Time2) 1.00 .29** .16** .08 ! ! 11.Harassment(Time2) 1.00 .55** .47** ! ! 12.Affectionatecontact(Time2) 1.00 .47** 13.Monitoring(Time2) 1.00 Note.TableentriesarefortheentiresampleofStudy2(N5407)andarepartialcorrelationscontrollingforsex,cohort,andethnicity/race. *p,.05.**p,.01. was the focal two-way interaction (Self-Esteem focal two-way interaction was examined separately " Monitoring) significant, F 5 10.68, p , .001. Follow- for each sex, it was significant for girls (F 5 7.97, up tests showed the predicted pattern: perceived p,.005)thoughnotforboys. maternal monitoring increasingly inhibited avoid- Each significant two-way interaction conformed ance as the level of girls’ self-esteem moved from to the predicted pattern: high self-esteem magnified low ( 1 SD) to medium (0 SD) to high (+1 SD), thecontributionofavoidantattachmenttoanegative ! respective bs 5 .07, ns; .10, ns; and .27, p , .002. view of the mother. Table 5 displays the results of ! ! Thus,girls(butnotboys)withhighself-esteemwho thefollow-uptests.Theseresultsareconsistentwith view their mother as unaware of their whereabouts, the notion that high self-esteem children rationalize activities, and companions reported greater avoid- theirantisocialconductviaaself-enhancingstrategy ance of their mother over time. However, high self- that disparages their adversary—in this case, their esteemdidnottransformothernegativeperceptions mother. ofthemotherintoavoidantbehaviortowardher. Table 6 tells the results of interaction follow- Thedisposition-rationalizinghypothesis:doeshighself- up analyses in which avoidance is treated as the esteem lead avoidant children to view their mother moderatoroftheimpactofself-esteemonperceptions negatively? This hypothesis was examined in five ofthemother.Fromtheentriesinthefirstcolumn,it regression analyses, with the Time 2 level of each can be discerned that high self-esteem is not prob- perceived parenting variable taking a turn as the lematicforchildrenwhodonotavoidtheirmothers; dependent variable. In each analysis, child sex, infact,forchildrenlowinmotheravoidance,higher cohort,ethnicity/race,andtheTime1perceivedpar- self-esteem appears to promote positive perceptions entingvariablewereenteredonthefirststep;Time1 ofthemother. self-esteemandTime1avoidancewereenteredonthe second step; and the focal interaction of self-esteem Discussion and avoidance was tested on the third step. Supple- mentary analyses examined whether child sex or AsinStudy1,therewassupportforthedisposition- cohort moderated any focal two-way interaction. rationalizing hypothesis: for children with high The focal two-way interaction (of self-esteem and self-esteem, an avoidant stance toward the mother avoidance) predicted change in perceived harass- forecastanincreasinglynegativeviewofthemother ment (F 5 6.98, p , .009) and perceived monitoring over time. For low-self-esteem children, avoidant (F510.25,p,.001).Also,thesupplementarythree- tendencies did not foretell disparagement of the way interaction of self-esteem, avoidance, and child mother. It is likely that avoidant children are locked sexwassignificant(F55.30,p,.03),andwhenthe in a power struggle with their mother. Those who CostsofHighSelf-Esteem 1635 Table5 RelationofTime1AvoidancetoTime2PerceptionofMotherasaFunctionofTime1Self-Esteem(Study2) Time1self-esteem Time2perceptionofmother Low( 1SD) Medium(0SD) High(+1SD) ! Harassment .04 .15** .26*** Monitoring .10y .22** .34*** ! ! ! Reliablesupport(girlsonly) .01 .16* .34** ! ! Note.TableentriesareunstandardizedbetacoefficientsfromtheAiken&West(1991)procedure.Withineachrow,thereissignificant variabilityamongthebetas. yp,.10.*p,.05.**p,.01.***p,.001. have high self-esteem may devalue their mother The aftermath of an avoidant attachment in pre- because this strategy discredits her, making her less adolescence warrants further investigation, espe- threatening,and,bycontrast,makingthemselvesfeel cially as affected by self-esteem levels. Given the morallysuperiorandblameless. escalation of negative attributions to the mother by Although results fit a disposition-justifying high-self-esteem avoidant children, these children hypothesis, other processes may have contributed. may be the quickest to trade the mother–child High self-esteem may cause avoidant children to relationship for relationships with peers, perhaps behave in ways that elicit real, not imaginary, deviant peers such as other avoidant and antisocial increased negative treatment by the mother, and the childrenlikethemselves(seeHodgesetal.,1999,for factthatavoidantchildrenwithhighself-esteemview evidence of homophilic attractions among avoidant their mother as monitoring them less and less over children).Low-self-esteemavoidantchildrenmaybe time may reflect more successful avoidance of their lesslikelytomakeaprematureexitfromthemother– motherbythesechildren.Highself-esteemmayalso child relationship, but they may be at greater risk cause avoidant children to perceive and remember for depression. However, because low-self-esteem maternal behaviors that are consistent with their avoidant children are less likely than their high- sensethattheirmothermaynotlovethemorispoised self-esteemcounterpartstogetcaughtupinavicious tofightthem.Itisalsopossiblethatavoidantchildren cycle of mutually exacerbating avoidance and nega- with high self-esteem are becoming more accurate tive perception of the mother, they may actually be over time in their perception of a mother who has themorelikelyeventuallytoabandontheiravoidant beenrejectingordisengagedallalong. relationshipstance. Manyoftheprocessesthatwehavespeculatedto Althoughhighself-esteemmayleadavoidantper- characterize avoidant children have been identified sonswhowantoutofarelationshiptomakenegative previously as qualities of avoidant individuals. The attributionsaboutapartner,highself-esteemmaylead contributionofthepresentfindingsistohighlightthe persons who are satisfied with a relationship to be novel (and somewhat counterintuitive) possibility especially forgiving of irksome behavior by their that high self-esteem magnifies, rather than mini- partner(e.g.,Murrayetal.,2002).Consistentwiththis, mizes, the relationship-undermining effects of an inthepresentstudy,childrenwhohadhighself-esteem avoidantattachment. butwerenotavoidant(i.e.,whoenjoyedtheirmother Table6 RelationofTime1Self-EsteemtoTime2PerceptionofMotherasaFunctionofTime1Avoidance(Study2) Time1avoidance Time2perceptionofmother Low( 1SD) Medium(0SD) High(+1SD) ! Harassment .18** .07 .05 ! ! Monitoring .17** .05 .07 ! Reliablesupport(girlsonly) .19* .02 .16 ! Note.TableentriesareunstandardizedbetacoefficientsfromtheAiken&West(1991)procedure.Withineachrow,thereissignificant variabilityamongthebetas. *p,.05.**p,.01. 1636 Menonetal. and used her as a secure base) developed the most findings. Several considerations suggest that this favorableperceptionsofher.Self-esteemmayinteract is unlikely. If shared method variance were respon- with relationship goals and partner perceptions to sible for associations among these variables, the affectrelationshipfunctioningandsatisfactioninsimi- disposition-activating hypothesis should have been lar ways across the age span. We hypothesize that if confirmed as often as the disposition-rationalizing adults who were once enamored with a relationship hypothesis; this was not the case. Moreover, the fact partner but decide they want to end the relationship that the disposition-rationalizing hypothesis clearly (i.e.,shifttowardanavoidantstance),highself-esteem received stronger support not only in this study but will make the exit easier for them; low-self-esteem alsoinStudy1(wherethemeasureswerenotallself- people may be more likely to remain longer in an reported)suggeststhatsomethingmorethanshared unsatisfyingrelationship. method variance underlies the pattern. Also, the There was slim support for the disposition- longitudinal design and regression analyses signifi- activating hypothesis that self-esteem would lead cantlyreducedthethreatofsharedmethodvariance. children to avoid a mother whom they perceive In these analyses, the Time 1 level of the outcome negatively. However, high-self-esteem girls were variable,whichisself-reported,wascontrolledonthe morelikelythangirlswithlowself-esteemtodismiss first step, and therefore self-report biases are effec- a mother perceived as low in monitoring. In this tively controlled (for elaboration of this point, see study, maternal monitoring was assessed as the Harold&Conger,1997;Kochenderfer&Ladd,1996; mother’s knowledge of the child’s conduct rather Steinberg,Lamborn,Dornbusch,&Darling,1992). than as the mother’s active attempts to control the child, and children who perceived their mother as lowinmonitoringmay simplynothavebeenspon- GeneralDiscussion taneously informing her of their activities, friends, and whereabouts (Kerr & Stattin, 2000; Stattin & Although high self-esteem surely makes people feel Kerr,2000).Thus,girlswithhighself-esteemwhoare good and may often carry developmental benefits disinclinedtocommunicatewiththeirmothersmay (e.g.,confidentpursuitofgoals),italsoappearssome- bethemostlikelytodevelopavoidanttendencies,at times to carry developmental costs. Previous work leastduringpreadolescence. had shown that people with high but insecure self- Traditionalattachmenttheoryholdsthatavoidant esteemarepronetoangerandaggression.Thepresent attachment derives from perceptions of parents as researchexploredtwoadditionalpossibledarksides unavailableorrejecting(Ainsworth,1979).Thismay ofhighself-esteem—thathighself-esteemencourages be true in infancy, but our data suggest that by children who harbor antisocial thoughts to act on preadolescence, the causal arrow between avoidant those thoughts (the disposition-activating hypoth- attachmentandnegativeperceptionsofparentsmay esis) and that it encourages children who behave run primarily in the opposite direction: avoidant in antisocial ways to rationalize their conduct, as attachment at Time 1 predicted deterioration in the by derogating their adversaries (the disposition- qualityofperceivedparenting,butperceivedparent- rationalizing hypothesis). Support was found for ing failed to forecast avoidant attachment (either as the latter, but not the former, hypothesis. For chil- a main effector ininteraction with self-esteem, with dren with high self-esteem, aggressive or avoidant the exception noted in the previous paragraph). tendencies led to the adoption of cognitions that Evidencefortheprimacyofself-views(i.e.,self-views couldjustifyprior(and,verylikely,future)antisocial precedingbeliefsabouthowothersfeelabouttheself) conduct. Over time, aggressive children with high has also been found for adolescents and adults self-esteem increasingly valued the rewards that (Cassidy, Ziv, Mehta, & Feeney, 2003; Kenny & aggression offers and devalued their victims, and DePaulo,1993;Sedikides&Skowronski,1995).Bypre- avoidantly attached children with high self-esteem adolescence,avoidantchildrenmayberejectingtheir increasingly viewed their mother as harassing and parentsratherthanreactingtotheirparents’rejection. uninvolved. Moreover, by strengthening their negative views of These results may be explained in a number of theirparents,theymaynolongerallowthemselvesto ways, but one interpretation is that high self-esteem feelloved. Ironically, this may be truer forhigh-self- leads children with antisocial proclivities to twist esteemchildrenthanforlow-self-esteemchildren. their perceptions of theirownand their adversaries’ Because in this study all the variables were self- behavior in self-serving, ego-protective ways. By reported,onemightwonderwhethersharedmethod belittling and blaming others, for example, they can variance(e.g.,responsebias,mood)accountedforthe feel better about themselves and can continue their

Description:
Over time, aggressive children with high self-esteem increasingly valued the rewards the sense that others harbor ill will toward them. (Salmivalli
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.