ebook img

The Design of Instruction and Evaluation: Affordances of Using Media and Technology PDF

232 Pages·2004·12.75 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview The Design of Instruction and Evaluation: Affordances of Using Media and Technology

THE DESIGN OF INSTRUCTION AND EVALUATION Affordances of Using Media and Technology This page intentionally left blank THE DESIGN OF INSTRUCTION AND EVALUATION Affordances of Using Media and Technology Edited by Mitchell Rabinowitz Fran C. Blumberg Fordham University Howard T. Everson The College Board LAWRENCE ERLBAUM ASSOCIATES, PUBLISHERS Mahwah, New Jersey London Copyright © 2004 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by photostat, microform, retrieval system, or any other means, without the prior written permission of the publisher. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers 10 Industrial Avenue Mahwah, New Jersey 07430 Cover design by Kathryn Houghtaling Lacey Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data The design of instruction and evaluation : affordances of using media and technology / edited by Mitchell Rabinowitz, Fran C. Blumberg, and Howard T. Everson. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN 0-8058-3762-0 (cloth : alk. paper) — ISBN 0-8058-3763-9 (pbk. : alk. paper) 1. Instructional systems—Design. 2. Learning, Psychology of. 3. Educational technology. I. Rabinowitz, Mitchell. II. Blumberg, Fran. III. Everson, Howard T. LB1028.38D47 2004 2004043288 CIP Books published by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates are printed on acid-free paper, and their bindings are chosen for strength and durability. Printed in the United States of America 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 21 Contents Introduction: The Design of Instruction and Evaluation: Psychological Foundations vii Mitchell Rabinowitz Part I Affordances of Media 1 Characteristics of Effective Materials for Informal Education: A Cross-Media Comparison of Television, Magazines, and Interactive Media 3 Shalom M. Fisch 2 Media Forms for Children's Learning 19 Sandra L. Calvert 3 Designing Multimedia Technology That Supports Human Learning 33 Richard E. Mayer Part II Affordances of Technology 4 Opportunities for Learning and Development in an After-School Computer Club 53 Mary Gauvain and Sharon Borthwick-Duffy v vi CONTENTS 5 Learning Affordances of Collaborative Software Design 77 Yasmin B. Kafai, Cynthia Carter Ching, and Sue Marshall 6 How the Internet Will Help Large-Scale Assessment Reinvent Itself 101 Randy Elliot Bennett 7 Technology, the Columbus Effect, and the Third Revolution in Learning 121 J. D. Fletcher Part III Affordances of Software 8 Asynchronous Learning in Graduate School Classes 143 Fran C. Blumberg, Meira Torenberg, and Lori M. Sokol 9 Hints in Human and Computer Tutoring 155 Rachel E. DiPaolo, Arthur C. Graesser, Douglas J. Hacker, Holly A. White, and the Tutoring Research Group 10 Intelligent Tutors Need Intelligent Measurement, or the Other Way 'Round 183 Howard T. Everson Author Index 205 Subject Index 213 INTRODUCTION: The Design of Instruction and Evaluation: Psychological Foundations Mitchell Rabinowitz This book is about the design of instructional and evaluation systems and the use and promise of media and technology within such systems. Successful design is a consequence of an interaction between art and science. Artistically, there are ways to present information that are pleasing and engaging. However, the artist might design a system that looks good, but does not work. In his book, The Design of Everyday Things, Norman (1988) presented a number of really interesting man- ufacturing designs that do not work (the contexts illustrated in that book are not instructional ones). Scientifically, I argue, there is a body of knowledge and prin- ciples that have been empirically tested that informs the design of instructional systems. However, the scientist might design instruction that should work, but is boring to interact with. Each is enhanced by the other. The chapters in this book are related more to the science than the art side, and they discuss and provide ex- amples of some of these knowledge bases and principles that are relevant to the design issue. In 1993, I edited a book entitled Cognitive Science Foundations of Instruction (Rabinowitz, 1993), which was also related to the topic of the design of instruc- tional and evaluation systems. The premise of that book was that the study of psychology provides the scientific underpinnings of successful design that im- proves learning. In this book, we continue with that premise. However, the psychological perspective presented within this volume significantly differs from that presented in the earlier volume given the developments and advancements within the field of psychology. vii Viii INTRODUCTION In this Introduction, I briefly overview three different perspectives on how psychology should inform the design of instructional and evaluation systems: the behavioral, cognitive, and affordances perspectives. I do not present these per- spectives as alternatives. Rather, I suggest they represent a development process in the advancement of knowledge and understanding. The cognitive perspective represents an advancement from the behavioral, and the affordances perspective represents an advancement over the cognitive perspective. Although not necessar- ily accepting the premises of the preceding, each still builds on the foundation al- ready developed. PSYCHOLOGICAL FOUNDATIONS The Behavioral Perspective The behavioral perspective emphasized the role of the environment in determining behavior. The basic assumption was that people (and other animals) learned to re- spond to environmental input in certain ways on the basis of contiguity and rein- forcement (Hulse, Deese, & Egeth, 1975). This perspective posits a causal connec- tion involving events in the environment, activity in the mind, and ultimately behavior (Flanagan, 1991). The behavioral assertion is that an event in the environ- ment causes something to happen in the mind, which then causes some behavior to occur—If A, then B, and then C. If the contiguity of these events occur regularly or lead to positive consequences, an association among A, B, and C is formed. The im- plication of this assertion is that if you knew or controlled A and an individual's learning history, you could predict or create C. To manipulate behavior (learning, in this context), there was no reason to investigate B or the mind. The behavioral per- spective never suggested that the mind was not part of the equation—just that you did not need to study it to predict or manipulate behavior. Thus, the mind became a black box in which information went in and behavior came out; what occurred in the black box was irrelevant. The primary emphasis was the relation between the environment and behavior. Thus, designers of instructional systems who adopted this perspective paid attention to setting up the environment and manipulating the consequences of different behavioral responses. The designs of teaching machines, programmed instruction, and behavior modification were a direct result of this ori- entation (Glaser, 1976; Skinner, 1968). The Cognitive Perspective Historically, the emphasis on cognitive processing arose as a reaction to the be- haviorist dogma, which emphasized the prominent role of the environment in de- termining behavior. Although there was extensive empirical evidence showing that people learned to respond to the environment, research also showed that the INTRODUCTION ix assumptions underlying the behavioral model were incorrect and that the model could only predict and account for a limited amount of behavior (Gardner, 1985). These studies showed that people did not respond in a direct causal way to that en- vironment—they responded to their representation or perception of the environ- ment. Hence, it was necessary to study how the mind processed information and the processing constraints the mind impose. Consequently, we had the cognitive revolution and the development of information-processing models of learning. The consequence of adopting this cognitive perspective was the development of instructional programs on the use of cognitive strategies. As Perkins and Grotzer (1997) stated, "... we often do not use our minds as well as it appears we could" (p. 1125). These programs were very successful (e.g., Pressley & Wolo- shyn, 1995). The chapters in the Rabinowitz (1993) edition were also written with this perspective in mind and illustrate a number of successful implementations of this perspective. The contrast between the behavioral and cognitive perspectives can be seen as a contrast between external and internal influences, in relation to the learner, on learning and performance. Whereas the behavioral approach emphasized the role of the environment (external to the individual) in determining behavior, the cogni- tive approach represented an advancement over the behavioral approach—it was not just an alternative that was deemed to be more appropriate. Specifically, the latter incorporated many ideas and empirical data from the behavioral research and added the mind into the equations. The cognitive approach clearly empha- sized the role of information processing (internal to the individual) in determining behavior. Bringing Back the Environment— The Affordance Approach I guess I have learned—during the intervening 10 years from the publication of my first book to this current book—that the definition of cognition offered earlier (the study of the mind) is too limiting. Cognitive psychology is not just the study of the mind—it is the study of the mind in interaction with the environment. One of the basic premises of the cognitive approach is that behavior is explained and manipulated by understanding how information is processed. Information can be defined in reference to knowledge and/or representation (internal to the person) or it can be defined in terms of the affordances within the environment (external to the person). Both sources of information—in combination and interaction—con- tribute to understanding learning and behavior. The term affordances was first introduced by J. J. Gibson (1977). Gibson de- fined affordances to mean features offered to the individual by the environment. Affordances exist relative to the capabilities of an individual—independent of the person's ability to perceive it and independent of the person's goals. The con-

Description:
This book is about empirically tested knowledge and principles that inform the design of instructional and evaluation systems, and the use and promise of media and technology within such systems. Historically, psychology has informed the design of instructional and evaluation systems in different wa
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.