ebook img

THE DARK TRIAD AND AGGRESSION The Dark Triad and Aggression in Early Adolescence PDF

31 Pages·2017·0.69 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview THE DARK TRIAD AND AGGRESSION The Dark Triad and Aggression in Early Adolescence

Running head: THE DARK TRIAD AND AGGRESSION The Dark Triad and Aggression in Early Adolescence: The role of Executive Functioning Nadine Luchsinger 559770 Supervisor: Jelle Sijtsema Second assessor: Carlo Garofalo Abstract This study considered the relation between the Dark Triad and self- and peer-reported direct and indirect aggression, and to what extent this relation depends on executive functioning (i.e. cognitive flexibility) in a group of 312 early adolescents (Mage = 14.42, SD = .97, 47% girls). Results from regression analyses showed that all Dark Triad traits were related to self-reported indirect aggression. Psychopathy and Machiavellianism both related to self-reported direct aggression, but only Machiavellianism related to both direct and indirect peer-reported aggression. These results suggest that Machiavellianism is a greater risk factor for aggression in adolescents of above average intelligence than perhaps even psychopathy. We did not find evidence that the associations between the Dark Triad traits and aggression were moderated by cognitive flexibility. Future studies might look into executive functioning and the Dark Triad in clinical samples and control groups. These samples should show a more diverse pattern of scores on the three traits as well as in executive functioning. Key words: aggression, the Dark Triad, cognitive flexibility, early adolescents The Dark Triad and Aggression in Early Adolescence: The role of Executive Functioning Direct and indirect aggressive behaviour are associated with a multitude of maladaptive behaviours, such as conduct problems, emotional dysregulation and peer-rejection, and internalising problems in the case of indirect aggression (Card, Stucky, Sawalani & Little, 2008). Direct aggression includes the use of physical and verbal aggression, e.g., kicking, hitting, pushing, calling names, etcetera. Indirect aggression includes gossiping, ridiculing and trying to damage another’s reputation (Björkqvist, Lagerspetz & Kaukiainen, 1992). Research into aggression in childhood and adolescence is extensive, as it is associated with a multitude of damaging behaviour such as conduct disorder and delinquency at a later age (Card et al. 2008; Carbone-Lopez, Esbensen, & Brick, 2010). In order to prevent such negative consequences at a later age, it is important to know more about aggression and its associations in early adolescence. The Dark Triad of personality, narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy (Paulhus & Williams, 2002) has been connected to different forms of aggressive behaviour in multiple studies (Baughman, Dearing, Giammarco, & Vernon, 2012; Muris, Meesters, & Timmermans, 2013; Kerig & Stellwagen, 2010). These three traits deserve attention, as they could be predictors of indirect and direct aggression. However, not all those with disagreeable personality characteristics show high rates of aggression. Klimstra, Sijtsema, Henrichs, and Cima (2014), for instance, did not find that all adolescents with narcissistic, Machiavellian or psychopathic traits displayed aggression. This study will seek to explain some of the mixed findings in the relation between Dark Triad traits and aggression by looking at another important factor regarding the display of aggressive behaviour: impulse control. This study examines whether the association between Dark Triad traits and aggression is stronger for those who demonstrate a lowered ability switch from one task to the next and control their impulses. To do so, I ask the following question: to what extent does the executive function cognitive flexibility moderate the relation between traits of the Dark Triad and indirect and direct aggression in young adolescents? The Dark Triad The Dark Triad consists of three subclinical traits: narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy. The construct of narcissism in the Dark Triad shares the facets of grandiosity, entitlement, dominance, and superiority with its clinical counterpart (Paulhus & Williams, 2002), but norms are established in ‘normal’ populations. (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001). The same applies to subclinical psychopathy. The clinical construct measures impulsivity and thrill-seeking along with low empathy and anxiety. Subclinical scales measure the same facets, but norms have been established outside of forensic settings (Paulhus, Hemphill, & Hare, 2012). Lastly, Machiavellianism is best explained as making habitual use of manipulation in interpersonal relations, while exploiting others in the process (Wilson, Near & Miller, 1996). Machiavellians in general population samples are more cold and manipulative. This can be seen when responding to, for instance, statements developed by Christie and Geis (1970). Though generally considered undesirable, Dark Triad traits can have their uses, especially in adolescence. Subclinical narcissism has been shown to be a protective factor against sadness, depression, loneliness, anxiety and neuroticism. It is also positively associated with subjective wellbeing (Sedikides, Rudich, Gregg, Kumashiro & Rusbult, 2004). Subclinical narcissism may serve adolescents who are coping with a multitude of physical changes, including those changes having to do with hormones and moods (Buchanan, Eccles & Becker, 1992). However, these subclinical traits may cease to be functional when they’re associated with antisocial behaviour (Chabrol, van Leeuwen, Rodgers & Séjourné 2009) such as violence perpetration (Pailing, Boon & Egan, 2014) bullying in adulthood (Baughman, Dearing, Giammarco & Vernon, 2012), animal cruelty (Kavanagh, Signal & Taylor, 2013) and aggression (Kerig & Stellwagen, 2010). The Dark Triad and its relation with two forms of aggression All three traits of the Dark Triad are related to aggression in one way or another. Jones and Paulhus (2010), for instance, found that different types of provocations elicited aggression in subclinical narcissists and psychopaths. Undergraduates with narcissistic traits responded aggressively when provoked by a threat to their ego, whereas undergraduates with psychopathic traits responded when threatened physically (Jones & Paulhus, 2010). Machiavellianism was not strongly related with direct (i.e., openly hostile, physical, or verbal) aggression (Jones & Paulhus, 2009). Machiavellianism was associated with relational aggression (which is similar to indirect aggression as defined by Björkqvist, Lagerspetz and Kaukiainen, 1992) in a study by Kerig and Stellwagen (2010). They considered the relation between Callous-Unemotional traits, narcissism, Machiavellianism, and physical and relational aggression in a group of middle-school children. Callous-Unemotional (CU) traits are considered a key part of psychopathy, and a few examples are a lack of empathy, a lack of guilt and limited, superficial emotions (Barry et al., 2000). Kerig and Stellwagen found that relational aggression associated most strongly with Machiavellianism, whereas physical aggression associated with CU and narcissism in the absence of Machiavellianism. The three traits share common variance, and so Kerig and Stellwagen’s results suggest that those aspects that are unique in CU and narcissism associate most strongly with physical aggression. The finding that Machiavellianism is associated with indirect aggression in both studies is not surprising, because Machiavellians use relational manipulation to achieve an end-goal (Wilson, Near & Miller, 1996) in a covert, ‘sneaky’ way (McIlwain, 2003). According to these studies, we might expect that psychopathy and narcissism are associated with direct aggression. However, Klimstra, Sijtsema, Henrichs and Cima (2014) found that Machiavellianism and narcissism were associated with indirect aggression, whereas psychopathy was associated with direct aggression. Other studies have found that narcissism is related to physical aggression, but not relational aggression (Kerig & Stellwagen, 2010). Klimstra et al.’s study considered the subclinical traits of the Dark Triad, as opposed to the clinical constructs which the other articles studied, which might also explain why narcissism was related to indict aggression in their study and not direct aggression as was found in the previous study. Another explanation might be found in unconsidered moderating variables, such as executive functioning. Brain development and EF One reason why a moderating role for executive functions is especially important in early adolescence is that these functions are not yet at a mature level. Chabrol, van Leeuwen, Rodgers, and Séjourné (2009) found that narcissistic, psychopathic and Machiavellian traits are more common in adolescents than in the student population. They propose that a lack of maturation plays a part and that dysfunctional personality traits may become less pronounced as adolescents grow older (Chabrol, van Leeuwen, Rodgers & Séjourné 2009). This maturation may be explained by on-going development of the brain in (early) adolescence and of one brain region in particular: the prefrontal cortex, which continues to develop into early adulthood (Welsh, Pennington & Groisser, 1991; Romine & Reynolds, 2005). There is a peak in the development of cortical grey matter of the frontal lobe in the preadolescent period, around the age of 12 (Giedd et al., 1999; see Lenroot & Giedd, 2006 for a review). Around this time synaptic pruning (the removal of infrequently used neural connections) takes place until mid-adolescence, which coincides with a period of improved information processing and logical reasoning (Steinberg, 2008). The frontal lobe, housing the prefrontal cortex, is believed to be related to the cognitive control of important functions, including executive functions (Miller & Cohen, 2001). These executive functions (EF) are defined by Zelazo and Carlson (2012) as top-down, neurocognitive processes concerned with conscious, goal-directed thoughts, actions, and emotions. Cognitive flexibility is one of these functions and will be discussed further on. Because the frontal lobe is still under construction during early adolescence, it is likely that the functions attributed to this area are not as well-developed in adolescents as in adults. Evidence that this is the case for EF comes from work by Huizinga, Dolan, and van der Molen (2006), who compared participants of varying ages on different EF-tasks. Their results provide insight into the average age at which different executive functioning skills are mastered at an adult level. They found that the scores on a shifting-task (used to index cognitive flexibility) no longer improved after the age of fifteen. Inhibition-tasks were at an adult-level at age eleven. Another study into the development of cognitive processes showed that cognitive flexibility increased with age, but was not yet at an adult level at age thirteen. Lastly, research in children and young adolescents using the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) test and a revised flanker test, (Zelazo et al. 2013) found that scores on cognitive flexibility and inhibition improved with age (Zelazo et al., 2013). All of these studies show that executive functions are still developing in early adolescence. These adolescents’ underdeveloped brain functions may have great influence on their behaviour. Moderation of the association between personality and aggression As was mentioned earlier, the subclinical traits of the Dark Triad can be functional, but cease to be when they are associated with antisocial behaviour (Chabrol, van Leeuwen, Rodgers & Séjourné 2009). Executive functioning might be a moderating factor in the difference between functional and dysfunctional traits of the Dark Triad. Adolescents with antisocial personality traits and poorer regulatory functions may be quicker to show aversive behaviour (e.g. bullying and other forms of direct aggression) than their higher functioning peers. Those peers with antisocial personality traits, but higher executive functioning may use their regulatory function to choose other - nonaggressive - behaviour to achieve their goals. Executive functions such as cognitive flexibility are crucial when attempting to stop automatic responses, such as aggression, in response to a threat. Studies into a possible moderation between traits of the Dark Triad and aggression are rare, but Ishikawa, Raine, Lencz, Bihrle and Lacasse, (2001) looked into the association between clinical psychopathy, criminal conviction and executive functioning. They labelled adult males who scored high on the PCL-R but had never been convicted of a crime ‘successful psychopaths’ and those who scored high and had been convicted ‘unsuccessful psychopaths’. When assessing their executive functioning the authors found that successful psychopaths’ executive functioning was higher than both unsuccessful psychopaths and controls. However, there was no difference between unsuccessful psychopaths and controls. This suggests that high executive functioning may serve as a protective factor for going to jail in psychopathic individuals. The participants of this study were adult males and so results cannot be generalised, but they are an indicator of what might be expected from this study. This study This study will examine traits of the Dark Triad, aggression and executive functioning in a group of young adolescents. Based on the above, it is expected that the Dark Triad traits will relate to aggression in the following way: i) narcissism relates positively to direct and indirect aggression, ii) Machiavellianism will relate positively to indirect aggression, but there will be no significant relation to direct aggression, and iii) psychopathy will only relate positively to direct aggression. We also expect that these associations are moderated by cognitive flexibility. Specifically, iv) adolescents scoring high on cognitive flexibility and showing high narcissistic tendencies, report less direct aggression than those with lower executive functions and high narcissistic tendencies; v) adolescents scoring high on cognitive flexibility and showing high psychopathic tendencies, will also report and show less direct aggression than those with lower executive functions and high psychopathic tendencies; vi) adolescents scoring high on cognitive flexibility and showing high Machiavellian tendencies, will report and show less indirect aggression than those with lower executive functions and high Machiavellian tendencies. All analyses will be performed while controlling for sex and age, because studies have shown that sex and age associate with aggression (Kerig & Stellwagen, 2010) and age also associates with executive functioning (Huizinga, Dolan and van der Molen, 2006; Davidson, Amso, Anderson and Diamond, 2006; Zelazo et al., 2013). Method Participants and study design Participants were recruited as part of an on-going longitudinal Study of Personality, Adjustment, Cognition, and Emotions among adolescents and their parents (SPACE-II). First, four schools gave their permission, after which parents and children were informed through an information letter. Parents of participants gave informed passive consent, children gave their active consent. Of the total of 1046 children recruited this way, 863 children (N = 461 girls) and (N = 402 boys) answered all the questions of the Dirty Dozen and the aggression scales. Their average age was 14.8 with a standard deviation of .97 and range from 12 to 16. Most children were in first (43%) or second (27%) grade, though some were in third (11%), fourth (13%) or even fifth (6%) grade. Most children studied at a HAVO/VWO level or higher (83%), indicating that they’re of higher intelligence that the average Dutch 14-year old. In this study, 312 girls (N = 146) and boys (N = 166) also completed the DCCS task next to the Dirty Dozen and aggression questionnaires. All of them were in first (66%) or second (33%) grade of a high school in Rotterdam or Den Haag. Their mean age was slightly lower 14.42 with a standard deviation of .97. All of the data were collected in June and December 2014. Measures Aggression. Self-reported aggression was measured using the Dutch version of the Direct & Indirect Aggression Scales (DIAS) (Björkqvist, Lagerspetz, & Kaukiainen, 1992). The subscales ‘direct aggression’ (5 items) and ‘indirect aggression’ (12 items) were used. Students could answer on a 4-point scale, ranging from ‘never’ to ‘very often’. The indirect and direct subscales had a Cronbach’s alpha of .92 and .86, respectively. Peer-reported aggression was measured using nominations: the students received a roster with the names of their classmates. The students could nominate classmates on two items: ‘quarrels or fights a lot with others’ (direct aggression) and ‘often gossips or excludes others’ (indirect aggression). The total amount of nominations was then divided by the number of classmates to calculate proportion scores of direct and indirect peer-reported aggression. These four measures of peer- and self-reported indirect and direct aggression will be used as dependent variables. Dark Triad personality traits. To measure Dark Triad personality traits, a Dutch version (Klimstra, Sijtsema, Henrichs & Cima, 2014) of the ‘Dirty Dozen’ (Jonason & Webster, 2010) was used. The English version was translated by two researchers into Dutch and then compared. Any differences were discussed and chosen by consensus. This version was then back-translated into English by another researcher who compared it to the original English version; they were very much alike (Klimstra, Sijtsema, Henrichs & Cima, 2014). Participants were asked to what extent they agree (from 1 ‘not at all’ to 5 ‘very much’) with twelve statements. The Dirty Dozen has three subscales, composed of four items each: narcissism (α = .84), Machiavellianism (α = .78), and psychopathy (α = .71). A composite of the three subscales is a measurement for the Dark Triad (α = .87) and the subscales are positively intercorrelated: rs .54 - .65, ps < .000. Executive functioning. Executive functions were measured using a Dutch, revised version of the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) task, which measures cognitive flexibility (Zelazo et al. 2013; Kirkham, Cruess, & Diamond, 2003; Farrant, Maybery, & Fletcher, 2012). The revised DCCS has shown excellent reliability and adequate to good discriminant validity as a measure of executive functioning (Zelazo et al. 2014). Fourteen children completed the task on laptops during school hours. Participants were seated behind a laptop in a quiet room. The experimenter (i.e., trained Psychology Master students) briefly explained the task, though most of the instructions were displayed on the computer screen at the beginning of each individual task. The DCCS starts with a written instruction on the display, after which there are two series of five practice trials. In the first series the objective was to, for example, sort the blue boat to the left and the red rabbit to the right; i.e. sorting by colour. In the second series, the objective was to sort by shape and ignore colour. When a mistake was made on the practice trials, they were given another try (with a maximum of three tries). If a mistake was made in the third round of practice trials, the test was concluded. The conditions were: (a) shape; sorting two stimuli by shape (5 trials), (b) colour: sorting the same stimuli by colour (5), and (c) the mixed condition; sorting the same stimuli by colour or shape (40). In all three conditions, the same pseudo-random order was used, that is: the same condition (colour or shape) was never offered more than two times in a row. Response time and the number of mistakes were automatically recorded. Statistical analyses Statistical analyses to answer the research questions were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics 21. To determine significance, a two-sided significance level of .05 was used. Direct and indirect, peer- and self-reported aggression were the dependent variables. The two types of aggression were measured using the DIAS self-report scale and using peer-nominations, which amounts to a total of four dependent aggression-variables. The individual traits of the Dark Triad were independent, continuous variables. EF was assumed to be a moderating variable and was also continuous. The DCCS task was scored using average accuracy (the number of mistakes) and the average response time (the average time it takes to react). The reaction time was then divided by the accuracy to create performance scores as in the study by Buss, Davis, Hobel and Sandman (2011). This means that higher scores are indicative of longer reaction times, i.e., worse performance, and lower scores indicate faster responses, i.e., better performance on the task. The associations were all between continuous variables, which means that tests of association will be used. Hierarchical multiple linear regression has a number underlying assumptions, including: multicollinearity, normality, linearity, homoscedasticity, and independence of residuals (Pallant, 2013). Multicollinearity was checked using the VIF and Tolerance outcomes from multiple regression. When performing multiple linear regression, residuals scatterplots were generated for the remaining assumptions and multicollinearity was checked in the statistics tab. Descriptives, i.e. means, standard deviations and ranges, were explored for all variables. For the DCCS scores, the first 40 trials (M = 782.39 SD = 218.36) were used, because participants

Description:
Rodgers & Séjourné 2009) such as violence perpetration (Pailing, Boon & Egan, 2014) bullying in adulthood (Baughman . regulatory functions may be quicker to show aversive behaviour (e.g. bullying and other forms of .. victimization: Gender differences in direct and indirect forms of bullying.
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.