THE BONOBO and THE ATHEIST In Search of Humanism Among the Primates FRANS DE WAAL with drawings by the author DEDICATION For Catherine, my favorite primate CONTENTS Cover Title Page Dedication CHAPTER 1: EARTHLY DELIGHTS CHAPTER 2: GOODNESS EXPLAINED CHAPTER 3: BONOBOS IN THE FAMILY TREE CHAPTER 4: IS GOD DEAD OR JUST IN A COMA? CHAPTER 5: THE PARABLE OF THE GOOD SIMIAN CHAPTER 6: TEN COMMANDMENTS TOO MANY CHAPTER 7: THE GOD GAP CHAPTER 8: BOTTOM-UP MORALITY Acknowledgments Notes Bibliography Index About the Author Picture Section Copyright Also by Frans De Waal Chapter 1 EARTHLY DELIGHTS Is man only a blunder of God? Or is God only a blunder of man? —Friedrich Nietzsche1 I was born in Den Bosch, the Dutch city after which Hieronymus Bosch named himself.2 This doesn’t make me an expert on the painter, but having grown up with his statue on the market square, I have always been fond of his surrealist imagery, his symbolism, and how it relates to humanity’s place in the universe under a waning influence of God. His famous triptych in which naked figures frolic around, The Garden of Earthly Delights, is a tribute to paradisiacal innocence. The middle tableau is far too happy and relaxed to fit the interpretation of depravity and sin advanced by puritan experts. It shows humanity free from guilt and shame either before the Fall or without any Fall at all. For a primatologist like myself, the nudity, the allusions to sex and fertility, the plentiful birds and fruits, and the moving about in groups are thoroughly familiar, and hardly in need of a religious or moral interpretation. Bosch seems to have depicted us in our natural state, while reserving his moralistic outlook for the right-hand panel, in which he punishes not the frolickers from the middle panel but monks, nuns, gluttons, gamblers, warriors, and drunkards. Bosch was no fan of the clergy and their avarice, which explains a small detail in which a man resists signing his fortune away to a pig veiled like a Dominican nun. The poor figure is said to be the painter himself. Five centuries later, we remain embroiled in debates about the place of religion in society. As in Bosch’s day, the central theme is morality. Can we envision a world without God? Would this world be good? Don’t think for one moment that the current battle lines between fundamentalist Christianity and science are determined by evidence. One has to be pretty immune to data to doubt evolution, which is why books and documentaries aimed at convincing the skeptics are a waste of effort. They are helpful for those prepared to listen, but fail to reach their target audience. The debate is less about the truth than about how to handle it. For those who believe that morality comes straight from God the creator, acceptance of evolution would open a moral abyss. Listen to the Reverend Al Sharpton debating the late atheist firebrand Christopher Hitchens: “If there is no order to the universe, and therefore some being, some force that ordered it, then who determines what is right or wrong? There is nothing immoral if there’s nothing in charge.”3 Similarly, I have heard people echo Dostoevsky’s Ivan Karamazov, exclaiming, “If there is no God, I am free to rape my neighbor!” Perhaps it’s just me, but I am wary of any persons whose belief system is the only thing standing between them and repulsive behavior. Why not assume that our humanity, including the self-control needed for a livable society, is built into us? Does anyone truly believe that our ancestors lacked social norms before they had religion? Did they never assist others in need, or complain about an unfair deal? Humans must have worried about the functioning of their communities well before current religions arose, which occurred only a couple of millennia ago. Biologists are unimpressed by that kind of timescale. In the lower right-hand corner of The Garden, Bosch depicted himself resisting a pig dressed like a nun, who tries to seduce him with kisses. She is offering salvation in return for his estate (hence the pen, ink, and official-looking paper). The Garden was painted around 1504, about a decade before Martin Luther galvanized protest against such church practices. The Dalai Lama’s Turtle The above introduced a blog entitled Morals without God? on the New York Times’ website, in which I argued that morality antedates religion and that much can be learned about its origin by considering our fellow primates.4 Contrary to the customary blood- soaked view of nature, animals are not devoid of tendencies that we morally approve of, which to me suggests that morality is not as much of a human innovation as we like to think. This being the topic of the present book, let me lay out its themes by describing the week that followed my blog’s publication, including a trip to Europe. Right before this, however, I attended a meeting between science and religion at Emory University, in Atlanta, where I work. The occasion was a forum with the Dalai Lama on his favorite theme: compassion. Being compassionate seems to me an excellent recommendation for life; hence I welcomed the message of our honorable guest. As the first discussant, I was seated next to him surrounded by a sea of red and yellow chrysanthemums. I had been instructed to address him as “your holiness,” but to speak of him to others as “his holiness,” which I found sufficiently confusing that I avoided all forms of address. One of the most admired men on the planet dropped his shoes and folded his legs under him in his chair, put on a huge baseball cap color-matched to his orange robe, while an audience of over three thousand people hung on his every word. Before my presentation, I had been appropriately deflated by the organizers’ reminding me that no one had come to hear me speak, and that all those people were there only for his pearls of wisdom. In my remarks, I reviewed the latest evidence for animal altruism. For example, apes will voluntarily open a door to offer a companion access to food, even if they lose part of it in the process. And capuchin monkeys are prepared to seek rewards for others, as we see when we place two of them side by side, while one of them barters with us with differently colored tokens. One token rewards only the monkey itself, whereas the other rewards both monkeys. Soon, the monkeys prefer the “prosocial” token. This is not out of fear, because dominant monkeys (who have least to fear) are in fact the most generous. Good deeds also occur spontaneously. An old female, Peony, spends her days outdoors with other chimpanzees at the Yerkes Primate Center’s field station. On bad days, when her arthritis is flaring up, she has trouble walking and climbing, but other females help her out. Peony may be huffing and puffing to get up into the climbing frame in which several apes have gathered for a grooming session. But an unrelated younger female moves behind her, placing both hands on her ample behind to push her up with quite a bit of effort, until Peony has joined the rest. We have also seen Peony get up and slowly move toward the water spigot, which is at quite a distance. Younger females sometimes run ahead of her, take in some water, then return to Peony and give it to her. At first, we had no idea what was going on, since all we saw was one female placing her mouth close to Peony’s, but after a while the pattern became clear: Peony would open her mouth wide, and the younger female would spit a jet of water into it. Such observations fit the emerging field of animal empathy, which deals not only with primates but also with canines, elephants, and even rodents. A typical example is how chimpanzees console distressed parties, hugging and kissing them, which is so predictable that we have documented literally thousands of cases. Mammals are sensitive to each other’s emotions and react to those in need. The whole reason people fill their homes with furry carnivores, and not with, say, iguanas and turtles, is that mammals offer something no reptile ever will. They give affection, they want affection, and they respond to our emotions the way we do to theirs. Up to this point, the Dalai Lama had listened attentively, but now he lifted his cap to interrupt me. He wanted to hear more about turtles. These animals are a favorite of his, because they supposedly carry the world on their backs. The Buddhist leader wondered whether turtles, too, know empathy. He described how the female sea turtle crawls onto land to look for the best spot to lay her eggs, thus showing concern for future young. How would the mother behave if she ever encountered her offspring? the Dalai Lama wondered. To me, the process suggests that turtles have been preprogrammed to seek out the best environment for incubation. The turtle digs a hole in the sand above the tide line, deposits her eggs and covers them, packing the sand tight with her rear flippers, and then leaves the nest behind. The hatchlings emerge a few months later to rush to the ocean under the moonlight. They never get to know their mother. Empathy requires awareness of the other and sensitivity to the other’s needs. It probably started with parental care, like that found in the mammals, but there is also evidence for bird empathy. I once visited the Konrad Lorenz Research Station, in Grünau, Austria, which keeps ravens in large aviaries. These are impressive birds, especially when they sit on your shoulder with their powerful black beak right next to your face! It brought back memories of the tame jackdaws I had kept as a student: much smaller birds from the same corvid (crow) family. In Grünau, scientists follow spontaneous fights among the ravens and have seen bystanders respond to distress. Losers can count on some cozy preening or beak-to-beak nudging from their friends. At the same station, free-ranging descendants of Lorenz’s flock of geese have been equipped with transmitters to measure their heart rate. Since every adult goose has a mate, that offers a window on empathy. If one bird confronts another in a fight, its partner’s heart starts racing. Even if the partner is in no way involved, its heart betrays concern about the quarrel. Birds, too, feel each other’s pain. If both birds and mammals have some measure of empathy, that capacity probably goes back to their reptilian ancestors. Not just any reptiles, though, because most lack parental care. One of the surest signs of a caring attitude, according to Paul MacLean, the American neuroscientist who named the limbic system the seat of the emotions, is the “lost call” of young animals. Young monkeys do it all the time: left behind by mom, they call until she returns. They look miserable, sitting all alone on a tree limb, giving a long string of plaintive “coo” calls with pouted lips directed at no one in particular. MacLean noted the absence of the “lost call” in most reptiles, such as snakes, lizards, and turtles. In a few reptiles, however, the young do call when upset or in danger, so that mom will take care of them. Have you ever held a baby alligator? Be careful, because they have a good set of teeth, but they also utter throaty barks when upset, which may bring the cow (mother) flying out of the water. That will teach you to doubt reptilian feelings! I mentioned this to the Dalai Lama, saying that we expect empathy only in animals with attachments, and that few reptiles qualify. I am not sure this satisfied him, because of course he wanted to know about turtles, which look so much cuter than those ferocious toothy monsters of the Crocodilian family. Appearances are deceptive, though. Some members of this family gently transport their young in their big jaws or on their backs and defend them against danger. They sometimes even let them snatch pieces of meat from their mouth. The dinosaurs, too, cared for their young, and plesiosaurs—giant marine reptiles—may even have been viviparous, giving birth to a single live offspring in the water, as whales do today. From everything we know, the smaller the number of offspring an animal produces, the better it will take care of them, which is why plesiosaurs are thought to have been doting parents. So, by the way, are birds, which science regards as feathered dinosaurs. Pressing me even further, the Dalai Lama jumped to butterflies and asked about their empathy, upon which I couldn’t resist joking, “They don’t have time, they live just one day!” The short life of butterflies is actually a myth, but whatever these insects feel about each other, I doubt it has much to do with empathy. This is not to minimize the larger thrust behind the Dalai Lama’s question, which was that all animals do what is best for themselves and their offspring. In this sense, all life is caring, perhaps not consciously caring, but caring nonetheless. He was getting at the idea that compassion goes to the root of what life is all about. Few reptiles have parental care, but the crocodile family does. A female alligator safely transports one of her young. Greeting Mama After this, the forum moved on to other topics, such as how to measure compassion in the brains of Buddhist monks who have meditated on it all their lives. Richard Davidson from the University of Wisconsin related how monks straight from Tibet balked at his invitation to submit to neuroscience since, clearly, compassion didn’t take place in the brain but in the heart! Everyone felt this was hilarious, and the monks in the audience shrieked with laughter. But the monks had a point. Davidson subsequently discovered the connection between mind and heart: compassion meditation brings about a quicker heart rate upon hearing sounds of human suffering. I had to think of the geese. But I also sat there wondering at this auspicious meeting of minds. In 2005, the Dalai Lama himself had spoken about the need to integrate science and religion, telling thousands of scientists at the annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, in Washington, how much trouble society has in keeping up with their groundbreaking research: “It is all too evident that our moral thinking simply has not been able to keep pace with such rapid progress in our acquisition of knowledge and power.”5 What a refreshing departure from attempts to drive a wedge between religion and science! This topic was on my mind as I prepared for Europe. I had barely received a blessing and a khata (a long white silk scarf) around my neck, and seen the Dalai Lama off in his limousine with heavily armed guards, and I was on my way to Ghent, a beautiful old city in the Flemish part of Belgium. This region is culturally closer to the southern part of the Netherlands, where I am from, than the part to the north that we call Holland. All of us speak the same language, but Holland is Calvinist, whereas the southern provinces were kept Catholic in the sixteenth century by the Spanish, who brought us the Duke of Alva and the Inquisition. Not the silly “Nobody expects the Spanish Inquisition!” of Monty Python, but one that would put actual thumbscrews on you if you so much as doubted Mary’s virginity. Not allowed to draw blood, the inquisitors loved the strappado, or reverse hanging, in which a victim is hung by wrists tied behind his back and a weight is attached to his ankles. This treatment is sufficiently debilitating that one soon abandons any preconceived notions about the link between sex and conception. Lately, the Vatican has been on a campaign to soften the Inquisition’s image—they did not kill every heretic, they followed Standard Operating Procedures—but the Jesuits in charge surely could have used some compassion training. This ancient history also explains, by the way, why one will look in vain for Bosch paintings in the lowlands. Most hang in the Prado, in Madrid. It is thought that the Iron Duke obtained The Garden when, in 1568, he declared the Prince of Orange an outlaw and confiscated all of his properties. The duke then left the masterpiece to his son, from whom it went to the Spanish state. The Spanish adore the painter they call El Bosco, whose imagery inspired Joan Miró and Salvador Dalí. On my first visit to the Prado, I could not really enjoy Bosch’s work, since all I could think was “Colonial plunder!” To its credit, the museum has now digitized the popular painting at an incredibly high resolution so that everyone can “own” it through Google Earth. After my lecture in Ghent, fellow scientists took me on an impromptu visit to the world’s oldest zoo collection of bonobos, which started at Antwerp Zoo and is now located in the animal park of Planckendael. Given that bonobos are native to a former Belgian colony, their presence in Planckendael is hardly surprising. Bringing specimens from Africa, dead or alive, was another kind of colonial plunder, but without it we might never have learned of this rare ape. The discovery took place in 1929, in a museum not far from here, when a German anatomist dusted off a small round skull labeled as that of a young chimp, which he recognized as an adult with an unusually small head. He quickly announced a new subspecies. Soon his claim was overshadowed, however, by the even more momentous pronouncement by an American anatomist that we had an entirely new species on our hands, one with a strikingly humanlike anatomy. Bonobos are more gracefully built and have longer legs than any other ape. The species was put in the same genus, Pan, as the chimpanzee. For the rest
Description: