ebook img

Territoriality in Notiothemis robertsi Fraser (Anisoptera: Libellulidae) PDF

11 Pages·1996·1.8 MB·English
by  ClausnitzerV
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Territoriality in Notiothemis robertsi Fraser (Anisoptera: Libellulidae)

Odonatologica25(4):335-345 December 1, 1996 Territoriality inNotiothemisrobertsi Fraser (Anisoptera: Libellulidae)* V.Clausnitzer Kirchweg 5,D-35043 Marburg,Germany ReceivedFebruary 20, 1996/Revised andAcceptedMay8, 1996 The study was carriedoutat small pondsin theKakamegaForest, arain forestin WestKenya.89 6 i weremarkedindividuallyand territorial onesobserved foratotal of3171 min.Malesbehaveterritoriallyforameanof 14days,amaximumof45 days. Intruders aredrivenoutbytheresident anddonotshow offensivebehaviour. Different activities ofterritorial c?<? aredistinguished.Mostofthe time themale spendsperch- inginthe territory(32%oftotal timein territory).Sunflightstothe tree tops arethe most frequentflightactivity. INTRODUCTION The behaviouroftropical Libellulidaehasattractedmoreandmoreattentionin the recent years (e.g. MILLER, 1983,MILLER& MILLER, 1985, 1991;PARR, 1980;SORIANO, 1987).Thereis still alackof informationforspecies inhabiting tropical rain forests and only a few authors have paid attentionto them(e.g. CORBET, 1962; LEGRAND, 1979; LEMPERT, 1988; MILLER, 1993, 1995; PARR, 1980).Thispaperdealswiththesofarscarcely studiedrainforestLibellulidae (Tetratheminae) NotiothemisrobertsiFraser, 1944. Therangeofterritorialbehaviourinmaledragonflies is wide,butseems tohave mainly onecommonpurpose: theaccess tofemales(EMLENetal„ 1977).Territo- rialityisalso describedas astrategy tominimiseintraspecificaggression (POETHKE & KAISER, 1987).Agrowing numberofmales atthemating places canleadtoan intensificationof territorial behaviour, e.g. in Acisoma panorpoides inflatum (HASSAN, 1978), Nesciothemis nigeriensis (PARR, 1983a), Corduliaaenea * Dedicatedtothe memory ofthe lateDr PeterL. MILLER 336 V Clausnitzer amurensis(UBUKATA, 1975)andAeshna cyanea(POETHKE&KAISER, 1987) or to a collapse oftheterritorial system, e.g.in Libellulasaturata (DEBANG, 1993). Theexpression ofterritorialbehaviouris influencedby frequency, numberand availability ofreceptive females.Further thelife span ofthe individualsandthe possible occurrence ofseasonality influencesterritoriality.Specieswithavery lim- itedtemporal appearanceatthemating places will spendmoreenergyinshorttime actions than those with less limitedmating periods. Thelatter wefindmainly in tropical rain forests, because ofthelack ofastrong seasonaleffect (GAMBLES, 1960).Thecostsandthebenefitsanindividualmaleexperiences willhelpto deter- minetheevolutionofa specific territorialbehaviour(BROWN, 1964). Muchhasbeen writtenabouttheestablishmentofterritoriesin dragonflies (e.g. ALCOCK, 1987;BICK&BICK 1965;CAMPANELLA&WOLF,1974;HARVEY & HUBARD, 1987;MILLER, 1983;PAJUNEN, 1966).Someauthors,e.g.MOORE (1987),andPARR (1980, 1983b) give quantitative analyses ofmaleterritoriality. This study is focussed onterritorialactsofmaleNotiothemisrobertsi,giving con- siderationstothepurposeandfunctionalexplanations ofthem. Theterm“territory” inthispaperis usedaccording tothedefinitionofNOBLE (1935) as “any defendedarea”. The territories are established by the males near waterbodiesto gain access tofemales.Theterritorialperiodof thestudiedspecies differsfromother dragonflies in length, a reduced intraspecific aggression anda low mating success (PARR, 1983a).The territorialmaleshows two majoractivi- ties: perching andflying(definitions inMAY, 1976). Thelatteris furtherdivided into patrolling, interspecific, intraspecific, inspection, sexual and sun flight(Tab. I). Feeding doesnot occurin orneartheterritories(MOORE, 1987). N.robertsibreedsinsmallshady pools inrainforests, isunspectacular andeasily overlooked. Theonly detaileddescriptionsofbehaviourandecology are given by LEMPERT(1988). Thestudy sitein theKakamega Forestwas smallandobserva- tionswereeasy tomake.Atthesametimetwo otherspecies oftheTetratheminae- NotiothemisjonesiRis, TetrathemiscorduliformisLongfield -occurred atthepools. Allthree species are similar inbody shape, colourandbehaviour. METHODS Notiolhemis robertsi wasstudied intheKakamegaForest(00°08' -00°24’N,34°20'- 34°33’E;alt. 1500-1700 m), WestKenya,at a small pond complex formed by gold-diggersalongtheLugusida River. 89 males weremarked individuallywith numbers (permanentpen EDDING 780 /silver) and released immediatelyafter capture. Placing theanimals with its legs onvegetationthey showed no escape reactions andterritorial males resumed normal behaviour intheirterritories immediatelyafter capture (PARR & PARR, 1974). The marked dragonfliescould be recognisedeasily with a short- -focus binocular (8 x 30) withoutcatchingthe animals again. Observations were made between 2 December 1994and 10February 1995 on56 daysbetween 10:00and 16:00hlocaltime, whenthe maleswereactive attheponds.Astop-watchand adictatingmachine wereusedtotime anddescribe Territorialityin Notiothemis robertsi 337 the observed activities. Some behavioural patterns were filmed (PANASONIC, NV-GIE) and ana- lysedlater.Forty males wereobserved foratleast 20minuteseach (max.265 min; total: 3,171 min). The flighttypes wereclassified into patrolling,interspecific,intraspecific,inspection, sexualand sun flight(Tab. I).Each ofthesebehaviours is distinguishableby flight. Table I Classification ofdifferent flight componentsin territorial maleNotiothemisrobertsi BBeehhaavviioouurr IInniittiiaattiioonn OOrriieennttaattiioonn DDeessccrriippttiioonn PPaattrroolllliinngg SSppoonnttaanneeoouuss IInnddeeffiinniitteeiinnssiiddeetthhee SSllooww;;hhoovveerriinngg,, sshhoorrtt tteerrrriittoorryy ddiissttaanncceeoovveerrtthhee wwaatteerr IInntteerrssppeecciiffiicc c6?6d ooffootthheerrtteerrrriittoorriiaall TToowwaarrddssootthheerr mmaallee RRaappiidd,, ddiirreeccttaapppprrooaacchhffrroomm ssppeecciieess,, mmaaiinnllyyOOrrtthheettrruumm ssiiddeeoorrbbeellooww,,ssoommeettiimmeess sstteemmmmaallee ccllaasshheess IInnttrraassppeecciiffiicc i66i ooffNN..rroobbeerrttssii TToowwaarrddss iinnttrruuddeerr RRaappiidd,, ddiirreeccttaapppprrooaacchhffrroomm ssiiddeeoorrbbeellooww,,rraarreellyyccllaasshheess IInnssppeeccttiioonn MMoovveemmeennttooffoobbjjeeccttsswwiitthh TToowwaarrddss oobbjjeeccttbbeeiinngg DDiirreecctt,,hhoovveerriinnggbbeeffoorree ssiimmiillaarrssiizzeeooff22 iinnvveessttiiggaatteedd oobbjjeecctt NN.. rroobbeerrttssii SSeexxuuaall fflliigghhtt 22NN..rroobbeerrttssii,, TToowwaarrddss ccoonnssppeecciiffiicc RRaappiiddaanndd ddiirreecctt,, NN..jjoonneessii,,TTeettrraatthheemmiiss ffeemmaalleess,,wwhhiicchh ddoonnoott ccaappttuurree ooffffeemmaallee ccoorrdduulliiffoorrmmiiss sshhoowweegggg--llaayyiinngg bbeehhaavviioouurr SSuunn fflliigghhtt ??LLoonnggsshhaaddyyppeerriiooddss TToowwaarrddss ttrreeeeccaannooppiieess DDiirreecctt aanndd lliinneeaarr ttoowwaarrddss ttrreeeettooppss;;mmaaiinnllyyuussiinnggtthhee ssaammeerroouuttee Sun flights interruptedthe observationperiod,hence theindividuals could notbe observed any longer. SometimesIcould followamalewith thebinoculars and foundthemperchingin sunspotsin the tree tops. In order to determine how new arrivingmales establish territories, Iremoved males, which had beenterritorialforseveral daysbefore. Thiswaseasy,because sizeandlocation oftheterritoriesnever changed.Experiments with the “Fishing-line”techniquewerecarried outon territorial males. In this studyIused livingspecimen ofnormal maleswhich werepresentedtoterritorial males. MalesofN. robertsi showednoreaction to dead individuals presentedtothem.Theexperimentalinsectsweretied with acottonthread, attached between the fore and hindwings and tying the hind legs. A detailed descriptionofthisapplicationofthe “Fishing-line”techniqueisgiveninMOORE (1952). Duetothe small size ofN. robertsi the “Fishing-line”techniquewas difficulttoapply(PAJUNEN, 1964a),The experimentalinsects sufferedfromthetiedcottonthreadand duringtheexperiments,e.g.thewingsor wingveinsbroke. Henceonly a limited numbef(n= 10) ofexperimentswerecarried outat different timesofthe day. RESULTS Seventy-one percentofthe89markedmalescouldbeobserved atleastasecond timeand56% ofthemaleswere seenmore than twice attheponds. Despite spe- cificsearches, noneofthemarkedanimalswererecordedatotherponds.Thelong- estperiod betweenfirstandlastobservationofamature malewas 62 days(Fig. I). 338 V.Clausnitzer Males were territorialonly forone time intheir lifespan. Forty-five percent of the markedmalesobtainedaterritory duringthe observationperiod. The length of theterritorialperiod was variable,butcouldextendto43 days.Aftertheterritorial time the males were seen again for amaximum ofone more day. SometimesI foundamaledeadinthewater ofaterritoryithadoccupied (Fig. 1). Fig. 1. Periodspent as aterritorial male and total observation lengthofsome individuallynum- bered males ofNotiothemis robertsi. Malesarrivedinaterritory(single roundpools; ca.2min diameter)only inthe morning (10:00-11:30 h).Newarriving malesneversucceededin winning territo- rial interactions.Long and aggressive fights couldbeobserved only on veryrare occasions and then it was always the resident who won. A territory, where the current owner was removedat 11:30h or laterremainedempty fortherest ofthe day. The results fromthe “fishing-line” experiments showed in over 50%ofthe tests anattack fromtheresidentmale.Theattacks were very shortand couldnever be provoked a second time. No evidence forsexually motivated flights towards tiedanimals were observed. Territorialmales startedactivities at theponds between 10:00and 11:00h. In cloudy weatherthey disappeared tothetree-tops.They perched on smallvantage- -points, whichallowed a good viewoftheirterritory. Fromthispoint they started theirflights. Thedetailedobservation of the maleno. 4from 10:00-14:00hon 5 TerritorialityinNotiothemis robertsi 339 TableII Flightactivities ofonemale Notiothemisrobertsi (No.4)inhisterritoryon5-XII-1994; at 13;30h clouds appearedand at 14:00hthe male left the territorybecause ofcloudiness h 10:00- 10:30- 11:00- 11:30- 12:00- 12:30- 13:00- 13:30- 10:00- 10:30 11:00 11:30 12:00 12:30 13:00 13:30 14:00 14:00 Totaltimespendin 5.67 5.07 12.52 12.67 11.48 29 24.8 20.07 121.29 territoryin min No. offlightsout of 7 14 17 16 11 0 5 5 75 territory Mean time in territory48.57 21.79 44.18 47.5 62.64 1740 297.6 240.8 97.03 ins.(min/max) (5/128) (8/42) (5/101) (5/99) (52/91) (-/-) (273/150)(137/400(5/401) Total flightin timein s, 29 48 74 57 86 30 36 34 394 (%) (8.53) (15.58) (9.85) (7.5) (12.86) (1.72) (2.42) (2.82) (5) No. offlights 6 13 22 14 23 15 18 17 128 (permin) (1.06) (2.56) (176) (11) (2) (0.97) (0.73) (0.85) (104) Meanduration ofeach 4.83 3.69 3.36 4.07 3.74 2 2 2 3.08 flightins.(min/max) (1/12) (2/6) (1/5) (1/9) (1/16) (1/3) (1/5) (1/5) (1/16) Patrol 3 6 18 II II - 4 7 60 (%) (50) (46.15) (81.12) (78.57) (47.83) (22.22) (46.67) (46.88) Aggressive flights - 3 2 - 1 - 1 1 8 (%) (23.08) (9.09) (4.35) (5.56) (5.88) (6.25) Inspection 3 4 2 3 9 15 II 9 56 (%) (50) (30.77) (9.09) (21.43) (39.13) (100) (61.11) (52.94) (43.75) Otherflights(sx: - - - 2(a) - 2(sx) - 2(sx) sexual; a:avoiding) 2(a) %ofpoolsunny - - 5-10 - - - 10 5 - Decembergives animpression oftheactivitiesofN. robertsi (Tab.II). Allobservationsofthevari- ous flightactivitiesofterrito- rial males were totalled and setin relationto therespec- tivehour.Thisgaveageneral diagram ofthe daily routine concerning the flight-behav- iour a male of N. robertsi showed in his territory (Fig. 2).Inthecourseofa day im- Fig. 2. Average number ofthe flightactivities per hour of portance of different flight territorial male Notiothemis robertsi in histerritorybetween activities of territorial N. 10:00-16:00 h (n= 1691;in brackets n perh). 340 V. Clausnitzer robertsishifted.Patrolling and interspecific flights decreasedandthelatterdisap- peared intheafternoon,whileinspection flightsandsexualflights increased. Focussing on thepossible functionsofthe differentbehaviouralacts I counted thetotal numbersofeachbehaviouras afirst or asecond act (Tab. III). Themost commonbehaviourwas perching with32% followedby sunflights with 28%and patrolling with 27% of the observed behaviours. Inspection, intraspecific, interspecific andsexual flightsoccurred less frequent. Table III Two-act sequencesinthebehaviour ofterritorialmalesofNotiothemisrobertsi. - [Thenumbers show theobserved action followingadistinctprecedingbehaviour (n=2777)] Followingaction Preceding Perch Sun Patrol Inspec- Inter- Intra- Sexual Total behaviour flight tion specific specific flight Perch _ 489 203 142 151 51 16 1052 Sun flight 396 - 270 - 18 17 - 701 Patrol 333 84 - 1 22 11 1 452 Inspection 90 25 7 - 7 - - 129 Interspecific 240 80 14 4 - 3 - 341 Intraspecific 16 15 28 - 1 - - 60 Sexual flight 10 7 2 - - - 23 42 Total 1085 700 524 147 199 82 40 2777 Over forty-onepercentofallflights were“sun flights” to sunny spots inor near thetree tops(Fig. 2,Tab.III). The time a male spent con- tinuousatafullsunnyor afull shady perch in the territory beforeleaving forasun flight was totalled and compared (Fig. 3).As thisis a very sub- jective methodIdid not con- sider perched males in twi- Fig.3.PercentageoftimeofamaleNotiolhemisrobertsispent lightor wherethe exposition at full sunny (n= 247) orfull shadyperches (n=86) before changed duringtheperching leavingforasunflight.The totaltimespent at each position time. correspondsto 100%. DISCUSSION Notiothemisrobertsidoesnotshowseasonality andlarval development lasts two months (LEGRAND, 1977).Thisfits theobservationsfor manytropical species, wheretheadultlifeexceeds thelarvallife (GAMBLES, 1960).Thelong life-span andthelack ofseasonality isassociatedwithalong territorialperiod (average: 14 Territorialityin Notiothemisrobertsi 341 d; max: 43 d; n= 31). Formanylibellulidsmuch shorterterritorialtimes are de- scribed (e.g. JACOBS, 1955;PAJUNEN, 1966;CAMPANELLA, 1975). In N. robertsithecompetition forterritoriesis lowandaresidentisrecognized andac- cepted by intruders.Other libellulidsshowoften a greatdealofphysical aggres- sion toestablish and to keep territories(e.g. PAJUNEN, 1966;PARR & PARR, 1974). The slowestablishmentofnew territorialmales in N. robertsi is possible becauseofthelonglife-span andnon-seasonalbreeding behaviour.DAVIES(1978) describessimilarresults fromtheSpeckled WoodButterfly Pararge aegeria. They can perform such a defensive behaviour, because there is a high probability of finding anotherterritoryandthenon-territorialmaleshavea90%chancetoestab- lishanown territorylater withoutfights. Territorialfights in dragonflies between the intruderand the resident cost energy which reduces theirterritorial time (MARDEN, 1989;MARDEN & WAAGE, 1990). As femaleN. robertsi appear very rarely attheponds (CLAUSNITZER, 1995) thelength oftheterritorialperiod is very importantformalestoobtainsuccessful copulations. Territorialmalesspend mostofthe timeperching. Only 5% ofthetotaltime in theterritoryare spentinflightactivities.This classifiesN. robertsias a“percher”, like many species oftheLibellulidae, e.g. Trithemis, Nesciothemis, Sympetrum, Diplacodes and Urothemis (PARR, 1983a). Perching is one way of dragonflies inhabiting shady rain-forest pools to save energy (SHELLY, 1982). The impor- tanceofthelatterwillbeshowninthereasons andfunctionsofthedifferentflight activities practised by territorialmaleN. robertsi(Tab.I). Thetotal numberofflights decreaseswith thetimeoftheday(Fig. 2).An aver- ageof43.6differentflightactivitiesbetweenthehoursof 10:00and 11:00halves toonly 24.6flight activitiesbetweenthehours of 15:00and 16:00,This isbased mainly on areductionofpatrollingandintraspecific flights andcorresponds with theestablishmentofterritoriesonly inthemorning hours. REASONSFOR,ANDFUNCTIONS OFTHEDIFFERENT BEHAVIOURS An importantfactoris the shady environmentofthemating places. Overforty- -onepercent ofall flights were sun flights. Thetotal numberofsun flights perhourdoesdecreaseonly very little(Fig.2) overthe day. Seventy percentofall sunflights followperching (Tab. III). Overfiftypercentofthemales perching ina shady position haveleftforasun flightafter30seconds. Only twelvepercentstay longer than 60seconds incomparison toover45%ofthemalesperching insunny positions (Fig. 3). These supports the ideathatsunflights areprobably important forthermoregulation. By sunbasking insunspotsandthetree tops themales ob- tainasufficientbody temperatureforactivitiesinsidetheirshady territories(MAY, 1976, 1977, 1991;McGEOCH &SAMWAYS, 1991). As I couldnot observe the animals inthetree tops, I can not excludeactivities like mating or feeding when they leavetheterritoriesforsunflights. Butfeedingis unlikely animportantstimu- 342 V.Clausnitzer lationforthe frequent sun flights, as thereis alot ofpossible prey atthepools. I havenever seenN. robertsi hunting or feeding atthepools,butotherspecies andI suppose N. robertsihuntsinthemorning andafternoonhours.There seems to be no reasontofeed exclusively inthetree tops during theterritorialtime,whichalso supports thermoregulation as themain functionof thesun flights. Perching has been regarded as an energy conserving act and to prepare metabolically for flying (MAY, 1976;HEINRICH& CASEY, 1978). This prob- ably applies toN. robertsias itshabitatare shadyrainforestpools (SHELLY, 1982). Perching mayannouncetoothermalesthatthepondis already occupied (MOORE, 1987).To some extendthis interpretation may beafunction fortheperching be- haviourperformed by maleN. robertsiastheterritorialmalesperch visible inthe centreoftheirterritories.Furtherthepermanentpresenceintheterritoryexceptthe sun flightsenables theterritorialmales to geta highpercentageoftherarely ap- pearing females.Femalesandnon-territorialmalesonly appearirregularly forshort timesattheponds. Especially thefemalesspendmostoftheiradultlivesinthetree tops.As territorialmalesspendmostoftheday attheponds, they haveto conserve energy formaintaining theirterritorialpossessions. Interspecific andintraspecific flights are aresponse to thepres- enceofotherdragonflies.Theassumption, thattheseflightsaresexually motivated (e.g. PAJUNEN, 1964b;MOORE, 1952)doesnot apply to N. robertsi. The share ofintraspecific flights inall flightactivities is only 10%and decreases duringthe day(Fig.2).Thisfitstheobservationsthatterritorieshavetobedefendedmainly in themorning. ALCOCK (1987) describesfor Paltothemislineatipes also areduc- tionofintraspecific clashesintheroutineofa day. Sexual flights are caused by females of N. robertsi, N. jonesi and Tetrathemiscorduliformis (all females look andbehave very similarly). Only a small numberofthese flights leadsto asuccessful mating, eitherbecause thefe- malebelongs to another species or is unwilling. Thenumberofsexual-flights in- creases over the day,but is not correlatedto anincreasing numberofmatings. I thinkthattheappetenceformating increasesover the dayandalsothenumberof femalesof T. corduliformis appearing atwater. Sexual flights depend onanexter- nal stimulusinthe formofafemale. The proximate reason forthein spe c t ion f 1i g hts is themovement ofa possible mate. Itincludesotherdragonflies, insectsandevenasmallwater rivulet. Theultimatereason fortheseflights is not easy toexplain - itmightbeofaggres- sive or sexualnature. As Ihaveneverobserved the animalshunting or feeding in theterritories, this cannot be regarded as apossible stimulus. The numberofin- spection flights increases over the day (Fig. 2). Atthe same time thenumberof inter- and intraspecific flights decreases, while the numberof sexual flights in- creases. ThereforeI interpret theinspection flights as sexually stimulated. Allflight activities discussed so farshow amoreor less distinct triggermecha- nism, e.g.presenceofmales, females,movement ofotherobjects, shadinessofthe Territorialityin Notiothemis mbertsi 343 perch. Moredifficultis theexplanation ofp at r o11 in g.Theseflightsseemtobe spontaneousandindefinite(Tab. I) andIregardthemas unspecifiedincomparison to inspection flights, but endogenous stimulatedby sexual andterritory-defence motivation. Patrolling isa commonbehaviourofterritorialmaledragonflies (e.g.DeBANO, 1993;HASSAN, 1978;PARR&PARR, 1974;WATANABE etal„ 1987),butsel- domexplained. WATANABE etal.(1987) describe thisflightas ananalogue be- haviourto theterritorialsongsofbirds.Inthe case ofN. robertsiIregardpatrolling together withperching as the behaviours to demonstratethat thisterritory is al- ready occupied. Mostofthebehaviour the territorialmales ofN. robertsi show corresponds to theirlongterritorialperiod, nonseasonality andscarcityoffemales.Itis anadoption totheirhabitat- smallandshady rainforestponds.Otherspecies, whichbelongto theTetratheminaeandliveinsimilarhabitats, show comparable territorialbehav- iour and mating systems, e.g. Micromacromiacamerunica, Notiothemisjonesi, Tetrathemiscorduliformis andT. camerunensis(e.g. LEMPERT, 1988;MILLER, 1993). ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 1ammost gratefultoMr WILBERFORCEOKEKAfor hishelpandhospitalityin theKakamega ForestandtoDrANDREASMARTENSforvarious supportingideasand commentsand foraconsid- erable amountoftime. I also wishtothankthe late Dr PETER MILLER for inspiringideas, Mr GRAHAMVICKforthe determination ofsomespecies.ProfessorMIKEPARRfor valuablecriticism onthe manuscript,my parents forfinancial support andhelpinthefieldandProfessor REINHARD REMANEandProfessor GEORG RUPPELL, who madethisworkpossible. REFERENCES ALCOCK, J..1987.Male reproductivetacticsinthe libellulid dragonflyPaltothemis lineatipes:Tem- poralpartitioningofterritories. Behaviour 103: 157-173. BICK,G.H.& J.C.BICK, 1965. Demographyand behaviour ofthe damselfly,Argia apicalis(Say), (Odonata: Coenagriidae).Ecology46: 461-472. BROWN, J.L„ 1964.The evolution ofdiversityin avian territorial systems.WilsonBull. 76: 160-169. CAMPANELLA, P. J.&L. L. WOLF, 1974. Temporalleks asamatingsysteminatemperate zone dragonfly(Odonata:Anisoptera)I: Plathemislydia(Drury). Behaviour 51: 49-87. CAMPANELLA,P.J..1975.Theevolutionofmatingsystemsintemperatezonedragonflies(Odonata: Anisoptera)2: Libellulaluctuosa (Burmeister). Behaviour 54: 278-309. CLAUSNITZER, V., 1995. Verhaltensiikologie von Notiothemis robertsi Fraser, 1944 (Odonata: Libellulidae).DiplArb.Philipps-Univ. Marbutg. CORBET, P.S., 1962.Abiologyofdragonflies.Witherby, London. DAVIES,N.B., 1978. Territorial defence inthe SpeckledWood Butterfly(Parargeaegeria):theresi- dentalwayswins. Anim.Behav. 26: 138-147. DeBANO, S. J., 1993. Territorialityin the dragonfly Libellula saturataUhler: Mutual avoidance or resourcedefense? (Anisoptera:Libellulidae). Odonatologica22: 431-441. 344 V. Clausnitzer EMLEN, S.T.&L. W.ORING,1977.Ecology,sexualselection,andtheevolution ofmatingsystems. Science 197: 215-223. FRASER, F. C, 1944. Anote onthe genus NotiothemisRis, with thedescription ofanew species (orderOdonata). Proc. R.Ent. Soc.Lend. (B) 13:40-43. GAMBLES, R.M„ 1960. Seasonal distribution and longevity in Nigeriandragonflies.JlVV! Afr. Sci. Am.6: 18-26. HARVEY, I. F, &S. F. HUBARD, 1987. Observations onthe reproductive behaviourof Orthemis ferruginea(Fabricius)(Anisoptera:Libellulidae).Odonatologica 16: 1-8. HASSAN, A.T, 1978. Reproductivebehaviour ofAcisomapanorpoidesinflatum Selys (Anisoptera: Libellulidae). Odonatologica7; 237-245. HEINRICH, B.& T. M. CASEY, 1978. Heattransferin dragonflies:“fliers” and “perchers”. J. exp Biol. 74: 17-36. JACOBS,M.E., 1955. Studiesonterritorialism and sexualselection indragonflies.Ecology 36: 566- -586. LEGRAND,J„ 1977. Deux TetrathemisBrauernouveauxduGabonetlalarve del‘und'eux (Aniso- ptera:Libellulidae). Odonatologica6: 245-251. LEGRAND,J„1979.Morphologic,biologicetecologiedeMalgassophlebiaaequatoris,n.sp.,nouveau TetratheminaeduGabon(Odonata;Libellulidae).Revuefr.Ent. (N. S.) 1: 3-12. LEMPERT, J„ 1988. Untersuchungen zurFauna, Okologie und zum Forlpflanzungsverhaltenvon Litelien(I(Odonatai))anGewdssern des twpischenRegenwaldesinLiberia, Westafrika.DiplArb. derFriedrich-Wilhelms Univ. Bonn. MARDEN, J.H„ 1989. Bodybuildingdragonflies:costs and benefits ofmaximisingflight muscle. Physiol. Zool. 62; 505-521. MARDEN, J.H.&J. K.WAAGE, 1990.Escalateddamselflyterritorial contestsareenergeticwarsof attrition.Anim.Behav. 39: 954-959. MAY,M.L.,1976.Thermoregulationandadaptiontotemperatureindragonflies(Odonata:Anisoptera). Ecol. Monogr.46: 1-32. MAY,M.L., 1977. Thermoregulationand reproductiveactivity intropicaldragonfliesofthe genus Micrathyria. Ecology 58: 787-798. MAY,M. L„ 1991. Thermaladaptationsofdragonflies,revisited. Adv. Odonalol. 5: 71-88. McGEOCH,M. A.& M. J. SAMWAYS, 1991. Dragonfliesandthethermal landscape:implications for theirconservation (Anisoptera).Odonatologica20: 303-320. MILLER, P.L., 1983, Theduration ofcopulationcorrelates with otheraspectsofmatingbehaviour in Orthetrum chrysostigma (Burmeister)(Anisoptera:Libellulidae).Odonatologica 12: 227- -238. MILLER, P.L„ 1993. Somedragonfliesofthe BudongoForest, WesternUganda(Odonata). Opusc. zool. flumin.102: 1-12, MILLER,P.L., 1995.SomedragonfliesofforestsnearKampala,Uganda,withnotes ontheirecology andbehaviour (Odonata). Opusc. zool. flumin. 136: 1-19. MILLER,P.L. &A.K.MILLER, 1985. Rates ofovipositionandsomeotheraspects ofreproductive behaviour in Tholymistillarga(Fabricius)inKenya(Anisoptera:Libellulidae).Odonatologica 14:287-299. MILLER, P. L. & A. K. MILLER, 1991. The dragonfliesofHunter'sLodge,Kenya, 1981-1991. Notui odonalol., 3(8): 123-129. MOORE,N. W., 1952. Notes on theovipositionbehaviour ofthe dragonflySympetrumstriolatum Charpentier.Behaviour 4: 101-103. MOORE,A.J„1987.Behavioralecology ofLibellulaluctuosa (Burmeister)(Anisoptera:Libellulidae). 2. Proposed functions forterritorial behaviors. Odonatologica 16: 385-391. NOBLE,G.K., 1935. The role ofdominanceinthesociallife ofbirds. Auk 56: 263-273. PAJUNEN,V.I..1964a.AggressivebehaviourinLeucorrhiniacaudalis Charp.(Odonata:Libellulidae). Annlszool. fenn. 1:357-369.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.