ebook img

Talking to Adults: The Contribution of Multiparty Discourse to Language Acquisition PDF

360 Pages·2002·1.28 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Talking to Adults: The Contribution of Multiparty Discourse to Language Acquisition

TALKING TO ADULTS The Contribution of Multiparty Discourse to Language Acquisition Edited by Shoshana Blum-Kulka Hebrew University, Jerusalem Catherine E. Snow Harvard University Copyright ' 2002 by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. All rights reserved. No part of the book may be reproduced in any form, by photostat, microform, retrieval system, or any other means, without the prior written permission of the publisher. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Publishers 10 Industrial Avenue Mahwah, New Jersey 07430 Cover design by Kathryn Houghtaling Lacey Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Talking to adults : the contribution of Multiparty Discourse to Language Acquisition / edited by Shoshana Blum-Kulka, Catherine E. Snow. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and indexes. ISBN 0-8058-3660-8 (cloth : alk. paper) – ISBN 0-8058-3661-6 (pbk. : alk. paper) 1. Language acquisition(cid:151)Parent participation. 2. Discourse analysis. 3. Parent and Child. 4. Pragmatics. I. Blum-Kulka, Shoshana. II. Snow, Catherine E. P118.5 .T35 2002 401".93(cid:151)dc21 2001051294 Books published by Lawrence Erlbaum Associates are printed on acid-free paper, and their bindings are chosen for strength and durability. Printed in the United States of America 1 0 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Contents Editors(cid:146) Introduction 1 PART I: ISSUES IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF EXTENDED DISCOURSE: NARRATIVES AND EXPLANATIONS 1 Deciding What to Tell: Selecting and Elaborating 15 Narrative Topics in Family Interaction and Children(cid:146)s Elicited Personal Experience Stories Diane E. Beals and Catherine E. Snow 2 Greek Children and Familiar Narratives in Family 33 Contexts: En Route to Cultural Performances Alexandra Georgakopoulou 3 (cid:147)What Did You Do in School Today?(cid:148) Speech Genres 55 and Tellability in Multiparty Family Mealtime Conversations in Two Cultures Vibeke Aukrust 4 (cid:147)Do You Believe That Lot(cid:146)s Wife Is Blocking the Road 85 (to Jericho)?(cid:148): Co-Constructing Theories About the World With Adults Shoshana Blum-Kulka 5 Peer-Group Culture and Narrative Development 117 Ageliki Nicolopoulou PART II: THE LANGUAGE OF AFFECT AND HUMOR: PRAGMATIC DEVELOPMENTAL PERSPECTIVES 6 Socialization of Affect During Mealtime Interactions 155 Christine HØrot iii iv CONTENTS 7 Cognitive Expressions and Humorous Phrases in Family 181 Discourse as Reflectors and Cultivators of Cognition Ruth Nevat-Gal 8 Language Games in the Strict Sense of the Term: Children(cid:146)s 209 Poetics and Conversation Alessandra Fasulo, Vivian Liberati, and Clotilde Pontecorvo PART III: ISSUES OF CONTEXT AND CULTURE IN PRAGMATIC DEVELOPMENT 9 Everyone Has to Lie in Tzeltal 241 Penelope Brown 10 Voice and Collusion in Adult(cid:150)Child Talk: Toward 277 an Architecture of Intersubjectivity Karin Aronsson and Mia Thorell 11 Bilingual Context for Language Development 295 Hiroko Kasuya 12 From Home to School: School-Age Children 327 Talking With Adults Catherine E. Snow and Shoshana Blum-Kulka Author Index 343 Subject Index 349 Editors(cid:146) Introduction THE PRESENT VOLUME History The question of how adults(cid:146) speech to children affects the process of lan- guage acquisition has attracted researchers(cid:146) attention since the late 1960s, when the first studies characterizing the nature of input to young language learners were undertaken. An edited volume entitled Talking to Children, published in 1977, brought together much of that early work, including a number of chapters devoted to the topic of baby talk, anthropological lin- guists(cid:146) descriptions of the special lexical and grammatical forms used with young children. Thus, Talking to Children (edited by Snow & Ferguson, 1977) in a sense constituted a bridge between an old tradition of research into (cid:147)sociolinguistic registers(cid:148) including baby talk and new work that analyzed similar data in new ways. The newer work was carried out within a theoreti- cal context defined by freshly formulated questions about language acquisi- tion that drew attention to the nature of linguistic input to language learners as one factor influencing language development. The body of research on input to young children, and on related topics such as input to second language learners, teacher talk in preschool class- room settings, adjustments in talk to children with various disabilities, and social and cultural differences in the nature of talk addressed to children, 1 LEA(cid:150)THE TYPE HOUSE (cid:150) TALKING TO ADULTS (BLUM-KULKA/SNOW) 2 EDITORS(cid:146) INTRODUCTION expanded quickly. By the early 1990s, a second edited volume was pub- lished, entitled Input and Interaction in Language Acquisition (edited by Gallaway & Richards, 1994) devoted to providing an overview of the full array of research findings concerning input. In contrast to Talking to Chil- dren, which was a collection of original research reports, Input and Inter- actionconsisted of extensive reviews of literature, reflecting the enormous growth of interest in these topics. The forward to Input and Interaction describes it explicitly as follows: [It(cid:146)s] an up-to-date statement of the facts and controversies surrounding (cid:147)Baby Talk,(cid:148) its nature and likely effects. With contributions from lead- ing linguists and psychologists, it explores language acquisition in differ- ent cultures and family contexts, in typical and atypical learners, and in second and foreign language learners. It is designed as a sequel to the now famous Talking to children. Input and Interaction contained 10 chapters summarizing and analyzing the work that had been carried out in the period since Talking to Children, and included references to well over 500 original research papers. It is clear from reading the chapters in Input and Interaction in Language Acquisi- tionthat the notion of input had been redefined and enriched, whereas much of the work reviewed and reported in the 1977 book was purely descriptive. The papers in the 1977 book operated from the presumption that character- istics such as phonological clarity, grammatical simplicity, and redundancy were the crucial defining features of (cid:147)baby talk.(cid:148) By 1994 input was seen as conversational, interactive, transactional. The papers in Gallaway and Richards provide richer descriptions and more problem-focused analyses of the inter- actions in which children and other language learners engage, with a primary emphasis always on the question of how these interactions simplify the prob- lem of language acquisition for the learner. Though enormously informative, the work reported in Gallaway and Richards (1994) is limited in a number of ways. First, the data being ana- lyzed were almost all derived from two-party interactions(cid:151)a child alone with a parent or a learner(cid:150)teacher dyad isolated from a classroom context. Many of the studies analyzed interaction during fairly structured tasks, but even those that adopted naturalistic observational methods, for example, recording conversations at home during the normal course of daily life, typically limited the focus of their analysis to dyadic relations. Similarly, even the studies that went beyond the mother(cid:150)child dyad to consider the role of siblings and fathers in language acquisition (such as those reviewed by Barton & Tomasello, 1994) mostly used the dyadic setting with these alternate conversational partners. Dyadic interactions probably do not domi- nate the experience of most children, so the degree to which the situa- EDITORS(cid:146) INTRODUCTION 3 tions typically analyzed inform us fully about the nature of input to language learners can be questioned. The work in Gallaway and Richards (1994) could be said to reflect the dominant paradigm within research on input to language learners(cid:151)an ap- proach influenced by the procedures and preferences of laboratory psycholo- gists. The very important question this research addresses is what sort of information about the nature of the adult language system is available to the learner(cid:151)are there aspects of input that can be related to relatively rapid and trouble-free acquisition? This question has dictated an analytic approach, an approach that characterizes the child as someone acquiring skills through exposure to certain language structures. An alternative conception of the child language learner can be encountered in the work of researchers who have taken a more anthropological approach to collecting data about interaction with young children. This approach is exemplified in the work of researchers such as Heath (1983), Scheiffelin (1990), and Ochs (1988), who have collected ethnographic data in naturalistic set- tings, focusing as much on the participation structures into which children can enter as on the exact nature of the language used by them. Researchers within the more anthropological paradigm see language as a cultural prac- tice, and thus describe language acquisition as a socializing process(cid:151)a pro- cess played out through language of acquiring the language use skills, rights, and values that constitute membership in a group. For such researchers, is- sues such as who may talk to whom, what language performances are highly valued, what cultural norms link language to social context, and what parents believe about how children learn to talk are very important. The descriptions they provide include rich evidence about the opportunities children have to learn certain sorts of social practices, but typically less documentation con- cerning what learning has actually occurred for a particular child as a result of a particular experience. Nor does this research normally provide a basis for estimating the frequency of various sorts of opportunities to learn about the social practices of interest, thus excluding the possibility of relating quan- tity or quality of exposure to speed of acquisition. Research within the anthropological paradigm has documented the impor- tance of one of the basic premises of the volume we present here(cid:151)the premise that children are offered unique opportunities to learn from participation in mul- tiparty interactions, because the demands and the displays inherent in multiparty interactions are different from, and more challenging than, those inherent in dy- adic interactions. The anthropological approach to studying input is distinct from the psychological approach in eschewing structured or non-natural contexts for observation and in viewing the child(cid:146)s task of learning language more holisti- cally, as one of achieving cultural membership rather than accomplishing par- ticular skills. The psychological approach, on the other hand, has docu- 4 EDITORS(cid:146) INTRODUCTION mented the importance of understanding differences in different children(cid:146)s experiences of interaction, showing that those differences have consequences for children(cid:146)s learning. Why This Book? The goal of the volume presented here is to focus on children learning lan- guage in naturalistic contexts. We take naturalisticto imply contexts that are often multiparty, that involve both multigenerational and peer interaction (or at least interactions in which the child(cid:146)s relationship to more than one adult or one child is simultaneously relevant). Most of the work presented in this vol- ume focuses on multiparty and multigenerational contexts, but we see the ideas suggested as equally relevant to multiparty peer interactions. We see this work as different from most of that in Gallaway and Richards (1994) precisely in that we focus on the value of such multiparty interaction, but also as different from previous work in the anthropological tradition in that our interest in describing input derives from an interest in what and how children learn and how that relates to their opportunities to learn. In other words, we view input from the perspective of both the adults and the children involved in the interaction. Thus, we hope to merge the strengths of the more cognitivist, psychological approach (its interest in the process of learning, its analysis of the learning task, its demand for evidence about what the child has learned, its focus on individual differences among children) with those of the more descriptive, anthropological approach (attention to the full array of opportu- nities to learn, recognition of the interconnectedness of being socialized into appropriate language use and achieving membership in a culture, consider- ation of alternate routes to any learning outcome). THE ADVANTAGES OF BEING MORE THAN TWO We argue, then, that there are important things about language and about cultural membership that children would not have the chance to learn if they had access to only dyadic interactions. Some of these things have to do with quantity of input(cid:151) that one simply hears more language if one is hearing it from many interlocutors. Others have to do with quality(cid:151)that certain complexities of role, of dealing with subsets of interlocutors as addressees, of having various relationships displayed within a particular interaction are possible only if more than two people are in- volved. Others have to do with the code(cid:151)that the need to incorporate different varieties, dialects, or languages is much more likely to be present with several interlocutors. Thus, we argue, becoming a full-fledged member of society, in which participating in groups, engaging a variety of interlocutors, and EDITORS(cid:146) INTRODUCTION 5 taking on a wide range of roles are prerequisites, requires participating as a child in multiparty interactions. It is the goal of the chapters in this volume to describe, for a number of specific cases and across a variety of cultures, what participation in multiparty interactions looks like, and what we can conclude children learn from it. Here we briefly preview some of the issues that emerge: Quantity of Input. Hart and Risley (1995) documented the amount of input available to children in different families varying in social class. Though it is difficult from their report to determine the occurrence of dyadic versus multiparty interactions, it is clear that the sparsest input was provided to children in single-parent, socially isolated, welfare-dependent families. The amount of language such children heard, and the size of the vocabularies they achieved, were strikingly smaller than in two-parent families or in families with many visitors. Similar findings emerged from analyses of dinner table conversations collected as part of the Home-School Study (Dickinson & Ta- bors, 2001; Snow, 1991). Mealtimes at which at least two adults were present were longer, contained more language per unit time, and were more likely to include stories, explanations, and other discourse contexts that offered op- portunities to hear challenging vocabulary and grammar, than mealtimes in single-parent families with no visitors. Thus, on the assumption that children who hear more talk learn to talk faster and better, there are advantages to having more adult sources of input and to having exposure to contexts in which more than one adult is present at any given time. Social Relationships. The mother(cid:150)child relationship is the only one that needs to be played out in dyadic interactions. The mother is more knowl- edgeable, a source of nurturance and of limits, the responsible party. The child is the learner, a seeker of nurturance and a tester of limits, with greater resources of playfulness and irresponsibility. This simple complementarity can be greatly complicated if a sibling enters the interaction(cid:151)then the mother may need to differentiate her level of nurturance or style of discipline, and the siblings have their own issues of dominance, competitiveness in access to the mother, potential for collaboration, possibly domains in which their shared competence is greater than the mother(cid:146)s. If at this point Dad comes home, then Mom(cid:146)s role as supreme arbiter and ultimate source of knowl- edge may (or may not) be threatened, and opportunities arise for exploita- tion of parental authority precisely because it is distributed ((cid:145)but Daddy said I could(cid:146)). Children have opportunities to learn something about the role of mother or father from dyadic interactions, but they have opportunities to learn about the roles of wife, husband, friends, siblings, and parent to an- other only from multiparty interactions.

Description:
This volume provides a comprehensive and in-depth analysis of the contribution of multiparty intergenerational talk in a variety of cultures to the development of children's communicative capacities. The book focuses on the complexity of the cultural and interactional contexts in which pragmatic lea
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.