ebook img

STARLING REVIEW The endocrinology of sexual arousal PDF

17 Pages·2005·0.18 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview STARLING REVIEW The endocrinology of sexual arousal

411 STARLING REVIEW . . The endocrinology of sexual arousal J Bancroft TheKinseyInstitute,IndianaUniversity,MorrisonHallthirdfloor,Bloomington,Indiana47405,USA (RequestsforoffprintsshouldbeaddressedtoJBancroft;Email:[email protected]) Abstract The relevance of testosterone, oestradiol and certain pep- tion hypothesis’ to account for the striking gender differ- tides (oxytocin (OT), (cid:1)-endorphin and prolactin (PRL)) encesisoffered.Thereislimitedevidenceofadirecteffect to sexual arousal in humans is reviewed. In addition to of oestradiol on sexual arousability in women. The extent behavioural studies, evidence of distribution of gonadal to which testosterone in women acts by conversion to steroid receptors in the brain and the limited evidence oestradiol or by increase of free oestradiol is not yet clear. frombrainimagingarealsoconsidered.Testosteroneplays The role of peptides in sexual arousal remains uncertain, a key role in the adult male, with clear, consistent partly because of the multiple roles and sites of action of evidencefromstudiesofhypogonadalandeugonadalmen. most peptides. OT and (cid:1)-endorphin appear to have both The roles of testosterone in the development of sexual excitatory and inhibitory effects. PRL has been proposed arousability, and in the aging male, are less clear. The asaninhibitoryfactorviadirectinhibitionofdopaminergic relevance of aromatization and of non-sexual effects of activity,buttheevidenceforthisisinconclusive.Whereas testosterone which might indirectly influence sexual thetraditionalconceptof‘hormone’continuestoapplyto arousalarenotwellunderstood.Testosteroneinthefemale the role of testosterone and oestradiol in sexual arousal, presents a more complex, less consistent picture. One peptides present a more complex role. possible explanation is a much greater variability across JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 women in responsiveness to testosterone. A ‘desensitiza- Introduction This paper will review the role of hormones in influ- encing sexual arousal, sexual arousability, orgasm and the The term ‘sexual arousal’ is used with variable meanings. post-orgasm inhibition of arousability. Its focus will be on It is commonly equated with genital response; thus the the human experience, but reference to the animal manwhohasanerectionissaidtobesexuallyaroused.As literaturewillbemadewhenhelpfulinunderstandingthe used in this paper, the concept of sexual arousal involves human condition. more than genital response (Bancroft 2002a), covering a state motivated towards the experience of sexual pleasure Current concepts andpossiblyorgasm,andinvolving(i)informationprocess- ing of relevant stimuli, (ii) arousal in a general sense, (iii) Gonadal steroids incentive motivation and (iv) genital response. ‘Sexual arousal’appliestothestate,and‘sexualarousability’tothe Androgens in the male capacity for experiencing sexual arousal, which varies Hypogonadal and eugonadal men Most controlled studies of across individuals, and within individuals across time. In testosterone replacement in hypogonadal men have used thispaper,sexualinterestisconceptualizedasanaspectof a period of withdrawal as a baseline, followed by the sexualarousal,whenallfourcomponentsmaybeinvolved administration of testosterone and placebo, using a to some extent, but where at least sexual information double-blind cross-over design (for review see Bancroft processing (e.g. sexual thoughts) is associated with some 2003). Such studies consistently show a reduction in the degree of incentive motivation. Orgasm needs to be level of sexual interest during testosterone withdrawal, considered,bothasagoaloftheincentivemotivation,and usually evident within 3 to 4 weeks, consistent with as a process associated, at least in males, with a temporary testosterone being necessary for normal levels of sexual suspension or inhibition of sexual arousability. interest (and arousability). If testosterone withdrawal lasts JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 DOI:10.1677/joe.1.06233 0022–0795/05/0186–411 (cid:1)2005SocietyforEndocrinology PrintedinGreatBritain Onlineversionviahttp://www.endocrinology-journals.org Downloaded from Bioscientifica.com at 03/02/2023 12:47:27AM via free access 412 JBANCROFT · Sexualarousal long enough, seminal emission will eventually be level of testosterone needed to avoid the sexual effects of impaired. Typically, in male studies of this kind, placebo testosterone withdrawal was substantially lower than the has only a modest effect, but testosterone replace- pre-treatment baseline level. This is consistent with most ment restores sexual interest and arousability. Effects on men having more circulating testosterone than they need sexual activity with a partner are less consistent, partly for the maintenance of normal sexual function. because they depend on partner and relationship The exploration of testosterone as a method of male characteristics. Frequency of masturbation tends to contraception has led to further studies of the effects of follow the level of sexual interest, although cultural increasing circulating testosterone above a normal base- factors may influence this pattern of sexual expression line by means of exogenous testosterone administration. (Anderson et al. 1999). Anderson et al. (1992), using weekly injections of either Psychophysiologicalstudieshavebeenusedtoassessthe placeboortestosteroneenanthate(200mg)over8weeks, effects of testosterone withdrawal and replacement on found no effect of the exogenous testosterone on sexual genital response (erection) to sexual stimuli. Early studies activity, either with a partner or as masturbation, but a werebasedonthemaximumchangeinpenilecircumfer- significant increase in a measure of sexual interest inde- ence as a measure of erectile response; they found little pendent of sexual interaction with the partner. Bagatell difference between hypogonadal men with and without et al. (1994b) and Yates et al. (1999), in eugonadal men, testosterone replacement. More recently, such assessment foundnoeffectsofincreasingtestosteroneonfrequencyof has also included penile rigidity as well as duration of sexual activity, or in the latter study, daily measures of penile response. This showed significantly more rigid sexual interest. and longer duration erectile responses with testosterone Buena et al. (1993) explored the effect of varying replacement. Without testosterone, the erectile response testosterone levels within the normal range. They first was ‘stimulus bound’, i.e. would recede as soon as the suppressed testicular function with a GnRH agonist stimulus was switched off. With testosterone replace- (Lupron), followed by exogenous testosterone admin- ment, the response would not only show greater rigidity, istration in either high or low dosage, to produce testos- but would also last beyond the sexual stimulus (Carani terone levels that were either at the high end or low end et al. 1995). of the normal range. They found no difference in sexual Nocturnal penile tumescence (NPT), the occurrence activity, sexual interest or NPT, although the rigidity of of spontaneous erections during rapid eye movement NPT was not measured. (REM) sleep, is relevant. The neurophysiological basis of NPT is still disputed, but one plausible explanation Androgens in male sexual development Whereas the impor- is that REM sleep is associated with a ‘switching off’ of tanceoftestosteroneinsexualdifferentiation,bothinearly the noradrenergic cells in the locus coeruleus (l.c.) development and around puberty, is beyond dispute, the (Parmeggiana & Morrison 1990) which, via their spinal impact of testosterone on the emergence of sexual arous- projections, are probably associated with inhibitory tone ability is less clear. Udry and colleagues carried out two in the penis. Thus, the reduction of inhibitory tone studies in teenage boys in which testosterone levels were duringREMpermitswhatcanbecalled‘excitatorytone’ related to various aspects of sexuality. In the first (Udry to be expressed as erection. NPT is clearly impaired in et al. 1985), a cross-sectional study, the free testosterone hypogonadal men, and restored to normal with testoster- index was found to be a strong predictor of ‘sexual one replacement. The l.c. has testosterone receptors motivation’, whereas stage of pubertal development was (Parmeggiana & Morrison 1990), and this putative ‘exci- not predictive. In the second (Halpern et al. 1993), a tatory tone’ can be regarded as testosterone dependent. longitudinal study over 3 years, with 6-monthly assess- Carani et al. (1995) evaluated the effects of exogenous ments, they found the reverse; the stage of pubertal testosterone on NPT in eugonadal men. Intramuscular developmentwasmuchmorepredictiveofsexualinterest testosterone enanthate had no effect on sleep parameters, and behaviour than the free testosterone index. One and did not affect frequency, degree or duration of NPT, possible explanation for this apparent contradiction is that when assessed as penile circumference, but did increase, the impact of testosterone on sexual arousability (and modestly but significantly, penile rigidity during NPT. hencebehaviour)hastogothroughstagesofdevelopment, Testosterone manipulation in eugonadal men has pro- which may involve changes in receptor numbers or duced results consistent with the earlier hypogonadal sensitivity, a process which will also be influenced by studies. Bagatell et al. (1994a) used the gonadotrophin- individual differences in receptor sensitivity. Gooren releasing hormone (GnRH) antagonist, NalGlu, to sup- (1988), in a study of hypogonadal teenage males, found press testosterone levels in eugonadal men over a 6-week that boys with primary hypogonadism showed less period. This lowered sexual interest and associated sexual response to testosterone replacement than boys with activity. An additional feature was the administration of secondaryhypogonadism.Otherstudiescomparinghyper- varyingdosesofexogenoustestosteroneorplaceboduring gonadotrophic and hypogonadotrophic hypogonadism theNalGluadministration.Thissuggestedthattheplasma have not shown such clear differences, but have all JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 www.endocrinology-journals.org Downloaded from Bioscientifica.com at 03/02/2023 12:47:27AM via free access Sexualarousal · JBANCROFT 413 involvedmaleswellbeyondtheageofnormalpuberty(for thelevelsoftestosteroneinthecirculationaresubstantially review see Bancroft 2003). higher than required to maintain sexual arousability, suggestingthatothereffectsoftestosterone,mostprobably AndrogensandaginginmenSchiavietal.(1990),inastudy in the periphery, require higher levels than are needed in of healthy, medication-free men aged 45 to 74 years, thecentralnervoussystem(CNS).Theroleoftestosterone found a clear decline in sexual interest, arousability and in the emerging sexual arousability of the peri-pubertal activity with age. The relatively predictable effects of maleisnotwellunderstood.Intheoldermale,thepicture testosteronewithdrawalandreplacementinyoungeradult is complicated by various aging effects, including altered men gives way to a more complex, or at least less well- hypothalamo–pituitary feedback, increased testosterone understoodpictureinoldermen.Anumberofage-related binding and reduced receptor sensitivity. changes may be relevant: altered negative feedback of testosterone and hence less increase in luteinizing hor- Androgens in the female In the female, in comparison mone (LH) with falling testosterone levels, increased sex with the male, we find inconsistent and often contradic- hormone binding globulin (SHBG) and hence relatively tory evidence. This is in spite of the fact that we have reduced free testosterone and the likelihood of an age- many more studies in women, usually involving larger relateddeclineintestosteronereceptorsensitivity.Schiavi samples, than are found in the male literature. This may et al. (1990) also found an age-related decline in NPT. result from the greater complexity of the reproductive This suggested an age-related decline in testosterone- endocrine system in women, who experience menstrual dependent central arousability (i.e. ‘excitatory tone’). It cycles, pregnancy and lactation and a clearly identifiable is noteworthy that, as yet, there has been no adequate menopause. At the same time, these endocrine variations placebo-controlled evaluation of the effects of testoster- offer more opportunities for studying hormone/sexual one replacement on sexuality in older men (Institute of arousal relationships than is the case with men. Medicine 2004). DevelopmentalaspectsIncreasinglevelsoftestosteroneoccur Clinical studies – relevance of testosterone to low sexual desire in the development of girls as they approach and go anderectiledysfunction(ED)Inonesmallplacebo-controlled through puberty. However, the changes are much less studyofeugonadalmenwiththeprimarycomplaintoflow substantialthaninthemale.Testosteronestartsatalower sexualdesire,O’Carroll&Bancroft(1984)foundamodest level in the infant girl, and effectively doubles through but significant increase in sexual desire with testosterone pubertal maturation, compared with an 18-fold increase treatment. The limited evidence of the effectiveness of in testosterone for boys. The most substantial evidence of testosterone in treating ED is inconsistent. O’Carroll & therelationshipbetweentestosteroneandemergingsexual Bancroft (1984) found no improvement in erections with arousability in females comes again from Udry et al. testosterone treatment in eugonadal men with ED. In (1986). As with their studies on adolescent boys, they studies of men showing some degree of hypogonadism in found discrepant results between their cross-sectional association with ED, Carani et al. (1990a) and Morales study of eighth to tenth grade girls (approximately 13–15 etal.(1994a)foundimprovementinerectilefunctionina years of age), where they found a relation between minority of cases. So far it has been difficult to predict testosterone levels and measures of sexual interest and which cases of ED are likely to benefit from testosterone, masturbation, but not with the likelihood of having although low baseline levels of testosterone certainly experienced sexual intercourse, and their longitudinal increase the likelihood. Based on a large clinical series of studyofgirlspost-menarchewherethereverserelationsto suchcases,Buvat&Lemaire(1997)recommendedthatin testosterone were found (Halpern et al. 1997). Similar men under 50 years of age, measurement of serum explanationsasdiscussedfortheirmalestudiescouldapply testosterone should only be done when there was an here,butinadditionthereisacrucialmethodologicalissue associated loss of sexual interest, whereas in men over 50 of timing of blood sampling for testosterone in relation to years of age with ED, testosterone should be measured in theovariancycle(forafullerdiscussionoftheseissuessee all cases. Bancroft 2003). Conclusions about the role of testosterone in the male The ThemenstrualcycleThereisalackofevidenceoftestoster- evidenceisfairlyclearthatinmenwhohavegonethrough one levels during the early cycles of post-menarcheal normal puberty and who have not yet been affected by adolescents,whichtendtobeirregularandnotpredictably aging, testosterone plays an important role in their sexual ovulatory.However,onceawomansettlesintoapatternof interest and associated sexual arousability. The evidence regular ovulatory cycles, testosterone levels typically rise points mainly to the effects of testosterone on central duringthefollicularphaseandareatamaximumapproxi- arousal mechanisms; the peripheral effects of testosterone mately for the middle third of the cycle, declining during in the human male, relevant to sexual arousal, are as yet thefinalthirdtoreachanadirduringthefirstfewdaysof unclear. It is also apparent that, in adult eugonadal men, thenextfollicularphase.Giventhispattern,iftestosterone www.endocrinology-journals.org JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 Downloaded from Bioscientifica.com at 03/02/2023 12:47:27AM via free access 414 JBANCROFT · Sexualarousal is important for sexual arousability in women, we should combined OC users? Graham et al. (1995), in a placebo- expect to find related temporal patterns of arousability controlled study of women who had been sterilized or throughthecycle.However,themid-cycleriseintestos- whosepartnershadbeensterilized,foundthatasubstantial terone is associated with other hormonal changes, and minority of women on the combined OC reported a hence correlational studies may not discriminate between decline in sexual interest and enjoyment, an effect not direct effects of mid-cycle testosterone levels, and the observed with a low-dose progestagen only method. In effects of other mid-cycle changes, such as the rise in a subsequent study of women starting on OCs, where oestradiol. a pre-OC baseline was established, negative effects on In the substantial literature on the pattern of sexual sexual interest and mood were the best predictors of interest and behaviour through the menstrual cycle there discontinuationoftheOC(Sandersetal.2001).Inbothof are many inconsistencies, and Hedricks (1994) has dis- these studies, lowered testosterone levels could be the cussed various methodological explanations for them. explanation.Asyet,however,wehavenodirectevidence There is a relatively consistent finding that sexual activity of the relation between testosterone levels and sexual is lowest during the menstrual phase. However, this does interest.Otherexplanationsfortheseadversesexualeffects notnecessarilymeanthatsexualarousabilityisatitslowest need to be considered (e.g. progestagenic effects). But if at that stage; there are a number of other non-hormonal lowered free testosterone is the explanation this illustrates explanations for the drop in sexual activity during men- once again that such reduction is only relevant in a struation.Thereisalsoatendencyacrossstudiesforindices proportion of women. of sexual interest to be highest during the follicular phase or around ovulation, though with considerable individual Effects of anti-androgens Cyproterone acetate (CPA) is an variability in this respect. This mid- to late-follicular anti-androgen with both negative feedback and direct patterniscompatiblewithaneffectoftherisingtestoster- androgen receptor antagonism, and which has been used one during the follicular phase, although one might have for the treatment of androgen-dependent conditions such expectedacontinuationofthistestosteroneeffectintothe as acne and hirsutism in women. Appelt & Strauss (1986) first part of the luteal phase. Clearly, other hormonal studied 36 women who had not had sexual problems explanations have to be considered. before starting on CPA, and of these 16 (44%) reported A much more limited literature looks at the correlation negativeeffectsontheirsexlife;thisrisesto61%ifwomen between testosterone level and sexuality through the not in sexual relationships are excluded. cycle, and it is very inconsistent (for review see Bancroft 2003). In part, there may be methodological reasons for Lactation Reduction in sexual interest and enjoyment is this, especially variability in the aspects of sexuality common during the post-partum period, and is more measured. But these inconsistent findings, involving marked in breast-feeding than bottle-feeding mothers studieswithrelativelysmallnumbersofparticipants,could (Alder&Bancroft1988,Hyde&DeLamater2000).Alder result from substantial individual variability in et al. (1986) assessed women prospectively for 6 months testosterone/behaviour relationships. post-partum. Not surprisingly, given the effects of lacta- Onlyonestudyaddressesthetimingoftheeffectsofan tion on ovarian function, bottle feeders had higher testos- increase in testosterone on sexual arousal. Eight healthy teroneandandrostenedionelevelsthanthebreast-feeding women with normal testosterone levels, given sublingual mothers. Of more relevance, five of the breast feeders doses of testosterone in a placebo-controlled experiment, reported reduced sexual interest, and their testosterone showed effects of increased testosterone on genital re- and androstenedione levels were consistently and signifi- sponse to erotic stimuli occurring 3–4h after the peak cantly lower than the breast feeders who reported no increaseinplasmatestosterone(Tuitenetal.2000).These reduction in sexual interest. This finding needs to be results suggest that, at least in those women in whom replicated. testosterone has an effect on their sexuality, cyclical variation in testosterone levels should be manifested by Aging and the menopause Adrenal androgens have been cyclical patterns of sexual interest and/or responsiveness. shown, in a number of studies (Crilly et al. 1979, Orent- reichetal.1984,Bancroft&Cawood1996,Sulcováetal. Steroidal contraceptives There is consistent evidence that 1997, Burger et al. 2000) to decline with age over a combined low-dose oral contraceptives (OCs) lower free relatively wide age span. Ovarian androgens start to testosterone (e.g. Bancroft et al. 1991). Two principle declineafewyearsbeforethemenopause,probablydueto mechanisms are involved: the mid-cycle rise in testoster- a reduction in the mid-cycle rise of testosterone (Roger one is blocked by suppression of ovulation and the et al. 1980, Zumoff et al. 1995, Mushayandebvu et al. associated pattern of gonadal steroid change and the 1996).However,thereisnopredictabledeclineinovarian combined OC increases SHBG levels and hence reduces androgens following the menopause (Bancroft & Cawood the free testosterone available. Given this predictable 1996, Burger et al. 2000). A crucial change in the hormonal effect, what happens to sexual arousability in function of the interstitial cells of the ovary from pre- to JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 www.endocrinology-journals.org Downloaded from Bioscientifica.com at 03/02/2023 12:47:27AM via free access Sexualarousal · JBANCROFT 415 post-menopause complicates the picture. Whereas in All subjects then received 1 month of placebo following pre-menopausalwomen,gonadotrophicstimulationofthe which they were crossed-over to one of the other three interstitial cells is regulated by the negative feedback of treatment groups. The oestrogen+testosterone and testos- ovarian steroids, the rise in LH that accompanies the terone only conditions showed significantly higher levels menopausal transition, resulting from the reduction in of sexual interest, fantasy and arousal than either the oestrogen-induced negative feedback, may stimulate the oestrogen only or placebo conditions. They did not differ interstitial cells to produce testosterone and androstenedi- in measures of sexual activity with a partner or orgasm. one, sometimes excessively. In addition, factors such as Thisistheonlystudyinwhichtestosteroneadministration body weight and insulin resistance influence this ovarian on its own has been evaluated. It is also noteworthy androgen production (for a brief review of the evidence becauseitfocusedontheimmediatepost-operativeperiod see Bancroft & Cawood 1996). in women who were not reporting significant sexual or Anumberofbehaviouralstudieshavereportedadecline mood problems pre-operatively. Mood was significantly insexualinterestinwomenastheyage(seeBancroft1989, better with all three hormone regimes (testosterone, Bancroft et al. 2003). Studies have varied in the extent to oestrogen+testosterone and oestrogen) compared with which the menopause per se contributes to this decline, placebo (Sherwin & Gelfand 1985a) and energy level, although there is consistent evidence of an increase in well-being and appetite were significantly higher in the vaginal dryness related to the change in oestradiol levels two groups receiving testosterone than in the oestrogen (Dennerstein et al. 2003). only or placebo groups (Sherwin & Gelfand 1985b). Dotestosteronelevelscorrelatewithmeasuresofsexual Shifren et al. (2000) studied women who had under- interest or activity as women get older? A number of gone surgical menopause from 1–10 years previously. In cross-sectional studies, using multivariate methods of contrast to the previous study, all had impaired sexual analysis, have found no relation between testosterone and function and all had been on Premarin, at least 0·625mg changing levels of sexual interest (Cawood & Bancroft daily for at least 2 months, when recruited for the study. 1996,Kirchengastetal.1996,Dennersteinetal.1997).In All subjects continued on the same dose of oral oestrogen their longitudinal study of women going through the throughout the study. After a 4-week baseline assess- menopausal transition, assessed over a 9-year period, with ment, they were all given daily transdermal patches with an average starting age of 48 years, Dennerstein et al. placebo,or150µgor300µgtestosteroneasthedailydose, (2003)foundnorelationbetweenandrogenlevelsandany eachfor12weekswiththeorderofpresentationrandom- aspect of sexual functioning. However, in none of these ized. There was a substantial placebo response; however, studies was there any assessment of the pre-menopausal therewassignificantlymoreimprovementwiththehigher testosterone decline. To adequately test the androgen– testosterone dose than with placebo on measures of behaviour relationship during the mid-life period, longi- frequencyofsexualactivity,andpleasure/orgasm,though tudinalstudiesareneededinwhichthebaselinetestoster- not for sexual desire or arousal (the opposite pattern onelevelsareassessedbeforethepre-menopausaldecline. reported by Sherwin et al. 1985). Mood was significantly improved with the higher testosterone dose for ratings of The effects of ovariectomy and hormone replacement With both depression and ‘positive well-being’. The transder- surgical removal of the ovaries there is an immediate and malrouteusedinthisstudyhastheadvantageofproducing substantial drop in circulating androgens. Androgen pro- more physiological and more stable serum testosterone duction by the interstitial cells of the post-menopausal levels than the intramuscular routes used in most other ovary is an important source of oestrogen from peripheral studies, where supra-physiological peaks soon after injec- conversion. Whether pre- or post-menopausal, therefore, tion are followed by gradual decline. the woman with both ovaries removed is in a state of A third study also warrants attention. Nathorst-Böös relative gonadal steroid deficiency. et al. (1993) were unusual in this literature in taking a Most of the literature on hormone replacement, with sample of women who had been oophorectomized and both natural and surgical menopause, has assessed the reporting the proportion who had experienced loss of effects of testosterone in combination with oestradiol, sexual interest. Approximately 50% of women came into compared with oestradiol alone or placebo. The evidence this category, which suggests that around 50% of women from such studies on the role of testosterone in sexual can undergo this depletion of ovarian steroids and not interest and response is inconsistent. Two studies will be experience loss of sexual interest. considered closely. (For a more comprehensive review of this literature see Bancroft 2003.) Hormone replacement of adrenal androgens Androstenedione Sherwin et al. (1985) investigated women who were has a substantial capacity for conversion to both oestradiol about to undergo hysterectomy and bilateral oophorec- and testosterone; dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) has a tomy. Post-operatively, women were assigned randomly verylimitedcapacityforconversiontotestosterone.There to one of four 3-month treatment groups: oestrogen only, islittleevidence,however,thatDHEAisdirectlyrelevant testosterone only, oestrogen plus testosterone or placebo. to sexuality in either men or women, whereas it may be www.endocrinology-journals.org JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 Downloaded from Bioscientifica.com at 03/02/2023 12:47:27AM via free access 416 JBANCROFT · Sexualarousal relevant to well-being and energy, and hence secondarily of women is powerfully influenced by mood, energy and to sexual arousability (Cawood & Bancroft 1996). In a well-being,aswellasbyotherpsychologicalmechanisms. placebo-controlledevaluationofDHEAadministrationto This may account for why, in several studies of younger women between 40 and 70 years (Morales et al. 1994b), women, the relationship between testosterone and sexu- there was significant improvement in a somewhat crude ality was most apparent in women whose sexuality was measure of well-being, but no effect on ‘libido’. In a unproblematic (e.g. Bancroft et al. 1980, Tuiten et al. placebo-controlled study of women with adrenal insuffi- 1996, Riley & Riley 2000). Thus, whereas testosterone ciency, the addition of DHEA to the corticosteroid mayplayaroleinthesexualityofmanywomen,itseffects replacement improved measures of sexual interest and can easily be obscured by the co-existence of other responsiveness as well as mood and general well-being, psychologicaloraffectivefactors.Testosteronealsoappears although the effects on mood were more substantial to have a mood and energy-enhancing effect in women. (Arlt et al. 1999). The sexual effects could therefore have Hence, some of the sexual effects of testosterone may been secondary to improvements in general well-being indirectly result from these mood effects. and energy. Where do androgens act in the brain? Clinicalstudiesofwomenwithlowsexualinterestorarousability AndrogenreceptorstudiesForobviousreasons,wearelargely Stuartetal.(1987)andSchreiner-Engeletal.(1989)found dependentonanimalstudiesonthistopic,althoughmainly no difference in testosterone levels between women primate studies will be considered here. Research on the presenting with low sexual interest and controls; Riley & site of action of testosterone in the non-human primate Riley (2000) found a marginally lower free androgen brain has been ongoing for more than 30 years, during index(FAI)inwomencomplainingoflife-longabsenceof which time there have been major changes in the tech- sexual drive than in controls. Controlled studies of the nologies used, and these have to be kept in mind when treatment with testosterone of women presenting with comparing findings across studies. sexual problems have been few. Although Carney et al. Michael et al. (1989), using autoradiography in male (1978)foundsomebenefitoftestosteronewhencombined rhesus monkeys, looked at aspects of both androgenic and with counselling for couples whose main problem was oestrogenic activity. They found 5(cid:2)-reductase activity to sexual unresponsiveness of the woman, other comparable be fairly evenly distributed in the brain, whereas, in studies failed to replicate this effect (Mathews et al. 1983, contrast, aromatase activity was much more localized. Dow & Gallasher 1989). They assumed that in areas of high aromatase activity, In a more experimental study, Tuiten et al. (1996) testosterone was acting by conversion to oestradiol. By studied eight young amenorrhoeic women with low identifying brain areas where there was overlap in weight, although none had the diagnosis of anorexia androgen- and oestrogen-concentrating neurons, they nervosa. They showed lower levels of sexual interest and concluded that the medial pre-optic and ventro-medial activity, and lower testosterone levels than a comparison hypothalamic nuclei, the bed nucleus of the stria termi- group of normally menstruating aged-matched women. nalis, and the cortical, medial and accessory amygdaloid The amenorrhoeic group was given testosterone unde- nuclei were the main sites at which the effects of testos- canoate(40mg)dailyfor8weeks,andplacebofor8weeks terone are mediated predominantly by oestradiol. In con- in a double-blind cross-over study. They were evaluated trast, testosterone activity was thought to be mediated by in a psychophysiology laboratory with measurement of androgenreceptorsinthelateralseptal,pre-mamillaryand vaginal pulse amplitude (VPA) in response to erotic intercalated mamillary nuclei. fantasiesanderoticfilms.VPAwassignificantlygreaterin Roselli et al. (2001) used in situ hybridization histo- response to film in the testosterone-treated condition. chemistry to locate cytochrome P450 aromatase mRNA However,thiseffectwasnotreflectedinsubjectiveratings andandrogenreceptormRNA(ARmRNA)inthehypo- of arousal. Nor did the two treatments differ in terms of thalamus and amygdala of cynomolgus monkeys. Their dailyratingsofsexualityormood.Thisexperimentthere- findings were broadly similar to those of Michael et al. fore demonstrated an effect of increasing testosterone on (1989). They concluded that testosterone acts through physiologicalresponsetoeroticstimulation,whichwasnot signallingpathwaysthatdiffereitherinspecificbrainareas, apparent in any subjective or mood measures. or within different cells from the same area. Summary of androgen effects in women Whereas there is Abdelgadir et al. (1999) studied the distribution of evidencethattestosteronewithdrawaland/orreplacement ARmRNA,usingaribonucleaseprotectionassay,inmale can have effects on women’s sexuality, the evidence is rhesusmonkeys.TheirlocalizationoftheARmRNAwas inconsistent and sometimes contradictory. One obvious again broadly similar to the distribution of androgen explanation is that women vary in the extent to which activity reported by Michael et al. (1989). In this study, their sexuality is influenced by testosterone, and several intactanimalswerecomparedwithcastratedanimalswith examplesofevidencesupportingthishavebeenpresented. and without testosterone replacement. They found little It is also becoming increasingly clear that the sexuality difference between these three groups in the distribution JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 www.endocrinology-journals.org Downloaded from Bioscientifica.com at 03/02/2023 12:47:27AM via free access Sexualarousal · JBANCROFT 417 orconcentrationofARmRNA,andconcludedthatinthe androgens have in these areas is not yet clear, but in monkey brain this androgen receptor is not regulated at addition to more specifically reproductive actions, other the transcriptional level by androgen. relativelynon-specificandrogen-mediatedbrainfunctions For both non-human primates and humans, the ma- need to be considered, including activation or general jority of evidence is from males. However, Finley & arousal as well as stimulation of neuronal growth and Kritzer(1999),reportingthepresenceofandrogenrecep- gender differentiation of brain function (Kelly 1991). tor protein in the pre-frontal cortex of rhesus monkeys, Brain imaging Functional brain imaging promises to throw found no differences between their male and female considerable light on brain activity related to sexual monkeysinthisrespect.Inastudyofhumans,Fernandez- function, but is still at an early stage of development, Guasti et al. (2000) used immunochemical methods to withmethodologicalinconsistenciesacrossstudiesmaking examine the distribution of androgen receptors in the interpretation of the findings more difficult (Sumich hypothalamus, involving post-mortem samples from five et al. 2003, Mouras & Stoleru 2005). So far, only two men and five women. In men, intense androgen receptor studiesprovideevidenceofdirectrelevancetotestosterone immunoreactivity was found in neurons of the horizontal activity. Park et al. (2001) reported on two hypogonadal limb of the diagonal band of Broca, and of the lateral men,assessedwithfunctionalmagneticresonanceimaging mamillary and medial mamillary nuclei. Intermediate in their response to sexual stimuli, with and without staining was found in other parts of the diagonal band of testosterone replacement. In both men, activation of the Broca, the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the pre-optic inferior frontal lobe, cingulate gyrus, insula and corpus area, paraventricular, suprachiasmatic, ventromedial and callosum was greater with testosterone replacement. infundibularnuclei.Weakerstainingwasfoundinthebed Redoute et al. (2005) compared positron emission tom- nucleusofthestriaterminalis,medialpre-opticarea,dorsal ographyscanevidenceofbrainactivityduringresponseto andventralzonesoftheperiventricularnucleus,supraoptic sexualstimuliinninehypogonadalmenwithandwithout nucleus and nucleus basalis of Meynert. In most areas, testosteronereplacement,andeighteugonadalmen.They women revealed less staining of receptor protein. This found greater activation in the controls and the treated gender difference was particularly marked in the lateral hypogonadal men than the untreated, in the right orbito- andmedialmammillarynuclei.Therewasalsostainingin frontal cortex, insula and claustrum. They also found the paraventricular and supra-optic nuclei in the males, deactivationoftheleftinferiorfrontalgyrus,suggestiveof whichwasapparentlyabsentinthefemales.Overall,there reduced inhibition of sexual arousal, but only in the wasgreaterindividualvariabilityintheintensityofrecep- controls and treated patients. There is some consistency tor staining in the women than in the men. acrossthesetwostudies,andandrogenreceptorshavebeen Asyet,therearenocomparativedataonthedistribution reportedintheorbito-frontalcortexofprimates(Finley& ofaromataseactivityinwomenorfemaleprimates,though Kritzer 1999) and to a limited extent, in the cingulate in the rat greater amounts of aromatase activity are found cortex (Abdelgadir et al. 1999). As yet, however, there is in the male than the female, in all areas examined except no evidence of androgen receptors in the insula or the medial preoptic nucleus (Roselli & Resko 1993). claustrumofprimatesorhumans.Sofar,therefore,thereis Furthermore, Roselli & Klosterman (1998) concluded only limited overlap between localization of testosterone from their findings that this male pattern of aromatization effects in the brain when comparing androgen receptors in the rat is established perinatally, and probably forms a and functional brain-imaging studies. This may in part fundamental component of masculinization. reflect methodological limitations of brain imaging; some Thelimitedevidenceofandrogenreceptordistribution relevant areas of the brain, especially the hypothalamus, in humans includes two studies of the temporal cortex, aremoredifficulttoimageinthisway.Butweshouldalso both of which found substantial amounts of androgen keepinmindthatthesetwotechniquesarelookingatvery receptor protein (Sarrieau et al. 1990, Puy et al. 1995). different markers of brain activity – steroid receptors and Bothstudiesinvolvedmaletemporalcortextissuebecause localized oxygen uptake. Physiological levels of testoster- it was available from surgical interventions in men with one may be necessary for a more generalized pattern of intractable epilepsy, not because the temporal cortex was interactivebrainactivity,muchofwhichisnotdependent regardedasaparticularlylikelysiteforandrogenreceptors. on direct testosterone effects. Whereasmuchofthereportedlocalizationisconsistent withandrogeniceffectsonsexualandreproductivebehav- Oestrogens and male sexuality Evidence of the effects iour, it is important to keep in mind that much of the of oestrogens on the sexuality of the human male, while research has been restricted to the hypothalamus and, limited, consistently suggests a negative effect of exogen- where more extensive studies have been done, androgen ousoestrogens.Oestradiolappearstoplayakeyroleinthe receptors appear to be widespread in the primate and negative feedback control of testosterone. In long-term human brain, including various cortical areas (e.g. pre- castrated rhesus monkeys, physiological doses of testoster- frontal cortex: Finley & Kritzer 1999, temporal cortex: onedonotreduceLHunlessasmallamountofoestradiol Sarrieau et al. 1990, Puy et al. 1995). What specific role isalsogiven(Reskoetal.1977).Also,menwitharomatase www.endocrinology-journals.org JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 Downloaded from Bioscientifica.com at 03/02/2023 12:47:27AM via free access 418 JBANCROFT · Sexualarousal deficiency or oestrogen resistance show raised LH and EE, l-norgestrel, combination of EE and l-norgestrel, follicle-stimulating hormone (see Roselli et al. 2001). and placebo. The EE only regime was significantly better In the past, oestrogens were used as a treatment for than the others in improving sexual interest, enjoyment, prostatic carcinoma, based on their assumed anti-andro- orgasmic frequency and also mood. Also found were geniceffect,andreductionofsexualinterestandresponse correlations between measures of sexual desire and of wasatypicalside-effect.Oestrogenshavealsobeenusedas mood, particularly ‘feelings of well-being’. a treatment of sexual offenders. Bancroft et al. (1974) Only one study of hormone replacement has incor- compared the effects of ethinyl oestradiol (EE) and CPA, porated different doses of oestradiol. Sherwin (1991) an anti-androgen, in 12 incarcerated sexual offenders. randomlyassigned‘perimenopausal’womentofourtreat- Both compounds reduced sexual interest, masturbatory ment regimes, involving either a low (0·625mg) or activity and erectile response to erotic fantasies and slides high (1·25mg) dose of Premarin, combined with either producingapatternverysimilartothatfoundinhypogo- Provera (5mg) or placebo. After a baseline month of nadalmen.However,althoughCPAsubstantiallyreduced assessment, each woman took the assigned regime for 12 total testosterone, with no change in SHBG, EE substan- months, and was assessed with daily ratings during the tiallyincreasedbothtotaltestosteroneandSHBG(Murray 3rd, 6th, 9th and 12th months. The main purpose of et al. 1975). the study was to evaluate the effects of the progestagen How can we reconcile these negative effects with the on mood and sexuality, not to compare the different widespread interaction between testosterone and oestra- doses of oestradiol. However, apparent from the graphed diol in the male brain? Is it possible that, in addition to sexualinterestdata,womeninthehighoestradiol+placebo interactionbetweentestosteroneandoestradiolinnegative group had substantially lower levels of sexual interest at feedbackcontrolofLH,someoftheeffectsoftestosterone baseline (i.e. pre-treatment), than the low oestradiol+ onsexualinterestandarousabilityaremediatedbyaroma- placebo group, yet by the 6th month of treatment and tizationoftestosteronetooestradiol?Gooren(1985)found continuing through the 12th month they were showing that,ineugonadalmen,theoestrogenreceptorantagonist, noticeably higher levels of sexual interest. This was not tamoxifen, and the aromatase inhibitor, testolactone, had commented on in the paper. It would appear to indi- no adverse sexual effects, and that dihydrotestosterone cate that the effects of oestradiol on sexual interest are (DHT) was as effective as testosterone in maintaining dose related. sexuality in hypogonadal men. He took this to mean that So to what extent are the effects of testosterone, the sexual effects of testosterone within the CNS are discussedearlier,theresultofaromatizationoftestosterone mediated by DHT but not by oestradiol. to oestradiol or, alternatively, of increased levels of free Whereas in other species, the role of oestradiol in oestradiol resulting from testosterone-induced reduction mediating many of the central nervous system effects of in SHBG? Wallen & Parsons (1998) have strongly advo- testosterone on male sexual behaviour is well established cated that the sexual effects of testosterone in women (Lindzey&Korach2003),itsroleinhumanmalesexuality result from the consequent increase in free oestradiol, remains unclear, with the possibility that exogenous and supporting this conclusion with experimental data from endogenous sources of oestradiol have fundamentally dif- rhesus monkeys. At this stage we should keep an open ferenteffects,particularlywhentheendogenousoestradiol mind about the role of testosterone and oestradiol in resultsfromaromatizationoftestosteronewithinthebrain. female sexuality. Oestrogens and female sexuality There is consistent Mode of action of androgens and oestrogens in the evidenceoftheimportanceofoestradiolfornormalvaginal brain The conventional view is that androgens act in the lubrication. However, whether oestradiol has a direct brain via genomic effects (Rommerts 1990). It has been effect on sexual interest and arousability remains unclear. known for some time, however, that oestradiol and pro- Anumberofstudiesofthesymptomsofeithernaturalor gesterone can, in addition, have much more rapid effects surgical menopause, reviewed above, have either taken directlyonthecellmembrane(Dufy&Vincent1980).As women on oestradiol replacement, who continue to have yet we have few directly relevant data on the speed of lossofsexualinterestorresponse,andevaluatedtheeffect action of androgens in the human, although one study in of adding testosterone, or have compared oestradiol re- women, discussed earlier (Tuiten et al. 2000), showed an placement with a combination of oestradiol and testoster- effectongenitalresponsewithin3to4h.Caldwell(2002) one,ortestosteronealone.Muchlessattentionhasfocused has proposed a model of steroid-influenced sexual arous- on the effects of oestradiol itself on sexual interest and ability, which depends on membrane-associated receptors arousal in post-menopausal women. linkedtoamoreenduringchangeinG-proteincoupling. Dennerstein et al. (1980) studied 49 surgically meno- However, the evidence he cites is largely restricted to pausal women, all of whom had stable, satisfying sexual oestradiol and progesterone. It remains a possibility that relationships, using a double-blind cross-over design in androgens may produce a relatively rapid membrane- which they spent 3 months on each of four treatments, mediated effect by means of aromatization. JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 www.endocrinology-journals.org Downloaded from Bioscientifica.com at 03/02/2023 12:47:27AM via free access Sexualarousal · JBANCROFT 419 Comparison of gonadal steroid effects in men and peripheral, classically ‘hormonal’ effects that may occur. women As yet, there is little evidence that would allow Towhatextenttheseintra-cerebraleffectsinvolvesynap- comparisonoftheeffectsofoestradiolonmaleandfemale tic transmission, ‘parasynaptic’ transmission through local sexuality. With testosterone, there is much more relevant diffusionordisseminationviathecerebrospinalfluidisnot evidence, allowing the following conclusions. clear,butallmaybeinvolved,togetherwithconsiderable (1) The evidence is more consistent for the male than potential for interaction with other neurotransmitters as for the female. Apart from methodological considerations, well as steroid hormones. The limbic system is character- which are more complex in women than in men, poten- ized by peptidergic neurotransmission, contrasting with tiallythemostimportantfactoraccountingforthisgender the synaptic neuronal structure of the cortex (Herbert differenceisagreatervariabilityofbehaviouralresponsive- 1993)andconsistentwiththemoreprimitivephylogenetic ness to androgens among women. origins of the brainstem. The specific effect of a peptide (2) Those women who are behaviourally responsive to may depend on its site of action, or even the context in testosteronerespondtolevelsoftestosteronewhichwould which it is acting. In this review, attention will be be totally ineffective in men. This greater sensitivity to restricted to oxytocin (OT), (cid:1)-endorphin and prolactin testosteroneinwomenismostapparentinbehaviouraland (PRL) as representatives of the main types of peptides other CNS responses and may be less marked in the relevant to sexual arousal, and most of the cited evidence anabolic and skin effects of testosterone. is from animal studies. (3) In the male, the evidence points to a threshold, below the normal physiological range, above which in- OxytocinItisgenerallyacceptedthatOTplaysakeyrole creased testosterone levels have little behavioural effect, during lactation, facilitating the milk ejection reflex. It andbelowwhichsignsofandrogendeficiencyarelikelyto may also play a role in facilitating uterine contraction occur.Thisthresholdconceptdoesnotappeartoapplyto during parturition. For such purposes, OT is produced in women. The most convincing behavioural effects of the the magnocellular neurons of the paraventricular nucleus administration of exogenous androgens to women have (PVN)ofthehypothalamus,whichprojectaxonsintothe involved supra-physiological levels, although in a number posterior pituitary and thence into the peripheral circula- of studies sensitivity to these levels appears to decrease tion (Kupfermann 1991). OT has also been proposed as a overtime.Ontheotherhand,itremainspossiblethatsigns key factor in affiliative behaviour (Insel 1992). The evi- ofandrogendeficiencywilloccurinwomenifcirculating dence for OT being important in sexual arousal, response levelsoftestosteronefalltoolow,thecriticalleveldepend- and behaviour is, however, less consistent. In animal ing on a particular woman’s sensitivity. studies, centrally administered OT has induced erection, (4) A fundamental difference in male and female an effect which is apparently testosterone dependent, and mechanisms of androgen production needs to be kept in OT receptor antagonists can prevent non-contact erec- mind. More than 90% of testosterone in the male is tions, considered an index of sexual arousal (Argiolas producedbythetestes.Inwomen,asubstantialproportion 1999).Dopamineagonistsmayenhancesexualresponseby of androgen production is from the adrenal glands and increasing central oxytocinergic transmission (Argiolas hence increased adrenal androgens can be expected in 1999).Infemalerats,studieswithOTantagonistsindicate states associated with increased adrenal activity, such as thatOTfacilitateslordosis,aneffectapparentlydependent anxiety, stress or depression (e.g. Weber et al. 2000). on progesterone priming. Less striking effects on procep- Given the strong relationship between mood and sexual- tive female behaviour (hopping and darting) were also ity, which has been demonstrated particularly in women reported.Therewasalsoanincreaseinvocalizationswhen (e.g. Bancroft et al. 2003), this aspect of testosterone mounted,consistentwiththetactilestimulationleadingto function warrants closer attention. lordosis and mounting becoming aversive (Insel 1992). This may be related to the effects of vagino-cervical stimulation (VCS), which has been shown to induce Peptides analgesia in rats. Komisaruk & Sansone (2003) have Notonlyisthereaseeminglylimitlessnumberofpeptides proposed that this analgesic mechanism, which may have tobefoundacrossspecies,itisapparentthattheyactina a fundamental role in reproduction by making mounting varietyofways;functioninglikeclassical‘hormones’being and intromission less noxious for the female, in part released into the circulation to act at a distance, but also involvesOT.TheyalsopresentevidencethatVCSresults acting as neurotransmitters or local regulators, leading in a release of OT from PVN neurons, which project to Polak & Bloom (1982) to call them ‘regulatory peptides’. thebrainstemandspinalcord,activatingthesympathetic Although peptides are released in many different parts division of the autonomic nervous system in the process. of the body, neuropeptides are produced in substantial Cantoretal.(1999)examinedtheinteractionofOTand amounts within the CNS. With the evolution of the serotonin. Having demonstrated that fluoxetine, a selec- blood–brain barrier, the mammalian brain has been able tive serotonin re-uptake inhibitor, impaired ejaculation to develop peptidergic communication independent of in male rats and also reduced appetitive behaviours www.endocrinology-journals.org JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 Downloaded from Bioscientifica.com at 03/02/2023 12:47:27AM via free access 420 JBANCROFT · Sexualarousal (level changing), they found that OT administration (cid:1)-endorphinTherehasbeen,formanyyears,widespread reversedtheeffectoffluoxetineonejaculationbutnoton awareness of the negative effects of exogenous opiates, appetitive behaviour. The authors concluded that sero- suchasmorphineandheroin,onthesexualityofmaleand tonin suppresses ejaculation by interrupting the action of female drug addicts, reducing sexual interest, impairing OT.TheysuggestedthattheeffectsofOTadministration genitalresponseandblockingejaculationandorgasm,with could have been mediated by a central stimulation of active and spontaneous reversal of such effects often sympatheticoutflow,orperipherallybyfacilitatingsmooth occurring during opiate withdrawal (for review see Pfaus muscle contraction in the reproductive tract. & Gorzalka 1987). In general, the subsequent animal Thus, so far the animal evidence is consistent with researchhasshownsimilarnegativeeffectsofbothexogen- both a peripheral and central role for OT; the most ous and endogenous opiate administration, in male and obvious peripheral effect being facilitation of smooth femaleanimals,withconfirmatoryevidencefromblocking muscle contraction – the central role being more as a such effects with opiate antagonists such as naloxone, as neuromodulator in a variety of response systems. Both well as withdrawal effects comparable with those in peripheral and central mechanisms, however, appear to humans.Thepictureiscomplicatedbythefactthatopiates be steroid hormone dependent. The enhancement of inhibitLH-releasinghormone,andhencereduceLHand erections is unlikely to result from the peripheral effects testosterone. However, the animal evidence shows that of OT given that they most obviously facilitate muscle thesexuallyinhibitingeffectsarepredominantlyindepen- contraction (necessary for ejaculation), whereas erection dent of this reduction of testosterone. is dependent on muscle relaxation. However, whereas (cid:1)-endorphinistheendogenousopiatethathasreceived the central role of OT in lactation remains the most research attention. Apart from the anterior unchallenged, its fundamental role in sexual behaviour is pituitary, the synthesis of (cid:1)-endorphin is limited to two questioned by the effects of OT gene ablation studies. cell groups in the brain, one in the arcuate nucleus of the OT knockout mice show clear impairment of milk hypothalamus,theotherinthenucleusofthesolitarytract let-down, but no apparent impairment of either mating in the brainstem. The relevant neurons in the arcuate or parturition (Nishimori et al. 1996, Young et al. 1996). nucleus project anteriorly to other parts of the hypothala- What evidence do we have for humans? Carmichael mus, including the medial pre-optic area, and also to the et al. (1987) found that plasma OT increased around the amygdala. Dorsally, neurons run to the PVN of the time of orgasm in men and women, remaining raised for hypothalamus and then on to the brainstem to structures at least 5min after orgasm. The authors postulated that involvedintheautonomicnervoussystem(Herbert1995). OT has a facilitatory role on sperm and egg transport by The sexual inhibiting effects of (cid:1)-endorphin, and related increasing smooth muscle contractility in the reproduc- opiatepeptides,arebelievedtooccurmainlythroughtheir tive tracts. In a recent study of men, OT increased in actiononthepre-opticareaandtheamygdala,theprecise some subjects following ejaculation, but the individual inhibiting effect depending on the site of infusion. Thus variability was such that the group effect was not (cid:1)-endorphininfusedintothemedialpre-opticareainhib- significant (Kruger et al. 2003a). Murphy et al. (1987) its consummatory mounting and intromission, providing reported an increase in OT in men during sexual that mounting has not already started with a particular arousal, which persisted beyond ejaculation, but with no female;inotherwords,inhibitionisoftheactivationofthe obvious increase at ejaculation. In a study of women, consummatory sequence rather than its completion. On Blaicher et al. (1999) found an increase in OT 1min the other hand, infusion of (cid:1)-endorphin into the medial after orgasm, but levels were close to baseline by 5min amygdala inhibits the initial appetitive phase (Herbert post-orgasm. 1995).AccordingtoArgiolas(1999)theinhibitoryeffectis It is difficult to draw clear conclusions from this dose dependent, with low doses of opiate having facilita- literature on OT and sexual arousal. Whether the tory and high doses inhibitory effects. (cid:1)-endorphin may increase of OT around orgasm, which has been facilitate appetitive behaviour by acting on the ventral somewhat inconsistently observed in the human tegmental area to activate the mesolimbic dopaminergic literature, has any specific function, rather than being an system. This raises a crucial question, at least about epiphenomenon of other changes, remains uncertain, exogenous opiates in humans, which remains largely though it is possible that this OT rise will affect the unanswered. Exogenous opiates can induce an intense experience of orgasm by influencing uterine and other feeling of pleasure, which has been likened to an orgasm, reproductive tract smooth musculature. The possibility followed by a state of relaxation and calm. What are the that a peri-orgasmic increase in OT contributes to the mechanisms underlying this? Is the activation of the post-orgasmic refractory period, at least in males, should mesolimbicdopaminergicsystem,whichisassociatedwith also be considered. Caldwell (2002) has proposed that ‘appetite’ for a variety of behaviours, including sexual OT is a satiety hormone that acts by de-coupling the (Hull et al. 1998), relevant? ‘Appetite’, however, is not G-protein (see above) and hence reducing sexual the same as intense pleasure, the origins of which, and arousability. The reader is entitled to feel confused. of sexual orgasm also, remain obscure. A fair amount of JournalofEndocrinology(2005)186,411–427 www.endocrinology-journals.org Downloaded from Bioscientifica.com at 03/02/2023 12:47:27AM via free access

Description:
state motivated towards the experience of sexual pleasure and possibly orgasm . in the human male, relevant to sexual arousal, are as yet unclear. It is also . sexual interest and mood were the best predictors of discontinuation of
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.