ebook img

Sleep and Cognition – American Psychological Association APA PDF

209 Pages·2016·13.59 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Sleep and Cognition – American Psychological Association APA

SLEEP COGNITION AND E D I T E D BY RICHARD R. BOOTZIN, J O H N F. KIH L STROM AND DANIEL 1. SCHACTER UNIVERSITY OF ARIZONA Arne r ica n Psyc hologi ca I Association Washington, DC C O N T E N T S Contributors ix Foreword X Preface xii PART ONE: GENERAL OVERVIEWS 1 The Neurocognition of Sleep Mentation: Rapid 1 Eye Movements, Visual Imagery, and Dreaming John Antrobus 2 Activation, Input Source, and Modulation: A 25 Neurocognitive Model of the State of the Brain-Mind J. Allan Hobson PART TWO: COGNITION DURING SLEEP 3 Event-Related Brain Potential (ERP) Studies of 43 Cognition During Sleep: Is It More Than a Dream? Marta Kutas Stimulus Control and Sleep 58 4 John Harsh and Pietro Badia 5 Memories in Sleep: Old and New 67 Pietro Badia vii 6 Behavioral Responses During Sleep 77 Frederick J. Evans 7 Learning During Sleep 88 Eric Eich 8 Lucid Dreaming: Psychophysiological Studies of 109 Consciousness During REM Sleep Stephen LaBerge 9 Cognitive Processing and General Anesthesia 127 Les Goldmann PART THREE: COGNITION BEFORE AND AFTER SLEEP 10 Insomnia: The Patient and the Pill 139 WallaceB . Mendelson 1 1 The Perception of Sleep Onset in Insomniacs and 148 Normal Sleepers Michael H. Bonnet 12 Are You Awake? Cognitive Performance and 159 Reverie During the Hypnopompic State David F. Dinges PART FOUR CLINICAL TOPICS 13 A Network Model of Dreams 179 Rosalind Cartwright 14 Nightmares (Dream Disturbances) in 190 Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Implications for a Theory of Dreaming Milton Kramer Index 203 viii C O N T R I B U T O R S John Antrobus, The City College of the City University of New York Pietro Badia, Bowling Green State University Michael H. Bonnet, Long Beach VeteransA dministration Hospital and University of California, Irvine Rosalind Cartwright, Rush-Presbyterian-St. Luke's Hospital David F. Dinges, University of Pennsylvania Eric Eich, The University of British Columbia Frederick J. Evans, University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey Les Goldmann, Reed College John Harsh, Universityo f Southern Mississippi J. Allan Hobson, Harvard Medical School Milton Kramer, Sleep Disorders Center of Greater Cincinnati Marta Kutas, University of California, San Diego Stephen LaBerge, Stanford University Wallace B. Mendelson, State Universityo f New York at Stony Brook ix F O R E W O R D Federal research agencies stopped regularly supporting investigator-initiated “state-of-the-art” research conferences in scientific psychology well over a de- cade ago. Yet over that same period, scientific psychology has continued to grow-as well as to diversify into many new areas. Thus there have been rela- tively few opportunities for investigators in new and cutting-edge research ar- eas to convene in special settings to discuss their findings. The American Psychological Association (APA), as part of its continu- ing efforts to enhance the dissemination of scientific knowledge in psychology, undertook a number of new initiatives designed to foster scientific research and communication. In particular, the APA Science Directorate, in 1988, ini- tiated the Scientific Conferences Program. The APA ScientificC onferences Program provides university-based psy- chological researchers with seed monies essential to organizing specialty con- ferences on critical issues in basic research, applied research, and methodologi- cal issues in psychology. Deciding which conferences to support involves a competitive process. An annual call for proposals is issued by the APA Science Directorate to solicit conference ideas. Proposals from all areas of psychologi- cal research are welcome. They are then reviewed by qualified psychologists, who forward substantive suggestions and funding recommendations to the Science Directorate. At each stage, the criteria used to determine which con- ferences to support include relevance, timeliness, and comprehensiveness of the topics, and qualifications of the presenters. In 1988, seven conferences were funded under the APA Science Directorate program’s sponsorship, and six conferences were funded in 1989. We expect to fund six more in 1990, at an annual program expense of $90,000 to $100,000. The APA Scientific Conferences Program has two major goals. The first is to provide, by means of the conferences, a broad view of specific topics (and, when appropriate, to provide for interdisciplinary participation). The second goal is to assure timely dissemination of the findings presented by publishing a series of carefully crafted scholarly volumes based, in part, on each conference. Thus the information reaches the audiences at each conference well as the as broader psychological and scientific communities. This enables psychology and related fields to benefit from the most current research on a given topic. X This volume presents findings reported at the January 1989 conference, “Sleep and Cognition.” Researchers in this area are focusing on a number of fascinating topics. They are applying the methods of contemporary cognitive psychology to traditional areas of interest such as the purpose of dreams and our potential to learn during sleep, well as exploring newer dimensions like as lucid dreaming, cognitive processes under anesthesia, and effects of sleepin- ducing medication. Researchers from the typically separate areas of sleep and cognition came together at the conferencet o exchange ideas and focus on new directions for research. This volume is representative of what we at the American Psychological Association believe will be a number of exceptional volumes that give readers a broad sampling of the diverse and outstanding research now being done in scientific psychology. We hope you will enjoy and be stimulated by this book and the many others to come. A list of the conferences funded through this program follows: Researching Community Psychology: Integrating Theories and Methodolo- gies, September 1988 Psychological Well-Being in Nonhuman Captive Primates, September 1988 Psychological Research on Organ Donation, October 1988 Arizona Conference on Sleep and Cognition, January 1989 Socially Shared Cognition, February 1989 The Role of Experience in ModifyingT aste and Its Effects on Feeding, April 1989 Perception of Structure, May 1989 Suggestibility of Children’s Recollections, June 1989 Best Methods for the Analysis of Change, October 1989 Conceptualization and Measurement of Organism-Environment Interaction, November 1989 Cognitive Bases of Musical Communication, April 1990 Conference on Hostility, Coping/Support, and Health, November 1990 Psychological Testing of Hispanics, February 199 1 Study of Cognition: Conceptual and Methodological Issues, February 199 1 Gary R. VandenBos, PhD Virginia E. Holt Acting Executive Director Manager, Scientific Conferences Program Science Directorate, APA Science Directorate, APA xi P R E F A C E Sleep was once a phenomenon of considerable interest to psychologists. One of Freud’s most popular books (and arguably one of his best) dealt with dreams. In the classic 1924 study by Jenkins and Dallenbach, sleep provided convincing evidence in favor of the interference theory of forgetting. More than 50 years later, and almost a century after Freud, some of the most fre- quently asked questions in the introductory psychology course concern what dreams mean and why we seem to forget them. Despite impressive advances in understanding sleep as a biological phenomenon and in understanding the nature of mental processes in the normal waking state, our knowledge of men- tal life during sleep remains extremely impoverished. Much of the current scientific interest in sleep can be traced to the dis- covery in 1953, by Aserinsky and Kleitmen, of the association between rapid eye movements and dreaming. Ironically, however, the discovery of physio- logically different sleep stages, and their differential association with dreaming and other mental activities, seems to have transformed sleep from a psycholog- ical topic to a biological one. A great deal of work addresses sleep as behavior at a purely biological level, as in studies of the ontogeny and phylogeny of sleep and other biological rhythms. And many major medical centers offer the services of a sleep disorders clinic oriented toward such problems as insomnia (a common symptom in depression and normal aging), narcolepsy, sleep ap- nea, and parasomnias such as enuresis and somnambulism. But surprisingly little current research focuses on perceptual, memory, and thought processes during this period of activity. There is, of course, a long tradition of work by psychologists on the effects of sleep deprivation on human performance. But research on cognitive processes during sleep itself has received comparatively little attention, both on the conference circuit and in print. Examination of Sleep Research, the annual archival publication of the Association of Profes- sional Sleep Societies, reveals relatively little cognitive psychology. Similar trends are apparent in the published proceedings of the biennial European Conferences on Sleep Research and in the volumes of Advances in Sleep Re- search. There is, of course, a large literature (arising primarily from within the psychoanalytic tradition) concerning the interpretation of dreams. Unfortu- xii nately, the methodology in these studies is generally clinical apd hermeneutic, without the careful controls characteristic of experimental psychology. Nevertheless, important questions about sleep persist that must be ad- dressed at a purely psychological level: How much information from the envi- ronment can be processed by the sleeper? What sorts of mental activities are associated with sleepwalkinga nd sleeptalking? Are dreams meaningful, and if so why aren’t they better remembered? What are the possibilities for learning during sleep? Such questions have been asked by psychologists and others, but only rarely have they been addressed with the concepts, principles, and methods of contemporary cognitive psychology. One factor contributing to this situation is the isolation of psychoanalysis, with its interest in dreams, from mainstream scientific psychology. Another is the phenomenology of sleep itself: The sleeper typically appears oblivious to his or her environment, and the occurrence of sleep is generally inferred from this lack of responsive- ness and the absence of conscious awareness of events during sleep. The com- mon identification of cognition with consciousness leaves the impression that little or no mental activity occurs during sleep. Of course, studies in which sleepers are awakened at various points in the night’s sleep typically yield reports of thoughts, images, and dreams, but these are rarely remembered spontaneously in the morning. Some individuals engage in episodes of sleepwalking and sleeptalking, which can involve rela- tively complex speech acts and behavioral sequences-but, again, these are rarely if ever remembered. One explanation for this universally experienced memory deficit is that the cortical centers that mediate complex mental pro- cessing are disengaged or deactivated during sleep, with the result that the sleeper does not attend to environmental and mental events occurring during periods of sleep. Because these events are not noticed, they are not processed in a manner that encodes accessible traces of them in memory. Thus, the most commonly accepted explanation of postsleep amnesia is in terms of consolidation failure. For example, it has been proposed that the low level of cortical arousal characteristic of sleep effectively prevents the sleeper from performing the cognitive operations necessary to encode memory traces of sleep events that are accessible in the subsequent waking state. Ac- cording to this view, dreams and other sleep events are remembered only when the sleeper awakens during them, permitting retrieval from short-term mem- ory. If the sleeper awakens shortly after a dream has occurred, residual infor- mation retrieved from short-term memory may serve a cue to the retrieval as of a highly degraded long-term memory trace. If retrieval is delayed until all traces of the dream have decayed or been displaced from short-term memory, the long-term traces will be virtually inaccessible. As intuitively appealing as this explanation is, recent empirical work on sleep, well theoretical advances in the study of cognition, call it into as as account. For example, the idea of consolidation failure assumes a rigid distinc- tion between short-term and long-term memory that is not supported by the xiii current literature. Moreover, cognitive theorists have recently begun to distin- guish between effortful processes, which require intention and consume atten- tional resources, and automatic processes, which do not. More important, the domain of automatic processing has expanded to include rather complex mental activities-the kind that ordinarily would be expected to leave residual traces in permanent memory. Finally, studies of both brain-damaged patients and intact subjects support a concept of implicit perception and memory, in which current and past events may influence ongoing experience, thought, and action even though the individual lacks awareness (concurrent or retro- spective) of the events themselves. Recent research on sleep processes bears directly on these theoretical ideas. While sleepers are ordinarily considered unable to engage in complex, intentional cognitive activities during sleep, some evidence tends to contradict this assumption. For example, in the phenomenon of lucid dreaming, selected subjects report that they become aware of the fact that they are dreaming and are able to consciously direct the contents of the dream, while remaining asleep. This claim is difficult to verify objectively, for obvious reasons. But a series of highly provocative and apparently well-controlled studies has shown that subjects can make discriminative responses to verbal suggestions deliv- ered while they are asleep. Although the possibility of sleep learning had been firmly rejected in a widely influential review published in 1955, studies of memory of events occurring during surgical anesthesia strongly suggest that sleep learning may be possible, provided that its success is measured in terms of implicit rather than explicit memory. These preliminary findings, which have emerged from a variety of laboratories employing rather different para- digms, suggest that cognitive activity of considerable complexity may be possi- ble during sleep, provided that it is mediated by automated procedures and assessed by measures that do not require awareness of the events on the part of the subject. Despite the research possibilities offered by current advances in theory, most texts on sleep, whether intended for undergraduate use or scholarly refer- ence, pay little attention to cognitive processes, aside from our ubiquitous fail- ure to remember our dreams. A major exception to this generalization is The Mind in Sleep (Arkin, Antrobus, & Ellman, 1978), which provided a compre- hensive account of research on cognitive processes during sleep since the dis- covery of the EEG correlates of dreaming. The volume focuses on such topics as the difference between stage REM and stage NREM mental activity, factors affecting dream recall, the effects of presleep and intrasleep stimulation, REM deprivation, sleeptalking, and night terrors. Unfortunately, the book is now out of date, especially with respect to current thinking in cognitive psychology; although a new edition has been promised, it is not yet available. Another primary source is a recent monograph, Dreaming: A Cognitive-Psychological Analysis (Foulkes, 1985). As comprehersive as the Arkin et al. anthology is, its contributors have almost nothing to say about the problem that concerned Freud most: the meaning of dreams. Foulkes’ book approaches this problem xiv

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.