ebook img

Salient Accented, but Not Unaccented Words Reveal Accent Adaptation Effects PDF

17 Pages·2016·1.19 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Salient Accented, but Not Unaccented Words Reveal Accent Adaptation Effects

View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Frontiers - Publisher Connector ORIGINALRESEARCH published:07June2016 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00864 The Penefit of Salience: Salient Accented, but Not Unaccented Words Reveal Accent Adaptation Effects Ann-KathrinGrohe*andAndreaWeber PsycholinguisticsandAppliedLanguageStudies,EnglishDepartment,FacultyofHumanities,UniversityofTübingen, Germany In two eye-tracking experiments, the effects of salience in accent training and speech accentednessonspoken-wordrecognitionwereinvestigated.Saliencewasexpectedto increaseastimulus’prominenceandthereforepromotelearning.Atraining-testparadigm was used on native German participants utilizing an artificial German accent. Salience was elicited by two different criteria: production and listening training as a subjective criterion and accented (Experiment 1) and canonical test words (Experiment 2) as an objective criterion. During training in Experiment 1, participants either read single German words out loud and deliberately devoiced initial voiced stop consonants (e.g., Balken—“beam” pronounced as ∗Palken), or they listened to pre-recorded words with Editedby: thesameaccent.Inasubsequenteye-trackingexperiment,lookstoauditorilypresented BerndKortmann, target words with the accent were analyzed. Participants from both training conditions UniversityofFreiburg,Germany fixatedaccentedtargetwordsmoreoftenthanacontrolgroupwithouttraining.Training Reviewedby: MelissaMichaudBaese-Berk, wasidenticalinExperiment2,butduringtest,canonicalGermanwordsthatoverlapped UniversityofOregon,USA in onset with the accented words from training were presented as target words (e.g., HelmutSpiekermann, WestfälischeWilhelms-Universität Palme—“palm tree” overlapped in onset with the training word ∗Palken) rather than Münster,Germany accented words. This time, no training effect was observed; recognition of canonical *Correspondence: word forms was not affected by having learned the accent. Therefore, accent learning Ann-KathrinGrohe wasonlyvisiblewhentheaccentedtesttokensinExperiment1,whichwerenotincluded [email protected] inthetestofExperiment2,possessedsufficientsaliencebasedontheobjectivecriterion Specialtysection: “accent.”Theseeffectswerenotmodifiedbythesubjectivecriterionofsaliencefromthe Thisarticlewassubmittedto trainingmodality. LanguageSciences, asectionofthejournal Keywords:nativeaccents,adaptation,eye-tracking,salience FrontiersinPsychology Received:11February2016 Accepted:24May2016 INTRODUCTION Published:07June2016 Citation: Languages typically consist of a number of regional dialects—that is, native accents. In the GroheA-KandWeberA(2016)The southwesternGermanstateofBaden-Württemberg,forexample,onedoesnothavetotravelvery PenefitofSalience:SalientAccented, fartoencountervariousnativeaccents,asSpiekermanndocumentedin2008(Spiekermann,2008). butNotUnaccentedWordsReveal Thisvariationcanposeaproblemfornon-locals.Whennon-localshearanativeaccentforthefirst AccentAdaptationEffects. Front.Psychol.7:864. time,theyoftendonotunderstandwhatisbeingsaidaseasilyasdolocals,whoareexperiencedwith doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00864 theregionalvarieties.Recentstudieshaveindeedshownthatlistenersprocessaccentsintheirnative FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 1 June2016|Volume7|Article864 GroheandWeber SalienceinAccentAdaptation language more easily when they are familiar with the accents animacydecisiontask.Decisiontimesinthesecondanimacytask than when they are unfamiliar with them (e.g., Adank et al., werenotfasterthanintheoriginaltask. 2009). Adaptation by non-locals to native accents is, however, Short-term adaptation was, however, found by Trude and possible. Adaptation has been found for longer periods of Brown-Schmidt(2012).Participantswerefirsttrainedonanative exposure to a novel accent (Evans and Iverson, 2007), but it English accent and then tested in an eye-tracking paradigm. can even be observed after just 4 min of listening to a new During training, participants listened to scripted dialogs with accent (Trude and Brown-Schmidt, 2012). This is also true an accented male speaker and an unaccented female speaker. for second language (L2) learners. Producing a new accent for The male speaker raised the /æ/ before /g/ to [eI], i.e., bag was only 7 min can facilitate subsequent accent understanding for pronounced/beIg/.Duringtest,targetwordswereeitherspoken L2 learners, even more so than listening to the accent does bythemaleorthefemalespeaker.Whenback,awordunaffected (Grohe and Weber, in press). The act of speech production by the accent, was the target and bag the competitor, bag was arguably makes an accent more salient than listening to that ruled out more quickly as a candidate word for trials with the accent does. Can the advantage of production experience also malespeakerthanitwaswiththefemalespeaker.Whenbake,a be observed in a listener’s native language (L1)? Next to signal wordwith/eI/initscanonicalform,wasthetargetandbagacted modality (production and listening), salience can also emerge as competitor, bake was fixated less often when it was spoken from concrete properties of the speech signal itself. Acoustic bythemale.Thiseffect,however,waslessstronglypronounced, distinctiveness of a speech signal can enhance its salience (e.g., i.e., competitor inclusion was more difficult than competitor Cho and Feldman, 2013). The present study used a training- exclusion. test paradigm in German in which salience was induced by Specific properties of the tested accents could account for either a subjective or an objective criterion and looked at the themissingeffectsofadaptationinthestudiesdiscussedabove, role of salience in native accent adaptation. The subjective but,moreimportantly,speaker-specificitycanexplainittoo.In criterionwasimplementedthroughtwodifferentaccenttrainings contrasttoAdankandMcQueen,whohaddifferentspeakerswith (production and listening) and the objective criterion through thesameaccentduringexposureandtestanddidnotfindaccent the featuring of either accented (Experiment 1) or canonical adaptation, Trude and Brown-Schmidt used the same accented (Experiment 2) target words during test in an eye-tracking speaker in both of their two experimental phases. Short-term study. adaptation to native-accented speech may therefore be rather speaker-specific.Thisproblemhasalsobeenaddressedinstudies Adaptation to Native Accents onforeignaccentadaptation,withmixedresults.Usingatraining Familiaritywithanativeaccentfacilitatesaccentprocessing.For testparadigm,BradlowandBent(2008)foundthatgeneralization example,listenerswithextensiveexperiencewiththeNewYork of accent learning (Chinese-accented English) to new voices is City(NYC)Englishaccentshowgreaterprimingeffectsforwords onlypossibleifthelistenerisexposedtomultiplespeakersduring withtheNYC-English-typicalfinalr-droppingthanlistenerswith training(forsimilarfindingsseealsoSidarasetal.,2009).Kraljic limitedexperience(SumnerandSamuel,2009).Similarly,Adank and Samuel (2007), on the other hand, found with L1 listeners et al. (2009) found that only listeners who were familiar with that the nature of the tested material has an effect on whether bothSouthernStandardBritishEnglish(SSBE)andGlaswegian perceptuallearningcangeneralizetonewspeakers.Theyfound English(GE)showedequalperformanceonbothaccenttypesin generalizationeffectsforplosivesbutnotforfricatives. a sentence verification task. The familiarity advantage probably results from adaptation processes, as demonstrated by Evans Adaptation with Production andIverson(2007).Intheirstudy,studentswhowereoriginally Speakerspecificityraisesthequestionofwhetherthereisaway from Northern England adapted to SSBE over the course of tomaketrainingmoreeffective,i.e.,allowingforgeneralization theiruniversitystudiesinSouthernEngland,asshownthrough acrossspeakers,andpotentiallyrenderingcompetitorinclusion productionandcomprehensiontasks.Processingadvantagesfor morerobust.Thismightbepossiblethroughproductiontraining. participants’ own accents over an unfamiliar accent were also InarecentstudybyGroheandWeber(inpress),theproduction found for French listeners (Floccia et al., 2006). In a lexical of a foreign accent in participants’ L2 promoted adaptation to decisiontask,reactiontimestoitemsinlongsentenceswerefaster that accent in a subsequent lexical decision task. Participants when sentences were presented in the participants’ own accent first either listened to an English short story that featured the (Northeastern French) than when they were presented in the replacement of all dental fricatives (“th”) with /t/ (e.g., theft unfamiliar Southern French accent. Furthermore, participants pronounced as ∗ teft), or they read the same story aloud with did not adapt to the unfamiliar accent during the course of thesamesubstitutions.Acontrolgrouphadnoaccenttraining. the experiment (see also Floccia et al., 2009). Additionally, Afterwards,allparticipantscompletedalexicaldecisiontaskon Adank and McQueen (2007) found no short-term adaption in wordswiththeth-substitutions.Theproductiongroupaccepted a study with regionally-accented Dutch. In their study, Dutch the accented words significantly more quickly than the control participantswhowerenotfamiliarwiththeFlemishaccenthadto group did. The listening group produced only a weak training makeanimacydecisionsonsinglewordsspokenbytwodifferent effect.WhenthesameexperimentwasrunwithL1participants, speakers,onewithaFlemishaccentandonewiththesameaccent notrainingeffectwasobserved.Referringtospeakereffects,L1 as the participants. Then, participants were exposed to another participantsintheproductiongroupproducedthecriticalaccent speaker with the Flemish accent before having to repeat the marker,buttheywerelisteningtoanL2speakerinthetestphase. FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 2 June2016|Volume7|Article864 GroheandWeber SalienceinAccentAdaptation According to Pickering and Garrod (2013), listeners are more phase. In a subsequent word recognition task, there was an likely to refer to their own previous production experience if it advantageforDutch-accentedwords. ishighlysimilartothespeakertheyarelisteningto(e.g.,interms Second,factorsbeyondlinguisticorstructuralpropertiesmay ofsex,L1background,dialect).Lesssimilarityleadslistenersto also affect salience. For the case of dialect accommodation, drawontheirexperiencewithothers’speech.SinceonlytheL2 Kerswill and Williams (2002) suggest intensity of dialect participantsinGroheandWeberhadthesameL1backgroundas contact as one of several factors. Considering the findings on therecordedtestspeakers,speaker-listenersimilaritywassmaller production effects on accent adaptation, we can extend the forL1participantsthanforL2participants. list of extra-linguistic factors toward cognitive mechanisms by Facilitatory effects of producing an accent were also found introducing accent learning modality (production vs. listening) inanaccentimitationstudywithL1speakersofDutch(Adank asanadditionalfactor.Severalstudieshavefoundanadvantage et al., 2010). Baese-Berk and Samuel (2016), however, found of production over listening for dialect accommodation; this that imitating a newly learned L2 sound can even inhibit has been named the production effect. It predicts that overt learning.Intheirstudy,participantshadtoimitatesoundsfrom production facilitates word recollection when compared with a sound continuum ranging from /s„a/–/ra/, which is arguably studying a word silently (MacLeod et al., 2010) and also when difficult for speakers to imitate correctly. A potential acoustic compared with listening to others producing a word aloud discrepancy between the sound prompt that was presented (MacLeod, 2011). It has been suggested that produced words and the participants’ productions may therefore have inhibited are more easily recalled because they are more distinctive and learning effects. A recent discrimination study with Danish therefore more salient. Distinctiveness results from listeners vowels (Kartushina et al., 2015) supports this interpretation. focusingmoreontheirownthanonothers’productions,which In that study, production accuracy was increased by concrete are,inthesenseoftheembodimenthypothesis(foranoverview: feedbackonproductions,whichinturnresultedinbettersound Glenberg,2010),moreembodiedthanothers’productions. discriminationperformanceafterproductionthanafterlistening Salience, as described above, has been further specified in training. sociolinguistic research. Referring to Schirmunski (1928) and Trudgill (1986), Auer et al. (1998) differentiate objective and subjectivecriteriaofsalience.Forexample,articulatorydistance Salience in Adaptation is described as an objective criterion and perceptual distance as We now turn to the concept of salience, which can potentially its subjective counterpart. The two relate in that articulatory explainboththeresultsofaccentadaptationandtheadvantage distance describes the magnitude which a linguistic token of produced compared to listened-to tokens. Salience has been deviates acoustically from the canonical realization, whereas generally defined as “the property of a linguistic item or perceptual distance describes which way a listener perceives feature that makes it in some way perceptually and cognitively this distance. Based on this information, we can conclude that prominent”(KerswillandWilliams,2002,p.81).Animportant subjectivecriteriaincreasethesalienceofastimulus,forexample, question,however,iswhatexactlymakesalinguisticitemsalient. duetoregularpractice,andtheresultingcognitivepre-activation. First, sociolinguistic research on salience suggests that an Objective criteria refer to properties of a stimulus that itself accent can increase a word’s salience. As suggested by Trudgill attractsattentionbecauseofitsdistinct,physicalcharacteristics. (1986), the phonetic difference between two variant forms Underthisview,theproductioneffectreliesonthepresenceof affects their salience; the greater the difference, the more a an objective criterion. A self-produced word can be physically dialect speaker is aware of it. Phonetic distance can also more distinct compared to a word read silently or a word that be considered within the framework of distinctiveness which is produced by others because these words were only tested in assumes that isolated, i.e., distinct, words are more salient mixedlists,i.e.,oneparticipanthadtolistento/silentlyreadand than others during encoding—provoking additional processing producewordsinthesamesession. and, therefore, better memory (McDaniel and Geraci, 2006, Insummary,priorresearchhasshownthatnativeaccentsare for a review). Geraci and Manzano (2010), for example, had moreeasilyprocessediftheyarefamiliartoalistenerthanifthey participants study a list of semantically related words that also arenew.Short-termadaptationtonativeaccentsispossible,and included a few semantically unrelated, i.e., distinct, words. In productionalonecanpositivelyaffectforeignaccentlearning,at ensuing tests,more unrelated thanrelatedwords were recalled. leastinL2learners.Bothrobustaccentadaptationandtherole Accordingly,Siegel(2010)claimsthatsaliencerequiresalistener of production in accent adaptation may be related to salience. to notice a contrast between two linguistic tokens. In terms Theroleandconcretenatureofsalienceinlearningaccentedvs. of phonetic variability, words that carry an accent are distinct canonicalwordsthroughdifferentformsoftraining,however,is from their unaccented counterparts and bear greater salience. notyetclear. Therefore, they can be learned more easily than unaccented words1. This was tested in a different memory study (Cho and Present Study Feldman,2013).L1EnglishparticipantslistenedtoeitherDutch- Thepresentstudytakesacloserlookatthisissuebyinvestigating accented or native-accented English words during a training subjective and objective criteria of salience separately, using modality and accent as criteria. In an exposure-test paradigm, 1Forexample,intheiraccountonsocialweighinginspeechperception,Sumner German participants first underwent native accent training etal.(2013),predictbettermemoryforaccentedwords—butonlyiftheaccentis sociallyprestigious. before adaptation was tested by a printed word eye-tracking FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 3 June2016|Volume7|Article864 GroheandWeber SalienceinAccentAdaptation task. A subjective criterion was established by having two is applied by default as soon as the auditory input potentially different types of training (production and listening), while the matches the accent, i.e., from initial /p/ presentation onward. objective criterion featured accented vs. canonical test words. When, in an eye-tracking experiment, the display includes Accentedtestwordshadtheirinitialvoicedbilabialorvelarstop PALME and BALKEN and ∗Palken is the auditory target, both devoiced.InExperiment1,accentedwords(∗PalkenforBalken— PALME and BALKEN should be fixated from word onset until “beam”)werepresentedduringtrainingandtest.InExperiment disambiguation (including /pal/). Only after disambiguation 2, the same accented words were presented during training should BALKEN be fixated more often than PALME. If the (∗PalkenforBalken),buttargetitemsduringtestwerecanonical accent rule is not learned robustly enough, the candidates that words that overlapped in onset with the trained accented word requiretherule(BALKEN)haveaweakeractivationthanthose (Palme—“palm tree”). Old word pairs from the training phase thatdonotrequiretherule(PALME).Consequently,duringthe as well as new word pairs that had not been included in the overlapping word portion, PALME will still be more strongly traininglistweretested.Thismanipulationwasincludedtotest activatedthanBALKEN,andBALKENwillonlybepreferredafter generalizationoflearning,i.e.,whethertheaccentisonlylearned disambiguation. fortrainedwordsoralsofornewaccentedwords. Successful recognition of a canonical word (Palme), as in A subjective criterion of salience was tested by comparing Experiment2,doesnotrequireaccentruleapplication.However, effects of individuals’ accent productions with that of listening successfulaccentlearningshouldresultinincreasedcompetitor during training. In contrast to MacLeod (2011), the current activation of words with a /b/ in initial position. This increase studydidnotmanipulatetrainingmodalityinmixedlistswithin in competitor activation might adversely affect canonical target participants but rather between participants. This permits the activation. The rule should be applied by default as soon as comparison of the magnitude of salience based on a subjective the auditory input potentially matches the accent, i.e., also criterion of the production modality with that of the listening when Palme is presented. Having PALME and BALKEN on modality. Individual participants are exclusively trained with the visual display, both words should be equally fixated during one modality. If producing an item in fact constitutes a /pal/. Only after disambiguation should PALME be preferred. subjective criterion for salience compared to listening to an The same predictions as above emerge if the accent rule is item,productiontrainingwiththatitemwouldresultingreater not learned strongly enough—the candidates that require the saliencethanlisteningtraining. rule(BALKEN)areactivatedlessstronglythancanonicalwords An objective criterion of salience, on the other hand, was (PALME).Consequently,PALMEwillbemorestronglyactivated manipulated by the presence of both accented and canonical thanBALKENevenfromthebeginningofwordprocessing. testtokens.InExperiment1,thepresentationofaccentedwords The accent in the present study is an artificial accent that assigned salience to the test tokens due to their great degree of centersononespecificphonologicalaccentmarkerandtherefore inherentdistinctiveness.Effectsofaccentasanobjectivecriterion must be differentiated from a dialect. Participants and pre- have been previously shown (Cho and Feldman, 2013), but recorded speakers are not L2 speakers, and all used standard withamemoryexperimentinwhichgeneralizationeffectswere Germanpronunciationintheexperimentalcontext.“Standard” not examined. In the learning phase, the accented words were here means that the pre-recorded speakers did not have a embeddedinalistoffillertargetwordsthatfeaturednoparticular noticeabledialectthatcouldallocatethemtoaspecificregionin accentmarker.Thismadetheaccentedwordsdistinctfromthe Germany, and the participants’ speech did not include specific fillers. Contrarily, in the present Experiment 2, the canonical local (e.g., Swabian) accent properties during the experiment. testwordswereexpectedtobelesssalient.Experiment2tested The tested accent affected German stop consonants and has, whether the salience inherent in the learned accent can modify to our knowledge, not been documented as an existing native the processing of words that include the accent target sound accentofGerman.Itreferstothelenis/fortis-contrastinGerman in their canonical form. A difference in learning effects can be bilabialandvelarplosives(/b,p/and/g,k/).InStandardGerman, reflected in the activation differences of canonical target words fortisplosivesarealwaysaspiratedinwordinitialposition,while starting with the manipulated accent’s target sound. Learning lenis plosives are never aspirated (Jessen, 1998; Kleiner and thatBalkenispronouncedas∗Palkenpotentiallyincreaseslexical Knöbl, 2015). Our accent neutralized this contrast, i.e., lenis competitionforthecanonicalPalme,which,inturn,slowsdown velar and bilabial stops were aspirated (/g/ pronounced [kh] recognitionofPalme.ThisisbasedonTrudeandBrown-Schmidt and/b/pronounced[ph]:Gitterpronounced∗Kitter,andBalken (2012),whofoundthataccentlearningcanimplytheinclusionof pronounced∗Palken).Theaccentedsoundwasalwaysaspirated, newcompetitors.Inthepresentstudy,Balkencouldbeincluded similartothecanonicallyfortisstops.Forsimplification,werefer asanewcompetitorforPalmeaftertraining,resultinginfewer toaspirated,fortisplosives(Palme)as“voiceless”andtothelenis targetlookstoPalme. plosiveswiththeadditionalaspirationintheaccentedversionas The pattern of target and competitor activation is especially “devoiced”(∗Palken). importantduringthesegmentaloverlapoftargetandcompetitor We opted for an accent with a target sound that is words.Referringtotheprinciplesofanabstractmentallexicon, included in the German sound inventory (Kohler, 1999). This we assume that accent learning is based on learning pre-lexical makes it easy to produce for native German participants and rules. When hearing ∗Palken in Experiment 1, successful word promises relatively stable acoustic properties of the target recognitionrequirestheapplicationofaspecificaccentrule(/b/ sounds across participants. The accent under investigation →/p/).Iftheaccentruleislearnedrobustlyduringtraining,it has to be differentiated from middle-Bavarian dialects where FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 4 June2016|Volume7|Article864 GroheandWeber SalienceinAccentAdaptation bilabial, alveolar, or velar plosives are not realized with not suffer from any hearing disorders and had normal or an aspiration contrast before /r, l, n, m/; they are always corrected-to-normalvision.Twoparticipantswereexcludeddue voiceless and unaspirated and therefore neutralize with their to unsuccessful calibration, resulting in the collection of data leniscounterpart,e.g.,Preiselbeeren—“cranberries”pronounced from 72 participants (26 production group, 22 listening group, ∗Breiselbeeren (Moosmüller and Ringen, 2004). Likewise, in and24controlgroup). AustrianGerman,thefortisplosives/p/and/t/arenotaspirated (Siebsetal.,1969),e.g.,Pinsel—“brush”pronounced∗Binsel.In Materials contrast,theaccentpresentedinthisstudyneutralizedallbilabial WordsduringtheTestPhase plosives to [ph] and all velar plosives to [kh]. Since the accent Wepresented92wordquadrupletsduringtest,eachcontaining tested in our study describes a voicing shift in the opposing four German nouns. Twenty-eight quadruplets were based on directionofexistingnativeGermanaccents,wecanassumethat critical word pairs; 64 quadruplets were based on filler word none of our participants had had experience with the accent. pairs.The28criticalwordpairswereeachcomposedofatarget Thisensuredtheobservationofonlylaboratory-specifictraining wordwithaninitialvoicedstopandacompetitorwordstarting effects. with the corresponding voiceless stop. Only target words were Wepredictthataccenttrainingwillresultinaccentlearning presented auditorily during the experiment. Fourteen had a effects. The training modality can determine the amount of bilabialonset(e.g.,targetBALKEN“beam”—competitorPALME saliencebasedononesubjectivecriterion.Thiswouldbeinline “palm tree”), and 14 had a velar onset (e.g., target GITTER with prior findings where producing rather than listening to a “grid”—competitor KITTEL “tunic”). We opted for plosives, wordresultedinbettermemory(MacLeod,2011).Saliencethat becauseithasbeenshownthatperceptuallearningofplosivescan relies on an objective criterion of the target token is expected generalize across speakers (Kraljic and Samuel, 2007), arguably to affect looking patterns such that the learned accent affects because they contain hardly no talker-specific information in processingofhighlysalient,accenteddevoicedtokensmorethan comparison to fricatives, for example. This was important thatofcanonicalvoicelesstokens. because participants in the training groups were trained with a differentvoicethanwasheardduringtest.Voicedstopsoccurred EXPERIMENT 1 only in the initial position of target words. The initial stop consonantwasalwaysfollowedbyavowel3.Apartfromtheinitial Experiment 1 tested if salience can result from training as consonant, target and competitor overlapped in at least two subjective criterion. Critical test words had a native accent and segments.Whenthetargetwordswerepresentedauditorily,the wereassumedtobehighlysalientbasedontheobjectivecriterion initialvoicelessplosivesweredevoiced(Balkenwaspronounced “accent.”Duringtraining,nativeGermanparticipantseitherread as ∗Palken), resulting in overlapping word onsets of target and aloudorlistenedtosingleGermanwordsthathadtheirinitial/b, competitorforatleastthreesegments.Auditorywordswiththe g/devoicedto/p,k/,e.g.,Balkenpronouncedas∗Palken,while nativeaccent(∗Palken)wereneverexistingwordsofGerman(see thecontrolgrouphadnotraining. TableA1fortarget-competitorpairs). Inthetestphaseoftheexperiment,participantsaccomplished Mean log-frequencies of target words were 0.61 per million theprintedwordvariant(McQueenandViebahn,2007;Weber for velar stop words, 0.85 for bilabial stop words, and of et al., 2007) of the eye-tracking task (Allopenna et al., 1998). competitors0.67forvelarstopwords,and0.88forbilabialstop Participants saw four printed words in their canonical spelling words according to the CELEX word form dictionary (Baayen (including a target, a competitor, and two distractors) on a et al., 1995). In order to form quadruplets, each of the 28 computer screen and were auditorily instructed to click on a criticaltarget-competitorpairswaspairedwithtwosemantically target word while their eye movements were recorded. They unrelated distractor words that matched in frequency with the listened to devoiced words (∗Palken) and had to click on a target-competitor pair. Distractor words never had a stop in visual display that included the target word (BALKEN) and a initialpositionbutcouldcontainstopconsonantsinotherword competitor (PALME). The competitor allows the investigation positions. of whether activation of the devoiced token can be as strong The 64 filler word pairs also had a target and a competitor. as activation of voiceless word forms without an accent. The There were 8 targets with initial /k/, 8 with initial /p/, 16 with proportion of target fixations was measured and compared initial /t/, and 32 targets with no initial stop in onset position betweentheproduction,listening,andcontrolgroups. (the “no-stop targets”). For the total of 32 targets with initial /k/, /p/, and /t/, half of the competitor word onsets overlapped Participants with the target word onset by at least three segments, and Seventy-four native German speaking female students (19– half were phonologically unrelated. Two phonologically and 30 years, mean age = 23.8, SD = 2.7; 5 left-handed) from SouthernGermandialect,thevariable“SouthernDialect”wastestedininitialdata the University of Tübingen participated for a small monetary analyses,resultinginnosignificanteffect.Participantswerenotselectedbasedon remuneration.Onlywomenweretestedinordertoaccountfor dialectcompetence,anditwasnotcounter-balancedacrossconditions;therefore the fact that the recordings were exclusively made by female thisfactorwasnotincludedinthemethodssection. speakers. German was their only mother tongue2, they did 3InsomevarietiesofGerman,speakerstendtodevoiceinitialvoicelessstopswhen theyarefollowedbyaliquid(e.g.,grillencanbepronouncedaskrillen).Byalways 2Fiftyparticipantsindicateddialectfamiliarity(42specificallywithaSouthern havingavowelfollowingtheinitialconsonant,potentialpreviousexperiencewith German dialect, mostly Swabian). As most dialect speakers had exposure to a theaccentwasavoided. FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 5 June2016|Volume7|Article864 GroheandWeber SalienceinAccentAdaptation semantically unrelated distractors were added to each target- effects, as observed by Trude and Brown-Schmidt (2012), can competitor pair. The 32 no-stop targets were paired with generalizetonewspeakersofthesamesex(bothfemale). competitors that also did not have stops in initial position. Recordings were carried out in a sound proof cabin with However, half of them overlapped in onset with the target an Olympus LS-11 sound recorder (44.1 kHz; 16-bit). Every for at least two segments (e.g., target Seife “soap”—competitor targetword wasrecorded inthecontextofthecarriersentence Seite “side/page”). There were four types of distractors for KlickenSiejetztauf —“Nowclickon.”Thedevoicedtargetwords the 32 no-stop target-competitor pairs, each containing eight (∗Palken) and the voiceless words (Palme) were all recorded distractor pairs. The bilabial (b/p), velar (g/k), and alveolar naturally, that is, the speakers were explicitly instructed to (d/t) distractor pairs followed the same prerequisites as the pronounce the /b/ in Balken the way they would pronounce corresponding critical target-competitor pairs. As they were the/p/inPalme.Thebestexemplarofthecarriersentencewas not presented auditorily, stop+consonant onsets (e.g., Brosche chosen for each voice, and the duration of the carrier sentence “brooch”—Prospekt“brochure/leaflet”)wereallowed.Thefourth wasmatchedbetweenbothvoices.Then,thecarriersentencewas grouphadtwosemanticallyandphonologicallyunrelatedinitial addedtoeachtargetwordrecording. soundsthatwereneverstops. Altogether, the test included 92 critical trials and four Procedure practicetrials.Halfofthecriticaltargetsandtheircorresponding AnSR-ResearchEyelink1000set-upwasusedfordatacollection competitorshadbeenincludedintheprecedingtrainingphase, with a sampling rate of 1 kHz, and the experiment was andhalfwerenewtoparticipants.Likewise,halfofthetargetsnot programmed with Experiment Builder (SR Research Ltd., startingwithastop(other-group)werenewtoparticipantsand Canada). Before the experiment started, the dominant eye of half were familiar from the training. Every participant had her eachparticipantwasdetermined.Then,participantswereseated ownexperimentallist,eachstartingwiththesamefourpractice in front of a computer screen and placed their chin on a chin trials.Fillerandcriticaltrialswereequallydistributedacrossthe rest. They were brought to a position in which they could stay lists,andacriticaltrialwasalwaysfollowedbyatleastonefiller comfortablyforthedurationoftheexperiment(∼30min).The trial. There were never more than two old and not more than eye-trackerwascalibrated,thenwritteninstructionswereshown fivenewtrialsinarow.Thevariousfillerconditionswereequally onthescreen.Participantshadasmuchtimeastheyneededto distributedacrossthelists. readthemandinitiatedtheexperimentwithamouseclick. Training WordsduringtheTrainingPhase The same training list was presented to each participant from Seventy-two single words from the above described target- the two training groups, while the control group received no competitor pairs were used for training. They included half of training. The training tokens were presented either visually the devoiced targets(7 targets with bilabialonset, e.g., ∗Palken, and auditorily (listening group) or visually only (production and 7 targets with velar onset) and their respective competitor group). The listening group first saw a fixation cross for 1000 (Palmefortarget∗Palken).Thedevoicedandvoicelessitemswere ms, then the orthographic transcript of the training word included twice in the training list, resulting in 28 devoiced and (black Arial font, size 24) appeared in the center of the 28 voiceless trials. Additionally, 16 filler targets from the no- screen. It corresponded to German spelling rules (BALKEN). stoptargetswereincluded,resultingin72trainingtrialsintotal. The initial letter that corresponded to the devoiced sound Training trials with the same initial sound did not occur more was colored red. Five hundred milliseconds later, the training thantwiceinarow,andeachdevoiceditemwasfollowedbyat word was played (∗Palken). Participants listened to the single leastonecanonicalitem. words (devoiced, voiceless, and fillers) through noise-canceling headphones(SennheiserHD215II)andatthesametimefixated RecordingsofTestandTrainingTokens the transcript on the screen. There was a 2000 ms inter-trial All tokens used for training and test were recorded by two interval. Participants in the listening group were explicitly told femalenativespeakersofStandardGermanwithoutanoticeable tolistenattentivelytothewordsandtobeawareofthespeaker’s regional accent (speaker A: 23 years; speaker B: 28 years). The accent while fixating the orthographic version of the words. speakersdidnotdiffersignificantlyinF0-rangeorspeakingrate, Witteman et al. (2013) have shown that a single word context and the authors judged their pronunciation to be comparable. issufficientforlistenerstolearnanaccent.Intheircross-modal Two different speakers were recorded to have different voices primingtaskparticipantswithoutpreviousaccentexposurehad for both the training and test phases in the listening group. increasedprimingeffectsinthesecondhalfoftheexperimental This permitted constant conditions across the training groups listcomparedtothefirsthalf. because the production condition always involved a different The production group did not wear headphones during the speakerduringthetraining(theparticipant)andthetest(thepre- training.Theysawthesameorthographictranscriptofthewords recorded talker). Nevertheless, speaker-listener similarity was on the screen and had to read every single word out loud granted by having participants and pre-recorded speakers with while their productions were recorded. Participants were asked the same sex and L1 background in the test phase. Acoustic to pronounce the initial red letter “B” as /p/ and the initial red differencesbetweenthetrainingandtesttokensareheldassmall letter “G” as /k/. Before every single trial, there was a fixation as possible. Moreover, it can be tested whether speaker specific cross for 1000 ms, and then the word was shown for 3500 ms FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 6 June2016|Volume7|Article864 GroheandWeber SalienceinAccentAdaptation (accounting for the timing in the listening condition: 500 ms recordings.Onaverage,only0.7outof28criticaltrialswerenot before the sound + 1000 ms mean word duration + 2000 ms devoiced as instructed. The proportion of correct clicks on the pause). The next trial would then start. Between training and targetduringthetestphasewas94.3%(equallydistributedacross test,thewritteninstructionsfortheeye-trackingtaskwereshown the training groups). However, five participants did not see the on the screen. The production group had about 5s to put on mouse cursor due to technical problems. We extracted fixation theirheadphones,andthelisteninggroupwaitedfor5suntilthe reports with the software Data Viewer (SR Research) and then initiationofthetestphase.Overall,thetrainingtookabout7min further processed the data with the software R (R development foreachparticipant. coreteam,2015).Thedatafromeachparticipant’sdominanteye was used to determine the coordinates and timing of fixations. TestPhase Only fixations that fell within a cell of one of the four interest The test phase started with four practice trials. A fixation cross areas—target, competitor, and two distractors—were analyzed preceded each trial for 1000 ms, then four printed words from (exclusion of 3.4% of the data). The interest areas each had a a word quadruplet were shown on the screen for 500 ms. The cellsizeof472×344pixelswithadistanceof40pixelsbetween words were printed in black Times New Roman font, size 34 vertical cells and 60 pixels between horizontal cells. Saccades on a screen with a white background. Screen resolution was (20.8% of the data) were not added to fixation times. We then 1024 × 768 pixels, and the words were distributed across four analyzed the fixations for the four interest areas in 20-ms steps different positions (256 × 576, 768 × 576, 256 × 192, and inatimewindowfrom0to1000msaftertargetwordonset.The 768 × 192 pixels), see Figure1. Display positions of target dependent variable “target” indicated whether in the respective and competitor were pseudo-randomized, and the target never 20-msstepaparticipantfixatedthetarget;“competitor”indicated appeared in the same display position more than three times a competitor fixation, and “distractor” a distractor fixation. in a row. The mouse cursor (represented by a small circle) was This resulted in three variables with binary values. Target locatedinthecenterofthescreenatthebeginningofeachtrial. and competitor fixation proportions were calculated with the Thenthecarriersentence(about1300ms)followedbythetarget empirical logit function. The plotted fixation proportions were word was played auditorily. Participants clicked on the target inspectedvisuallytodeterminethecriticaltimewindowtowhich wordwiththemouse.Visually,participantssawthetargetword linearmixedeffectsregressionmodels(Baayenetal.,2008;Bates in its correct spelling (BALKEN); auditorily, it had the same et al., 2015) were then applied. For each analysis we built an accentaspresentedduringthetrainingphase(∗Palken).Asmall individual, best fitting model that included a particular choice fixation circle appeared on the screen after every six trials to of fixed and random factors. Random effect structure included initiateanautomaticdriftcorrectioninthecalibrationoftheeye- random intercepts for participants and items as well as those tracker.Theexperimentconcludedwithalanguagebackground randomslopesthatsignificantlyimprovedthemodelfitastested questionnairebasedontheLEAP-Q(Marianetal.,2007). bylikelihoodratiotests.Significanceoffactorswasindicatedby t > |2|. Corresponding p-values, as reported in the text below, Analysis and Results weredeterminedwithlikelihoodratiotests.Asfixedeffects,we Duringtraining,theproductiongroupperformedtheinstructed considered training (production vs. listening vs. no training), accentquitewell.Theexperimenterdecidedbasedonperceptual familiarity (old, i.e., included in the training, vs. new), sound judgments whether the critical training tokens were devoiced condition (bilabial vs. velar word initial sound), and speaker as communicated in the instructions. Every instance where (speakerAvs.speakerB).Proportionoftargetfixationswasthe the devoicing was not clearly perceivable was documented and dependentvariable. subsequentlyvalidatedbymeansofacousticmeasurementsofthe DescriptiveAnalysis Notsurprisingly,thedistractorswereruledoutaspotentialtarget words very early by the participants (from about 200 ms, see Figure2), i.e., the fixation proportion of distractors decreased veryquickly.Aslaunchingaprogrammedeyemovementusually takes about 200 ms (e.g., Altmann and Kamide, 2004), word processing is reflected in fixation proportions from this point intimeon.Competitorswerepreferredovertargetsbyallthree groupsfromabout280msonuntilabout700ms.Targetfixations show that the two training groups started to fixate the target more often than the control group from about 250 ms on. The advantage of both training groups became more pronounced andstartedbeingrobustfromabout350mson.Visually,there wasnodifferencebetweentheproductionandlisteninggroups. FIGURE1|ExampledisplayofatesttrialinExperiments1and2.In Statistical analyses were run for the time window 250–750 Experiment1,BALKENwasthetargetandPALMEthecompetitor.In ms because it included the whole process of target-competitor Experiment2,PALMEwasthetarget,andBALKENthecompetitor.RETTER disambiguation,andhereitbecameevidentthatthetwotraining andVENTILweredistractorsinbothexperiments. groupshadastableadvantageoftargetfixationscomparedtothe FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 7 June2016|Volume7|Article864 GroheandWeber SalienceinAccentAdaptation FIGURE2|Proportionsoftarget(BALKEN)andcompetitor(PALME)fixationsoftheproduction,thelistening,andthecontrolgroupinExperiment1. Thebottomlinedescribesthemeannumberofdistractorfixationsofallthreegroups. controlgroup.AscanbeseeninFigure2,theactualadvantage 8% from listening group data between 250–750 ms and re-ran lastedmuchlonger—atleastuntil1000ms. the same model with the modified data. Despite the reduction of the listening group’s target fixation data, training was still StatisticalAnalysis significant(χ2 =6.2,p<0.05):thelisteninggroupstillfixated First,amodelwithdatainthetimewindow0–200mswasrun. the target more often than the control group (ßtraining= 0.30, Thistestedlookingbiasesbeforeprocessingoftheactualtarget SE = 0.15, t = 2.0), and there was no difference between the wordbegan.Traininggroupwasthefixedeffect,andparticipant productionandlisteninggroups(t =0.2).Thissuggestsrobust anditemwererandomintercepts.Therewasasignificanteffect differences between the control group and the two training bytraining(χ2 =7.2,p<0.03);theresultsofthemixedmodel groups. showthatthelisteninggrouphadmoretargetfixationsthanboth the control group (ß = 0.39, SE = 0.15, t = 2.6) and the Discussion training productiongroup(ß =0.31,SE=0.15,t=2.1),hintingat Wefoundthataccentadaptationwaspossibleafterbothlistening training atargetbiasforthisgroup. andproductiontraining.Theproportionoftargetlooksinboth The second model analyzed data between 250–750 ms. It training groups was higher than in the control group. The included training group and sound condition as fixed effects listening group, however, fixated targets more often than the as well as participant and item as random intercepts. Training other groups, even before actual target word processing began, wassignificant(χ2 =10.7,p<0.005);boththelisteninggroup which might be argued to have affected the listening group (ß = 0.48, SE = 0.15, t = 3.2) and the production group advantageinthesubsequentcriticaltimewindow.This,however, training (ß = 0.33, SE = 0.14, t = 2.3) fixated the target more can be excluded because the pattern of results persisted even training often than the control group. There was no difference between whenthefixationdataofthelisteninggroupinthelarger,later the two training groups (t = 1.0). Furthermore, there was a time window were penalized for its advantage in the initial, maineffectofsoundcondition(χ2 =7.5,p<0.007),resulting smaller time window. There were no effects of speaker, i.e., inmoretargetfixationsforbilabialthanvelaritems(ß = learningoccurredequallywellwithspeakerAandB.Themain condition 0.35,SE=0.13,t=2.8).Duetothebiasforthelisteninggroup effect for sound condition may be related to specific sound found from 0–200 ms, the critical time window was further properties but does not further affect the general pattern of examined.Onaverage,from0–200mstheproportionoftarget results.Moreover,thesamepatternwasobservedforoldtokens fixations was 8% higher for the listening group than for the fromthetrainingphaseandnewtokens,indicatinglearningofa control group. To account for this early bias, we subtracted rulethatgeneralizestonewwords. FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 8 June2016|Volume7|Article864 GroheandWeber SalienceinAccentAdaptation Our results suggest accent learning for the production and duetocalibrationproblems,resultingin72participants(18–31 listening groups, with no difference between the two training years,mean=23.2,SD=3.2;14left-handed)whosuccessfully groups. Thus, we found robust effects of accent training when completed the experiment. None of them suffered from any testingsingleaccentedwords,hintingatagreateffectbytarget hearingdisorders,allhadnormalorcorrected-to-normalvision, words’ accent as objective criterion of salience. Production and and German was their only mother tongue4. The participants listening training seemingly do not differ from one another for wererandomlyassignedtooneofthethreeexperimentalgroups L1intermsofsalience. (24production,24listening,and24controlgroup). Experiment 1 provides evidence for successful accent adaptationafterlisteningtoorproducinganaccent.However,the Methods and Material canonicalcompetitors(Palme)wereactivatedforaverylongtime The training list was based on that of Experiment 1. However, (untilabout700ms)beforethedevoicedtargetwordwasfixated devoiced(∗Palken)itemswerepresentedtwiceinarow(rather moreoften.Thistimewindowcoverstheentireinitialportionof than just once), resulting in 100 training trials in total (twice thewordbeforedisambiguation(averagedisambiguationpoint: the amount of training with the devoiced tokens compared 280 + 200 ms for launching the eye movement = 480 ms; to Experiment 1). Due to the greater amount of training, the earliestdisambiguationpoint:150+200ms=350ms;longest trainingphasetook1minlonger. disambiguationpoint:420+200ms=620ms)andevenlonger. During test, the same word quadruplets were presented on Thissuggeststhat,despitesuccessfulaccentadaptation,canonical thescreen—92criticaltrialsand4practicetrialswiththesame word forms still remained more easily accessible than accented propertiesasinExperiment1.However,therolesoftargetand wordforms.Therewaspotentiallynotenoughaccentexposure competitor words were switched. Targets were now voiceless fortheaccentedformstobeabletofullycompetewithcanonical tokens (Palme) in their canonical form, and competitors were wordforms.Wesuggestthatifalearnedaccentweretobeable wordsthathaveavoicedonsetintheircanonicalform(Balken). to have effects on the access of canonical, voiceless words with Auditorily,voicelesswordswerepresented(Palme)thatmatched the same onset as the accent’s target form, a greater amount of in their onset with the target word on the screen (PALME). trainingisrequired. Voiced tokens (BALKEN) that had been devoiced during the Experiment2examineswhetheraccentlearningcanbestrong training(∗Palken),werevisuallypresentedcompetitors.Alltarget enoughastoaffecttheprocessingofvoiceless,canonicalwords words had already been recorded in the recording session for with double the amount of accent training. Successful accent Experiment1bythesamefemalespeakers. learning could imply competition effects from words that were previously not included as competitors. Thus, accent training Analysis and Results haspotentiallyeffectsnotonlyforunderstandingaccentedword ThesameprocedureforanalysisasinExperiment1wasapplied. forms, but accented forms can function as competitors and During training, the production group performed quite well affect the recognition of canonical word forms. As opposed to in accomplishing the substitutions (mean: 0.8 errors out of 56 Experiment 1 where highly salient accented target words were devoicedwordtrials).Theaccuracyofclicksduringthetestphase tested, in Experiment 2, we focused on test words that are was99.8%(equallydistributedacrosstraininggroups).Saccades expected to have a much smaller degree of salience based on (17%ofthedata)andfixationsthatdidnotfallintooneofthe theobjectivecriterion“accent,”i.e.,standardGermancanonical fourinterestareas(3%)wereremovedpriortoanalysis. words.Trainingeffectsofdevoicedwords(∗Palken)weretested on words that canonically start with the accent’s target sound DescriptiveAnalysis (Palme). In order to increase the likelihood that accented Figure3 illustrates the proportions of target, competitor, and forms could influence target recognition in their function as distractor fixations of the production, listening, and control competitors, the training was doubled. If the accent is robustly groups. The distractors were ruled out from the beginning of learned, we would expect fewer target fixations by the training word processing (200 ms) on, meaning that the proportion of groupsthanwithoutaccenttraining. fixationsdecreased.Target(PALME)preferencestartedveryearly (at about 250 ms), and competitors (BALKEN) were quickly EXPERIMENT 2 ruledoutaspotentialtargetwords.Thecompetitorsstayedata relatively stable level of activation from 200–400 ms, and then Again, three participant groups were tested. The training fixationsdecreasednoticeably.Thisrepresentsapproximatelythe involved the same tokens as in Experiment 1, but the amount intervalwheretargetandcompetitorstilloverlap(meanoverlap: of training with the devoiced tokens was doubled. During test, 273 ms). Target fixations by training group did not differ from participants did not hear the devoiced words (∗Palken) this one another from the beginning until the overall mean end of time, but voiceless, canonical words (Palme), while seeing the word processing (measurements of the voiceless target words same printed words on the display as the participants from resulted in a mean word duration of 767 ms). Disambiguation Experiment1. between targets and competitor occurred relatively early, and there was no clear advantage of one of the training groups in Participants 4Fifty-threeparticipantsindicateddialectfamiliarity,50ofwhomhadexposureto Seventy-eight female students from the University of Tübingen aSouthernGermandialect(mostlySwabian).Thevariable“SouthernDialect”was participatedformonetaryreimbursement.Sixhadtobeexcluded testedininitialdataanalyses,resultinginnosignificanteffect,asinExperiment1. FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 9 June2016|Volume7|Article864 GroheandWeber SalienceinAccentAdaptation FIGURE3|Proportionsoftarget(PALME)andcompetitor(BALKEN)fixationsoftheproduction,listening,andcontrolgroupsinExperiment2.The bottomlinedescribesthemeannumberofdistractorfixationsofallthreegroups. targetfixations.Statisticalanalyseswererunforthetimewindow Acoustic Analyses from 250–750 ms, which included the entire disambiguation Pre-recorded target and training stimuli as well as the tokens process between targets and competitors and parallels analyses producedbytheproductiongroupduringtrainingwereanalyzed inExperiment1. acoustically. This tested if the difference between training and test stimuli was too great for adaptation effects to be observed. StatisticalAnalysis Particularly in the production group, the acoustic properties of The baseline model for target fixations (0–200 ms) revealed theaccentedplosiveswerelikelytovaryindividually.Thestops no significant effect by training (t < 1). Mixed effects models thatmarkthemanipulatedaccentwerefocusedonintheanalysis. revealed no significant effect of any of the considered fixed Voiceonsettime(VOT)andburstintensity(relativetototalword effects (all t < |1.3|) in the critical time window (250–750 ms). intensity)weremeasuredforeachtokenthatwaspartofanold Auditorilypresentedvoicelesswords(Palme)thatstartwiththe criticalwordpair,i.e.,wordpairsthatwereincludedinboththe same onset as the trained, devoiced words (∗Palken) triggered trainingandthetestphase.Onlyoldwordpairswereincludedin strong target activation from the beginning of word processing analyses,becausetherewasnoreferencetothetrainingphasefor on. There was no effect of learning, neither by the production newwords.Eachcriticalvoicelessword(Palme)anditsdevoiced nor the listening group. In contrast to Experiment 1, the test pairedword(∗Palken)wasconsideredforanalysis.Bothinstances words did not have the critical accent, but the voiceless paired weretakenasseparatereferencepointsinordertocalculatethe words with the same sound onset as the devoiced, accented differencesoftherespectiveacousticpropertyvaluebetweenthe words were tested. As the devoiced training words included a trainingandthetesttoken(Palme).Inthefollowing,wereferto soundsubstitution,thequestionis,especiallyfortheproduction thePalme-Palmecomparisonasthevoicelesswordpairandthe group: How much did the acoustic realizations of the devoiced ∗Palken-Palme comparison as the devoiced word pair. During tokens encountered during training differ from those of the training, one word was presented several times (devoiced: four voiceless tokens encountered during test? In other words, did times, voiceless: twice). This did not pose any problem for the the participants’ own productions of the accent differ enough listeninggroupitemsbecausethesamerecordingwaspresented fromtheproductionsofthetestspeakertopreventgeneralization severaltimes.Fortheproductiongroup,however,singletokens across speakers? The missing training effect for both groups differed individually. This issue was solved by taking average reinforces the question of effects of single tokens’ acoustic values.TwoVOT-andtwoburstintensitydifferencevalueswere properties. Therefore, acoustic properties of both the training assignedtoeachcriticalwordforeachparticipant—onewiththe materialsandthetestmaterialswereanalyzedinanextstep. valuesfromthevoicelesswordasareferencepoint(Palme)and FrontiersinPsychology|www.frontiersin.org 10 June2016|Volume7|Article864

Description:
criterion and accented (Experiment 1) and canonical test words (Experiment 2) as an objective from concrete properties of the speech signal itself. Acoustic . An important question, however, is what exactly makes a linguistic item salient. First, sociolinguistic research on salience suggests that
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.