ebook img

Running Head: EMOTIONAL MARKETPLACE The Emotional Marketplace Anny Dow Yale ... PDF

116 Pages·2016·0.61 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Running Head: EMOTIONAL MARKETPLACE The Emotional Marketplace Anny Dow Yale ...

Running Head: EMOTIONAL MARKETPLACE The Emotional Marketplace Anny Dow Yale University Advisors: Dr. Gregory Samanez-Larkin Dr. June Gruber Author Note Anny Dow, Program in Cognitive Science, Yale University. This research was supported in part by the Psi Chi Undergraduate Research Grant and the Silliman College Mellon Undergraduate Research Grant. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Anny Dow, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520. Contact: [email protected] Emotional Marketplace - 2 Abstract The current research investigates how people construe emotional experiences in economic terms through the use of modified experimental economic paradigms. We conducted three exploratory studies to examine how motivated individuals are to achieve distinct emotions, and how these motivations change with temporal delays and social contexts. In Study 1, we used a willingness to pay (WTP) paradigm asking participants to allocate a fixed “emotional budget” between specific positive and negative emotions. In Study 2, we expanded our investigation by exploring discount rates for positive and negative emotions using a modified version of a delay- discounting questionnaire. In Study 3, we used a dictator game to examine social decision- making regarding emotions. Together, these studies explore new approaches for investigating emotional preferences, demonstrating how people assign value to different emotions and how motivated they are to achieve or avoid them. Keywords: Emotion; Decision-Making; Consumerism; Well-Being Emotional Marketplace - 3 Table of Contents I. Introduction 5 II. The Economics of Emotion 6 III. Measuring Emotional States 8 IV. The Present Investigation 11 V. Study 1: Emotional “Spending” in the Present 12 Method 15 Data Collection and Aggregation 15 Participants 16 Self-Report Measures 16 Procedure 19 Results 19 Positive Emotion Condition 19 Negative Emotion Condition 20 Combined Emotion Condition 21 Baseline Correlations 22 Discussion 23 VI. Study 2: Delay of “Emotional” Gratification 28 Method 31 Participants 31 Procedure 31 Results 32 Expanded Study 2 33 Discussion 34 VII. Study 3: Emotions and Dictator Games 36 Method 40 Participants 40 Procedure 40 Results 41 Dividing Emotions with Player 2 41 Dividing Emotions with Closest Friend 42 Dividing Emotions with Tradeoffs 42 Correlations between WTP and Dictator Games 42 Discussion 44 VII. General Discussion 48 Moderating Factors of Emotional Preferences 49 IX. Limitations and Future Directions 52 X. References 58 XI. Appendix 88 Survey Questions 90 Study 1 90 Study 2 91 Study 3 102 Self-Report Questionnaires 108 Mental Health Groupings 108 Emotional Marketplace - 4 Subjective Happiness Scale (SHS) 108 Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) 109 Hypomanic Personality Scale (HPS-20) 110 Altman Self-Rating Mania Scale (ASRM) 112 Beck Depression Inventory Short Form (BDI-SF) 113 Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI) 115 Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) 116 List of Tables 1 Study 1: Descriptive Statistics for WTP to Experience Positive Emotions 72 2 Study 1: Descriptive Statistics for WTP to Avoid Negative Emotions 73 3 Study 1: Descriptive Statistics for WTP to Experience Positive and Avoid 74 Negative Emotions (Combined) 4 Study 2: Results of T-tests and Descriptive Statistics for Emotional Discount 75 Rates by Group 5 Study 2: Correlations Between Individual Difference Questionnaires and 76 Discount Rates for Non-clinical Population 6 Study 2: Correlations between Individual Difference Questionnaires and 77 Discount Rates for Clinical Population 7 Study 3: Descriptive Statistics for Difference Scores for Dividing Emotions 78 with Player 2 8 Study 3: Descriptive Statistics for Difference Scores for Dividing Emotions 79 with Closest Friend List of Figures 1 WTP Judgments and Confidence Intervals for Experiencing Positive Emotions 80 2 WTP Judgments and Confidence Intervals for Avoiding Negative Emotions 81 3 WTP Judgments and Confidence Intervals for Experiencing Positive and 82 Avoiding Negative Emotions (Combined) 4 Discount Rates for Experiencing Joy 83 5 Discount Rates for Avoiding Sadness 84 6 Difference Scores in Minutes for Dividing Emotions with Player 2 85 7 Difference Scores in Minutes for Dividing Emotions with Closest Friend 86 8 Difference Scores in Minutes for Dividing Emotions with Tradeoffs 87 Emotional Marketplace - 5 The Emotional Marketplace If money could buy happiness, how much would people pay for it? People spend thousands of dollars every year to experience certain emotions—buying tickets for roller coaster rides to feel excitement, exotic vacation getaways to feel relaxed, or for insurance to feel less anxiety. Like money, emotion is a representation of value, offers utility to the consumer, has costs and benefits, and serves as a common currency in judgments and decisions (Clore, 2005; Peters et al., 2006; Montague & Berns, 2002). While the field of behavioral economics has examined the important role of emotions in economics, little research has been done on how people construe emotions directly in financial terms. Given that people are motivated not by money itself, but by the underlying value it represents (Clore, 2005), and almost all satisfaction comes from emotional experiences (Elster, 1996), we are interested in investigating how individuals map monetary value onto the underlying value of specific positive and negative emotions. Across a series of studies, we will systematically investigate how much people value experiencing specific emotions, and how these values change across time and context. A wealth of research has demonstrated the critical role of emotions in shaping our judgments and decisions. Emotions exert causal effects on the quality of our relationships (e.g., Ekman, 2007; Graham et al., 2008; Gottman & Levenson, 1986; Lopes, Salovey, & Straus, 2003), economic choices (e.g., Lerner et al., 2003; Rick & Loewenstein, 2008), political and policy choices (e.g., Lerner, Small, & Loewenstein, 2004; Small & Lerner, 2008); creativity (e.g., Fredrickson, 1998; 2001; Isen et al., 1987), motivation (e.g., Frijda, Manstead, & Bern, 2000), mental health outcomes (e.g., Kring, 2010; Gross & Thompson, 2007; Gruber, Mauss, & Tamir, 2011), and overall well-being (e.g., Ryff & Singer, 1998). But while researchers have explored the relationship between different emotions and measures of global well-being (Kuppens, Realo, & Diener, 2008), we know little about which emotions people think will have Emotional Marketplace - 6 the greatest influence on happiness and health outcomes, and which emotions they are consequently more likely to pursue or avoid (Lau, White, & Schnall, 2013). Scholars have explained this distinction as the difference between decision utility (actual choice based on predicted emotional experience) and experienced utility (what people actually feel) (Kahneman, 2000). This differentiation is important because there are often discrepancies between people’s predicted experiences—which consequently impacts their decisions, and their actual experiences (Wilson & Gilbert, 2003). Given that emotions have such a crucial effect on decision-making, behavior, and health, an investigation into how people think about and choose to pursue or avoid certain emotions may offer insight into optimal strategies for regulating distinct emotions and maximizing global well-being. These exploratory studies may also allow us to construct more nuanced theories regarding the cognitive and motivational mechanisms underlying affect valuation. The Economics of Emotion Just as there is a monetary economy, there is an affective economy, which we refer to as the “emotional marketplace.” There are a number of parallels between the behavior of money and emotion. For example, depressions and inflations can occur not only with monetary tokens of value but also with affective tokens of value (Clore, 2005). Monetary resources are scarce in economic depressions; similarly, emotional resources are often scarce in psychological depressions. In economic inflations, the relationship between representations of value and actual value becomes distorted; likewise, when more emotional currency (e.g. dopamine) is produced than is justified, the feelings lose value, potentially causing individuals to seek more powerful affective experiences (e.g. gambling, taking drugs, engaging in other risky activities). Another possible parallel is affective hoarding (Clore, 2005); just as a scarcity of money can lead to Emotional Marketplace - 7 hoarding in the monetary economy, exacerbating the scarcity, an emotional shortage can lead people to reduce their emotional investments, further diminishing emotional rewards. Additionally, both money and emotion can serve as common currencies in decision- making. Research in neuroeconomics provides insight into the relationship between emotion and money by demonstrating how the brain evaluates rewards. Subjective values of money and emotion are both encoded on a common neural scale by the same neurobiological valuation path (Montague & Berns, 2002; Izuma, Saito, & Sadato; 2008; Saxe & Haushofer, 2008; Levy & Glimcher, 2012). Specifically, single-unit recordings in animals and human fMRI studies (Monosov & Hikosaka, 2012; Smith & Huettel, 2010) suggest that reward-related computations take place in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), and that vmPFC activity reflects both the subjective value of rewards and the exchange rate between rewards in different domains (Kable & Glimcher, 2007; Chib et al., 2009; Rangel & Hare, 2010; Levy & Glimcher, 2011; Levy & Glimcher, 2012). Importantly, the vmPFC not only encodes the value of external rewards, but also the value of emotional stimuli (Goel & Dolan, 2001), offering support for the idea that the vmPFC tracks the value of rewards by a domain-general value signal. These findings support the idea of a “common neural currency” for reward (Montague & Berns, 2002), since distinct rewards—such as money and emotional states—can be compared. Behavioral research has also suggested that emotions can serve as a common currency facilitating the evaluation of gambles with many different outcomes (Connolly & Butler, 2006). Individuals may have trouble with the idea of “economizing” or pricing emotions. For example, trying to exchange money for love is largely looked down upon (Clore, 2005). However, given that economics is concerned with maximizing human satisfaction in the face of scarce resources, and almost all human satisfaction comes from emotional experiences (Elster, Emotional Marketplace - 8 1996), trying to understand monetary spending without taking affective goals into account would involve overlooking one of the most important motivators of decision-making. Thus, we hope to critically investigate how motivated individuals are to achieve different emotions, using experimental paradigms to further understand how individuals evaluate emotional costs and benefits. Measuring Emotional States In the past, economists largely neglected the study of emotions due to their lack of a uniform metric. However, many psychologists have argued that discrete emotions exist and each has measurable properties (Tomkins, 1962; Ekman & Friesen, 1971; Izard, 1971). Correspondingly, emotion researchers have proposed a number of theories for measuring the emotional states that people experience and value. Tsai, Knutson, and Fung (2006) developed the Affect Valuation Theory (AVT), which aims to explain differences in how people ideally want to feel and how they actually feel. Carstensen (1993) proposed the Socioemotional Selectivity Theory, maintaining that limitations on perceived time lead to motivational shifts that direct attention to emotional goals. Harmon-Jones et al. (2011) argued that attitudes toward emotions are organized in a discrete emotions framework—in which individuals have distinct attitudes toward discrete emotions—and also predicted that attitudes toward discrete emotions should predict emotion situation selection. For example, people who greatly dislike embarrassment will be more likely to avoid situations that could potentially arouse embarrassment; conversely, people with a greater liking for awe will be more likely to approach situations that arouse awe than people for whom awe is not as valued. Based on these theories, given that individuals indicate differences between how they actually and ideally want to feel, have motivational differences, have differing attitudes toward distinct emotions, and make behavioral choices in Emotional Marketplace - 9 line with these attitudes, our research will investigate how people assign value to discrete emotions. Although people’s ideal affect and the importance they assign to certain emotions may fluctuate based on the environment and situation, people generally retain an idea of how they want to feel that holds across situations (Tsai, 2013). As ideal affect is mostly stable, it follows that people can be asked to evaluate different emotions without pronounced moment-to-moment shifts caused by external variables. So far, past studies have measured ideal emotional states by asking participants to indicate how much they want to experience or avoid experiencing certain emotions (Augustine et al., 2010; Kampfe & Mitte, 2009; Rusting & Larsen, 1995; Tsai, Knutson, & Fung, 2006). However, it may be useful to consider not just how people evaluate distinct emotions, but how motivated they are to achieve them, even when it involves the investment of additional resources like money or time. Economists often investigate peoples’ preferences as revealed through choice; analyzing market data can be used to identify preferences and estimate price-response functions (Breidert, Hahsler, & Reutterer, 2006). However, when goods are not openly exchanged in the marketplace and purchasing data is not available, stated preferences can be used as an effective substitute (Kahneman et al., 1993). In these situations, people are asked how much they would be willing to pay (WTP) to obtain something positive or to avoid something negative. It follows that emotional experiences could also be quantified using a similar method. Unlike behavior and revenues, emotions are difficult to measure and quantify, but by applying experimental economic paradigms to emotional considerations, we can begin investigating how people assign value to emotions. According to Lau, White, and Schnall (2013), there are a number of reasons why WTP judgments may offer advantages over traditional Likert scale Emotional Marketplace - 10 ratings as a metric for evaluating emotions. First, using the WTP approach accounts for opportunity costs associated with decision utility. In real life, all activities have trade-offs. When participants are asked to price and spend money on emotional experiences, they need to forgo the opportunity of spending that sum on alternative goods; Likert scales do not capture these opportunity costs and trade-offs. Second, the WTP approach more clearly reveals relative preferences. Asking participants to quantify emotional experience using monetary values enables measurable, scaled comparisons between distinct emotions. The WTP measure allows us to not only determine whether experiencing happiness is valued more than experiencing awe, but helps us quantify this preference. Furthermore, while individuals using a Likert scale could report both disgust and anger as “extremely undesirable,” such a response would not reveal their relative preferences for the emotions. Third, this approach extends current understanding of affect valuation by making emotional experiences comparable with other common consumption domains. In comparison to more ambiguous response scales used in past attempts to capture affect valuation, the WTP approach offers a closer approximation of real choices people make in search of specific emotional experiences in their daily lives. By having participants assign value to emotions and other consumption behaviors using a common metric, the WTP approach serves as a more ecologically valid measure for assessing how individuals value emotional experiences relative to other types of rewards and punishments. Asking people to price emotions may seem unintuitive as emotions are not objective and quantifiable in the same way that money is. However, an experimental study conducted by Lau, White, and Schnall (2013) provided preliminary evidence that people are capable of assigning monetary value to emotional experiences; they demonstrated that participants can in fact use a WTP approach to differentiate their preferences for specific emotions. In the study, participants

Description:
emotional marketplace, we plan to use different paradigms to measure additional dimensions of .. how to design an emotional discounting paradigm.
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.