ebook img

Ritualized Aggression and Unstable Dominance in Broods of Crested Ibis (Nipponia nippon) PDF

2004·3.4 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Ritualized Aggression and Unstable Dominance in Broods of Crested Ibis (Nipponia nippon)

Wilson Bulletin, 116(2), 2004, pp. 172-176 RITUALIZED AGGRESSION AND UNSTABLE DOMINANCE IN BROODS OE CRESTED IBIS (NIPPONIA NIPPON) XINHAI LI,' DIANMO LI,' ZHIJUN MA,^ TIANQING ZHAI,^ AND HUGH DRUMMOND^ — ABSTRACT. In broods of Crested Ibis {Nipponia nippon), aggressive dominance was unstable over time, even within feeding sessions. All chicks took turns pecking aggressively while broodmates hung their heads submissively, although roles were contested at the start offeeding bouts when chicks were 11-17 days old. In all broods, at least halfofall pecks werefalsepecks, which did not strike broodmates even when within reach. False pecks seem to be ritualized displays that function to solicit food from parents and possibly to threaten rivals. Received 9 July 2002, accepted 24 March 2004. We describe an extraordinary form of ago- days, 4 weeks after departure from the nest. nistic interaction between broodmates of the Because of food competition, broods of most Crested Ibis {Nipponia nippon), a critically ibis species are facultatively reduced to two endangered ciconiiform (Liu 1981). Aggres- fledglings (Matheu and del Hoyo 1992), but sion among altricial broodmates occurs in a brood reduction is relatively uncommon in variety of avian taxa, including some ibises, Crested Ibis: 78.3% ofhatchlings fledge (Zhai egrets, raptors, boobies, anhingas, guillemots, et al. 2001), compared to 56% ± 14.1 (SD) and kingfishers (reviews in Mock 1984, Mock that fledge in 29 bird species with parental and Parker 1997, Drummond 2002). Gener- feeding and a modal clutch size greater than ally, broodmate hierarchies are formed one (reviewed by Royle et al. 1999). through pecking and biting (review in Drum- We recorded ibis behavior at hillside nests mond 1999), and, in species where siblicide in Shaanxi Province (33° 18' N to 33° 24' N is facultative (Lack 1947, 1954; Ricklefs and 107° 23' E to 107° 28' E), China. Observ- 1965), the intensity of aggression varies with ers sat upslope ofthe colony at vantage points the amount of food provided by parents 15-50 m away from nests and watched broods (Drummond 2001a, 2001b; but see Mock et through a telescope from 07:00-19:00 UTC al. 1987, Forbes and Mock 1994). Threatening + 08. Hatching order (a-chick, b-chick, and postures and calls are common (Drummond so on) was evident from marked differences 2001b), but no species has been reported to in body size that persisted throughout the nest- show ritualized attacks that do not impact the ling period (as in the Bald Ibis, Geronticus victim. eremita; Hirsch 1979). In 1999, we recorded The Crested Ibis feeds on loaches, eels, lo- behavior at nest 9918, where two broodmates, custs, and freshwater invertebrates, including which hatched 2 days apart, were visible from insects, and lays two to four eggs in a tree a vantage point 30 m away. We observed be- nest; eggs hatch at 1- to 2-day intervals havior daily between hatching and fledging 41 (Zheng 1973, Li and Huang 1986). Both par- days later, recording all feeding sessions on ents feed the chicks by regurgitation until the video. In addition, we observed seven broods chicks become independent at about age 70 of two, three, or four chicks (n = one, five, one broods, respectively) on 16 days (2.7 ' State Key Lab. of Integrated Management of Pest days/brood) in 1997, 1998, and 2000, when Insects and Rodents, Inst, of Zoology, Chinese Acad- emy of Sciences, Beijing 100080, China. broods were in Stage 3 (>18 days old). ^ Inst, of Biodiversity, Fudan Univ., Shanghai We recorded the absolute frequencies of 200433, China. feeds and pecks. During each parent’s period Crested Ibis Conservation Station, Yang County of nest attendance, it typically fed the brood 723300, Shaanxi Province, China. in a single session of two to eight regurgita- Inst, de Ecologia, Univ. Nacional Autonoma de MexiCcoor,reAs.pPon7d0i-n2g75a,ut0ho4r5;10e-mDa.iFl.,: Mexico. ctihoincsk. aEgagrcehssrieognuragnidtaatisoinngleelifceietde.dAafbeoeudtwaosf [email protected] recorded whenever a chick received food by 172 — Li et ai • AGGRESSION AND DOMINANCE IN CRESTED IBIS 173 — — Mutual pecks Pecks by a-chick Pecks by b-chick 1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 29 31 33 35 37 39 41 Age (days) ofa-chick EIG. 1. Rate ofpecking at a Crested Ibis (Nipponia nippon) nest, Shaanxi Province, China, 1999. The two chicks (brood 9918) pecked at similar rates, taking turns at aggressive pecking. inserting its bill (usually its whole head) into chick pecked for several seconds and the par- the parent’s bill. Pecks included any rapid ent offered food to it. Sometimes, while the downward thrust of the head, including real chicks were exchanging mutual pecks, the pecks, where open bill tips struck the brood- parent offered its open bill to one of them, mate (usually on the head or nape), diwdfalse which then fed. After submission by one pecks, where no target was struck and the chick, the other chick seemed to diminish its downward thrust ended at the nest floor. The attacks. In Stage 3 (18—41 days old), upon ar- two categories of pecks were recorded simply rival of the parent, one chick (a or b) started as pecks because we often could not tell pecking and the other usually responded by whether a peck was real or false. When the hanging its head submissively. The aggressive chicks at nest 9918 were aggressive during a chick then begged and was fed one or more bout {mutual pecking), they pecked at similar times, all the while continuing to peck its un- frequencies and it was difficult to count all fed nestmate unfil the latter started to peck the pecks; thus, we counted only the mutual pecks aggressive chick and beg for food. Then the ofthe more visible chick. Pecking frequencies fed chick promptly hung its head submissive- of brood 9918 were recovered from video re- ly, and the unfed chick received the next feed- cordings, and those of the seven broods in ing. Stage 3 were recorded using a hand counter. Chicks of brood 9918 begged, without vo- Video recordings were transferred from tape to Audio Video Interleave files using a calizing, by raising their bills and repeatedly tapping the parent’s bill. Pecks at the brood- VideoKing^'^ video compress card (Beijing 1 mate usually were accompanied by simulta- ' wKeerfea Ealneacltyrzoendic uCsoi.ng 19A9d7o),beanPdretmhio.esree fi5l.e0s neous chirping, which occurred in no other (Adobe Creative Team 1998). context and was interpreted as a threat call. Development of aggression andfeeding. Aggression by both chicks of brood 9918 in- In brood 9918, we categorized development creased more or less steadily throughout Stage of feeding and agonistic behavior into three 2 and during the first 10 days of Stage 3, be- stages, according to the age of the a-cfiick. fore declining steadily o\er the last 15 days During Stage (0-10 days old), agonism was of Stage 3 (Eig. I). Parents pro\ided 5.3 ± I absent. During Stage 2 (11-17 days old), ar- 0.8 (SD) feeding sessions per day, with 4.1 ± rival of the parent at the nest was followed by 2.1 regurgitations per session. Atlults did not a bout of begging and mutual peeking until obviously interfere in broodmatc aggression one chick (a or b) submitted by hanging its and tended to Iced whichever chick was beg- head low, whereupon the other (aggressive) ging. Over the 41-day nestling period, the a- 174 THE WILSON BULLETIN • Vol. 116, No. 2, June 2004 chick received 483 feeds and the b-chiek re- chick pecked its broodmates. The a-chieks and ceived 451 feeds. b-chicks (all broods) did not differ (86.8 ± In the other broods, all of the chicks 36.2 and 78.3 ±31.4 pecks/day, respectively; showed aggressive pecking on every day of Wilcoxon rank sum test, Z = 0.886, P — 0.38, = observation. This followed the pattern of two tailed n 7). The a-chiek, b-chick, and brood 9918 in Stage 3, with chicks taking c-chick of each brood did not differ either turns pecking and begging while their brood- (81.9 ± 25.0, 72.6 ± 21.5, and 73.1 ± 23.6 mates hung their heads submissively; there pecks/day, respectively; Kruskal-Wallis test, was no clear consistency with respect to X" = 2.02, P = —0.36, n - 6). which fed hrst or more frequently. Whenever False pecks. False pecks included the a chick pecked, it pecked at all of its brood- threat call and frequently passed within cen- mates. There was no significant difference in timeters of the broodmate, but they also oe- the daily feeding frequencies of a-chicks and curred when the broodmate was out of range. b-chicks (13.7 ± 4.7 and 12.3 ± 3.6 feeds, Despite the victim being immediately in front, respectively; Wilcoxon rank sum test, Z = the aggressor directed pecks toward its own 0.93, P = 0.35, two tailed, n = 7) or of a- flanks, to one side and then the other, clearly chicks, b-chicks, and c-chicks (12.3 ± 1.2, avoiding the easy target (which might be 12.1 ±2.8, and 1 1.4 ± 3.3 feeds, respectively; standing with head lowered in submission) Kruskal-Wallis test, x" = 1.31, P = 0.52, n = and striking nothing. False pecks occurred in 6). — all eight focal broods, and they appeared to Dominance. Dominance oecurred when represent roughly 60-70% of total pecks in one chick was aggressive and the other adopt- brood 9918 and more than half of total pecks ed a submissive posture. Although a chick of- in each ofthe other seven broods. During mu- ten dominated its broodmate briefly, domi- tual peeking, false pecks decreased to <10% nance between chicks was unstable over time, of total pecks. False pecks occurred in Stages even within feeding sessions. In brood 9918, 2 and 3, usually after ordinary begging failed over the 41-day nestling period, the a-chick to elicit parental feeding. They could occur in pecked its broodmate 53.6 ± 39.4 times a day, the absence of genuine pecks at the brood- versus 59.5 ± 48.5 pecks by the b-chick (ex- mate, but they were almost invariably per- cluding mutual pecking). In Stage 2, neither formed by the chick that currently dominated chick tended to dominate first and get the first its broodmate. After fledglings departed the feeding. Sometimes when the b-chick was nest, real pecks were rare because victims pecking aggressively and about to be fed, the promptly fled. False pecking continued during a-chick rose up and, using its superior height, the next 4 weeks (when parental feeding was intercepted the feed. In Stage 3, chicks were supplemented by attempts at self-feeding), only fed while temporarily dominant, and sim- even when the broodmate was out of sight. ilar feeding rates of the a-chick and the b- Like begging and real pecking, false pecking ehiek (XL unpubl. data) reflect similar fre- never occurred in the absence of a parent, and quencies of temporary dominance by the two when false pecks occurred, parents offered broodmates. In most feeding sessions a single food exelusively to the aggressor. chick maintained dominance throughout, but False pecking appears to be a ritualized suceessive dominance was also eommon. form ofreal pecking, and both forms ofpeck- Thus, in Stage 3, 68% of the a-ehick’s 152 ing may elicit parental feeding. Originally, se- feeds were obtained in sessions where the a- lection may have favored parents feeding ag- chick dominated throughout or initially, and gressors, either because dominant chicks are 32% in sessions where the b-chick dominated more worthy of investment or because ap- initially; for the b-chick’s 171 feeds, the b- peasing aggressors is a way ofprotecting their chick’s corresponding values were 64 and broodmates (when aggression is food depen- 36%, respectively. dent, Drummond 2001a). In either scenario, Similar absence of stable dominanee ap- the door would be open for the evolution of peared to be the rule in all dyads of the seven signal funetion: parental feeding could be elic- broods observed in Stage 3. For each brood ited initially by aggressive pecking and sub- we calculated the mean number of times each sequently by false pecking. For the aggressor. Li et al. • AGGRESSION AND DOMINANCE IN CRESTED IBIS 175 the advantage of using false pecks over real 1979, Pegoraro and Thaler 1993, Tuckova pecks may be that false pecks do less physical 1999, Ros et al. 2001). harm to the (long- and sensitive-billed) ag- ACKNOWLEDGMENTS gressor itself or to its sibling broodmate. Hence, false pecks could be more effective For advice on an earlier version of this paper, we than ordinary begging for inducing regurgi- thank H. Kallander and D. W. Mock, as well as M. tation and ensuring feeding priority, and less Cargill, P. O’Conner, and A. Smith. The current costly than real pecks to the aggressor’s in- version was improved by A. Gonzalez Voyer, J. L. Osorno Cepeda, C. Rodriguez Juarez, W. A. Estes and dividual and inclusive fitness. two anonymous referees; C. Rodriguez Juarez helped Selection on parents to discriminate false with manuscript preparation. The study was suppor- pecks from real pecks would not necessarily ted by state key basic and developmental research result in parents declining to respond to false G2000046805, CAS Innovation Program, and the Chi- pecks. Discriminating parents could simply naNatural ScienceFund 39893360; HD’sparticipation devalue the signal, responding to false pecks was supported by CONACYT grant PCCNCNA- 031528. less than to real pecks (but more than to or- dinary begging). Additionally, false pecking LITERATURE CITED could be an especially potent signal if it also warns that violence will follow if food is not Adobe Creative Team. 1998. Adobe Premiere 5.0; classroom in a book. Peachpit Press, Berkeley, forthcoming or goes to the rival; it could deter California. rivals from begging, or blackmail parents into Bejing Kefa Electronic Co. 1997. Guide to preferentially feeding the signaler. In brood VideoKing video compress card. Beijing Kefa 9918 at Stage 2, it seemed that whenever one Electronic Co. Beijing, China (in Chinese). chick begged during its broodmate’s false Drummond, H. 1999. Agonism anddominance in nest- pecking the broodmate responded by attacking ling birds. Proceedings of the International Orni- thological Congress 22:1621-1631. more intensely, with real pecks. Drummond, H. 2001a. A revaluation of the role of False pecking may be associated with the food in broodmate aggression. Animal Behaviour Crested Ibis’s unusual system of unstable 61:517-526. broodmate dominance. Other aggressive Drummond, H. 2001b. The control and function of brood reducers frequently attack even when agonism in avian broodmates. Pages 261-301 in food is not offered (Mock and Parker 1997), Advances in the study of behavior, vol. 30 (P. J. puseirnmganreeanltpseucbkosrdtiontartaiionnbr(oDordummamteosndint2o00m1obr)e. DrumJB.m.oRSnolpdaet,err,,HE.Jd.s2.S).0.0R2Ao.sceaBndbeelgamgtiitcn,gPCr.veesTrs.,suSNsneoawwgdgoYroners,ks.iaonnd Ti.n For whatever reason. Crested Ibis chicks ap- avian broodmate competition. Pages 337-360 in parently attack only to secure immediate feed- The evolution of begging: competition, coopera- ing priority, which may not require intense tion and communication (J. Wright and M. E. Le- and extended violence. onard, Eds.). Kluwer Academic fhiblishers. Dor- Ultimately, false pecking may be related to drecht, The Netherlands. Drummond, IT, C. RoDRi'ctii;/.. A. Vai.i.arino. C. the favorable ecological prospects of Crested Vai.derrabano, G. RociI;i.. and E. Tobon. 2003. Ibis broods, in which all young ordinarily Desperado siblings: uncontrollably aggressiveJu- Hedge (Zhai et al. 2001). Because junior niorchicks. Beha\ioral Ecology anti Socit)biology chicks do not usually face severe food short- 53:287-296. age, they may pose only a negligible compet- Ft)RBi;s, L. S. and D. W. Mock. 1994. Prt)\imate and itive threat to the survival of a-chicks, and as ultimate tieterminants of a\ian brootl rediictitm. earcaontnseoqfuetnhceem, a(-Dcrhiucmkmsomnady bete easl.pec2i0a0l3l)y.toIln- CIIh’’aaugrrem,sig.2iSt3wm7iit-/2ea5trul6ianidEn..ISn.faVntoimcidSeaaankdIpkalsr.e)n.taHlacrawroeo(dS.. Bald Ibis broods, however, frequent brood re- IIiRSCii. U. 1979. Stuilies of West Palearctic birds duction signifies more severe food shortage 183 Bald Ibis. British Birds 72:313 325. (although chicks show successive dominance I.A( K. I). 1947. I'he signilicance of clutch si/e. Ibis within feeding bouts, similar to the Chested 89:302 352. Ibis); in this case, the order of dominance ex- L\( K. D. 1954. Ihe natural regulation ot animal num- bers. Clarentlon. Oxiortl. I’niled Kingilom. pression and access to food is dictated by a I.i. F' L. AND S. (,) III wo. 1986. Ihe iinestigation of stable-dominance hierarchy and false pecking the re[iroiluction beha\iorol the Cresteil Ibis. Bul- does not occur (llirsch 1979, Oliver et al. letin of Biology 12:6 8 (m Chinese). 176 THE WILSON BULLETIN • Vol. 116, No. 2, June 2004 Liu, Y. Z. 1981. Rediscovery ofthe Crested Ibis in Q Pegoraro, K. AND E. Thaler. 1993. Postembryonic Mountain. Acta Zoologica Sinica 27:273 (in Chi- development and juvenile phase of the Northern nese). Bald Ibis or Waldrapp Ibis Geronticus eremita. Matheu, E. and J. del Hoyo. 1992. Eamily Thres- Okologie der Vogel 15:155-192 (in German). kiornithidae. Pages 472-506 in Handbook of the Ricklefs, R. E. 1965. Brood reduction in the Curve- birds ofthe world, vol. 1: ostrich to ducks (J. del billed Thrasher. Condor 67:505-510. Hoyo, A. Elliott, and J. Sargatal, Eds.). Lynx Ed- Ros, A. E H., K. Hirschenhauser, andR. E Oliveira. icions, Barcelona, Spain. 2001. The interaction between organizational and Mock, D. W. 1984. Infanticide, siblicide, and avian activational effects of testosterone in the control nestling mortality. Pages 3-30 in Infanticide: of early aggression in birds: a comment on Sas- comparative and evolutionary perspectives (G. vari, Hegyi and Peczely. Ethology 107:851-853. Hausfater and S. B. Hrdy, Eds.). Aldine, New Royle, N. j., I. R. Hartley, I. P. E Owens, and G. A. Parker. 1999. Sibling competition and evolution York. Mock, D. W., T. C. Lamey, C. E Willliams, and B. oSfocgireotywtohfrLatoensdoinn,biSrdesr.iePsroBce2e6d6i:n9g2s3-o9f3t2h.e Royal J. Ploger. 1987. Proximate and ultimate roles of Tuckova, K. 1999. Nestlingsaggression bei Waldrap- food amount in regulating egret sibling aggres- pen {Geronticuseremita). M.Sc. thesis. University sion. Ecology 68:1760-1772. of Vienna, Austria. Mock, D. W. and G. A. Parker. 1997. The evolution Zhai, T. Q., X. R. Lu, B. Z. Lu, Y. M. Zhang, and H. of sibling rivalry. Oxford University Press, Ox- J. Wang. 2001. Nest building, egg laying, hatch- ford, United Kingdom. ing, and breeding of Crested Ibis (Nipponia nip- Oliver, W. L. R., M. M. Mallet, D. R. Singleton, pon). Acta Zoologica Sinica 47:508-51 (in Chi- 1 AND J. S. Ellett. 1979. Observations on the re- nese). productive behavior of a captive colony of Bare- Zheng, Z. X. 1973. The avifauna of Qinling Mount. faced Ibis Geronticus eremita. Journal ofthe Jer- Chinese Science Press, Beijing, China (in Chi- sey Wildlife Preservation Trust 16:11-35. nese).

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.