ebook img

Rhythms of the brain PDF

465 Pages·2006·5.429 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Rhythms of the brain

Rhythms of the Brain György Buzsáki OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS Rhythms of the Brain This page intentionally left blank Rhythms of the Brain György Buzsáki 1 2006 3 Oxford University Press, Inc., publishes works that further Oxford University’s objective ofexcellence in research, scholarship, and education. Oxford New York Auckland Cape Town Dar es Salaam Hong Kong Karachi Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi New Delhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto With offices in Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam Copyright © 2006 by Oxford University Press, Inc. Published by Oxford University Press, Inc. 198 Madison Avenue, New York, New York 10016 www.oup.com Oxford is a registered trademark ofOxford University Press All rights reserved. No part ofthis publication may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise, without the prior permission ofOxford University Press. Library ofCongress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Buzsáki, G. Rhythms ofthe brain / György Buzsáki. p. cm. Includes bibliographical references and index. ISBN-13 978-0-19-530106-9 ISBN 0-19-530106-4 1. Brain—Physiology. 2. Oscillations. 3. Biological rhythms. [DNLM: 1. Brain—physiology. 2. Cortical Synchronization. 3. Periodicity. WL 300 B992r 2006] I. Title. QP376.B88 2006 612.8'2—dc22 2006003082 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Printed in the United States ofAmerica on acid-free paper To my loved ones. This page intentionally left blank Prelude Ifthe brain were simple enough for us to understand it, we would be too sim- ple to understand it. —Ken Hill The short punch line ofthis book is that brains are foretelling devices and their predictive powers emerge from the various rhythms they perpetually generate. At the same time, brain activity can be tuned to become an ideal observer ofthe en- vironment, due to an organized system of rhythms. The specific physiological functions ofbrain rhythms vary from the obvious to the utterly impenetrable. A simple but persuasive example is walking. Bipedal walking is a periodic series of forward falls interrupted regularly by alternate extensions ofeach leg. It is almost as natural to us as breathing. This effortless exercise is made possible by the pre- dictive nature ofspinal cord oscillators. On smooth terrain, the alternation ofleg movements can take us any distance. Perturbation of the clocking, on the other hand, signals a change in the terrain. This general mechanism is the same in all animals, including eight-legged scorpions and centipedes. The notion that oscilla- tors or “central pattern generators”1are responsible for the coordination ofmotor 1. Neural circuits that produce self-sustaining patterns ofbehavior are called central pattern gen- erators. The most studied central pattern generator is an intraspinal network ofneurons responsible for locomotion. Grillner (1985) summarizes the pros and cons ofthe pacemaker view ofcentral pattern generators in the spinal cord and brain. Stein et al. (1997) and Burke (2001) are nice updates on the topic. Central pattern generators are also responsible for many other types ofrhythmic movements, e.g., peristaltic motor patterns oflegless animals, rhythmic movement ofthe wings ofcrickets during song production, respiration, heart control, movements ofthe stomach, and other parts ofthe digestive system. My favorite review on this topic is Marder and Calabrese (1996). viii Prelude patterns, such as breathing and walking, is old and well accepted in neuroscience. But the tantalizing conjecture that neuronal oscillators can be exploited for a plethora ofother brain-generated functions, including cognition, is quite new and controversial. And it is the latter topic, the contribution ofoscillations to the in- visible, inferred operations ofthe brain, that this book is mostly about. Exposing the mechanisms that allow complicated things to happen in a coordi- nated fashion in the brain has produced some ofthe most spectacular discoveries ofneuroscience. However, I do not want to mislead you from the outset. Clocks are not thinking but ticking devices, no matter how precisely they can predict time. Time needs to be filled with content, provided by the appropriate firing pat- terns ofneurons, whose assembly activity, in turn, is regulated by brain oscilla- tions. Interestingly, the neuronal assemblies that generate the content are often the same as those that give rise to the time metric ofoscillations that in turn orga- nize the cell assembly pattern. This peculiar reciprocal causation, brought about by the self-organized features ofbrain activity, begs for an explanation. A good part ofthe volume is devoted to discussing experiments that attempt to elucidate these emerging properties ofneuronal networks. At the physiological level, oscillators do a great service for the brain: they co- ordinate or “synchronize” various operations within and across neuronal net- works. Syn(meaning same) and chronos(meaning time) together make sure that everyone is up to the job and no one is left behind, the way the conductor creates temporal order among the large number of instruments in an orchestra. A close view ofSeiji Ozawa at the end ofa concert, sweat falling from his face, is proof that conducting an orchestra is a physically and mentally demanding job. In con- trast, coupled oscillators perform the job ofsynchronization virtually effortlessly. This feature is built into their nature. In fact, oscillators do not do much else. They synchronize and predict. Yet, take away these features, and our brains will no longer work. Compromise them, and we will be treated for epilepsy, Parkinson’s disease, sleep disorders, and other rhythm-based cognitive maladies. As I point out repeatedly in Cycles 1–13 ofthis volume, virtually no nervous function exists without a time metric, be it the simplest motor or the most complex cognitive act. While we know quite a bit about neurons, the building blocks of the brain, and have extensive knowledge about their connectivity, we still know very little how the modules and systems ofmodules work together. This is where oscillations of- fer their invaluable services. My connection with brain rhythms began in April 1970, during a physiology lecture given by Endre Grastyán in the beautiful town ofPécs, on the sunny slopes of the Mecsek mountains in Hungary. The University of Pécs, or Universitas Quinque Ecclesiensis, as it was called when founded in 1367, has produced a re- markable set ofneuroscientists, including János Szentágothai, the legendary neu- roanatomist; Béla Flerkó and Béla Halász, pioneers of neuroendocrinology; György Székely, the renowned spinal cord physiologist; and Ferenc Gallyas, the creator ofthe silver impregnation methods widely used for neuronal labeling. Like many ofus at a young age, in his twenties Grastyán could not quite make up his mind about his future. Finding nothing too interesting or challenging initially, Prelude ix he decided to train for the priesthood to get some orientation in philosophy. But his mind, far too curious and questioning, prevented him from becoming a preacher. He ended up in medical school during the stormy years after World War II and became the assistant ofProfessor Kálmán Lissák. Lissák, a student ofOtto Loewi in Graz, Austria, and subsequently Walter Cannon’s assistant at Harvard, had returned to Hungary to become Chair of Physiology just before the war. Grastyán’s pairing with Lissák was fortunate because Lissák, of course, knew quite a bit about rhythms from his years with Loewi, who provided the first evi- dence that a chemical—a neurotransmitter—is released at the junction (synapse) between the vagus nerve and the heart muscle.2Although Grastyán was perhaps Lissák’s closest friend, the two were as different as can be. Lissák was a reserved man, and his lectures were scarcely attended. In contrast, Grastyán was a per- forming artist whose seminars were carefully composed and choreographed. The huge lecture room in the medical school was always packed, and even students from the neighboring law school came over to listen to his mesmerizing lectures. He generated so much enthusiasm that we students became convinced that the topics he discussed were among the most important in the whole universe. In that particular lecture ofApril 1970, he talked about how the brain outputs, such as movement and cognition, control its inputs, rather than the other way around. His key idea was that control in living systems begins with the output. This is the seed for further evolution ofthe brain. Even in the most complex animals, the goal ofcognition is the guidance ofaction. Indeed, the first simple biological systems did not have any inputs; they did not need them. They simply used an eco- nomical motor output, a rhythmic contraction ofmuscles. This is, ofcourse, is suf- ficient only when food is abundant in the sea environment. More complex forms of life evolved form this simple solution by modifying the simple rhythmic output. Sensation ofdirection and distance developed only after the “invention” ofmove- ment through space. The idea ofoutput control and feedback is a profound thought even today. Back then, when Pavlovian sensory–sensory association was the dom- inant ideology in the East and the stimulus–decision–response paradigm domi- nated Western thinking, Grastyán’s teachings were unusual, to say the least. After his lecture, I rushed home to read the relevant chapters in our official 2. Loewi called the chemical “Vagusstoff,” which Henry Hallett Dale from Cambridge, England, identified later as acetylcholine, the first neurotransmitter. They received the Nobel Prize for their dis- coveries in 1936. I have heard various versions ofthe story behind the Vagustoffexperiment from Lis- sák. Here is one from Loewi’s own pen: The night before Easter Sunday ofthat year I awoke, turned on the light, and jotted down a few notes on a tiny slip ofthin paper. Then I felt asleep again. It occurred to me at six o’clock in the morning that I had written down something most important, but I was unable to decipher the scrawl. The next night, at three o’clock, the idea returned. It was the experiment to determine whether or not the hypothesis ofchemical transmission that I had thought about years ago was correct. I got up immediately, went to the laboratory, and performed a simple experiment on a frog heart according to the nocturnal design. (Loewi, 1960, 15) Dale became better known about his “principle”: ifa chemical is released in one synapse, the same chemical is released in all the other synapses made by the same neuron.

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.