ebook img

Research Article Mismatch Negativity and P50 Sensory Gating in Abstinent Former Cannabis Users PDF

12 Pages·2016·1.56 MB·English
by  
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Research Article Mismatch Negativity and P50 Sensory Gating in Abstinent Former Cannabis Users

Hindawi Publishing Corporation Neural Plasticity Volume 2016, Article ID 6526437, 11 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/6526437 Research Article Mismatch Negativity and P50 Sensory Gating in Abstinent Former Cannabis Users SamanthaJ.Broyd,1,2Lisa-marieGreenwood,1,2HendrikaH.vanHell,1,2 RodneyJ.Croft,1,2,3HannahCoyle,1BenLee-Bates,1JuanitaTodd,4,5StuartJ.Johnstone,1 PatriciaT.Michie,4,5andNadiaSolowij1,2,5 1SchoolofPsychology,UniversityofWollongong,Wollongong,NSW2522,Australia 2IllawarraHealthandMedicalResearchInstitute,UniversityofWollongong,Wollongong,NSW2522,Australia 3CentreforHealthInitiatives,UniversityofWollongong,Wollongong,NSW2522,Australia 4SchoolofPsychologyandPriorityResearchCentreforTranslationalNeuroscienceandMentalHealth,UniversityofNewcastle, Newcastle,NSW2308,Australia 5SchizophreniaResearchInstitute,Sydney,NSW2021,Australia CorrespondenceshouldbeaddressedtoNadiaSolowij;[email protected] Received30September2015;Accepted20January2016 AcademicEditor:ChristianCasanova Copyright©2016SamanthaJ.Broydetal. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License,whichpermitsunrestricteduse,distribution,andreproductioninanymedium,providedtheoriginalworkisproperly cited. Prolongedheavyexposuretocannabisisassociatedwithimpairedcognitionandbrainfunctionalandstructuralalterations.We recentlyreportedattenuatedmismatchnegativity(MMN)andalteredP50sensorygatinginchroniccannabisusers.Thisstudy investigatedtheextentofbrainfunctionalrecovery(indexedbyMMNandP50)inchronicusersaftercessationofuse.Eighteen ex-users(median13.5yearspriorregularuse;median3.5yearsabstinence)and18nonuserscompleted(1)amultifeatureoddball taskwithduration,frequency,andintensitydeviantsand(2)aP50paired-clickparadigm.TrendlevelsmallerdurationMMN amplitude and larger P50 ratios (indicative of poorer sensory gating) were observed in ex-users compared to controls. Poorer P50gatingcorrelatedwithpriordurationofcannabisuse.DurationofabstinencewaspositivelycorrelatedwithdurationMMN amplitude,evenaftercontrollingforageanddurationofcannabisuse.ImpairedsensorygatingandattenuatedMMNamplitude tendedtopersistinex-usersafterprolongedcessationofuse,suggestingalackoffullrecovery.Anassociationwithprolonged durationofpriorcannabisusemayindicatepersistentcannabis-relatedalterationstoP50sensorygating.Greaterreductionsin MMNamplitudewithincreasingabstinence(positivecorrelation)mayberelatedtoeitherself-medicationoranacceleratedaging process. 1.Introduction alterations to the regulatory role that the endocannabinoid systemplaysinsynapticplasticity[7–10].Thelatteristhought Regular and prolonged cannabis use is associated with a to occur, at least in part, via downregulation of N-methyl- range of adverse psychological outcomes, including psy- D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor activity [7–9, 11]. Evidence chosis [1–4]. The primary constituent of cannabis, Δ9- isalsoemergingforanalogousalterationsincannabisusers tetrahydrocannabinol (THC), is a partial agonist at central and patients with schizophrenia, including cognition, brain cannabinoidreceptor(CB1R)siteswhicharedenselylocated function, and structure, and there is increasing support throughoutthebrainbutespeciallywithinregionscriticalto for an association between cannabis and schizophrenia in attention,learning,andmemory,suchasthehippocampus, vulnerable individuals [12–14]. In light of this evidence, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex [5]. It is these cognitive research has turned to examining common neurobiological functions in particular that are impaired in cannabis users andneuropsychologicalmarkersofdysfunction(forareview, (for a review, see [6]), arguably due to cannabis-related see[6]). 2 NeuralPlasticity Twosuchcandidatemarkersarethemismatchnegativity determine the degree to which recovery may occur as a (MMN) and the P50, event-related potential (ERP) brain functionofpriorcannabisexposureandlengthofabstinence. markers of early sensory memory and of sensory gating, Theaimofthecurrentcross-sectionalstudy,inwhichwe respectively. MMN issensitiveto NMDAreceptorfunction recruited former cannabis users with a range of abstinence (for a review, see [15, 16]) and elicited by irregular acoustic andpriorhistoryofuse,wastoinvestigate(1)whetherMMN deviations (“deviants”) in a predictable pattern of auditory amplitude and P50 sensory gating improve as a function stimuli(calledstandards[17,18])reflectingevaluationofnew of increasing periods of abstinence following cessation of auditorysensoryinputagainstcurrentmodelsoftheacoustic use and (2) the extent to which recovery may depend on environment [18]. Robust reductions in MMN have been the degree of prior cannabis use. Consistent with previous notedinpatientswithschizophrenia[19,20]andlessconsis- work, we predicted that ex-users would exhibit attenuated tently in their first degree relatives [21–23]. Cannabis users frequencyanddurationMMN(impairedsensoryprocessing) also show reduced MMN, with the strongest evidence for and larger P50 ratios and smaller P50 difference scores alterations to frequency deviants [24–26], but three studies (poorer sensory gating) compared to nonuser controls and have also reported group differences for duration deviants thatimprovementinbrainfunctionwouldbepositivelycor- [24, 27, 28]. We recently reported significantly smaller relatedwithdurationofabstinenceandnegativelycorrelated frequency MMN in both long-term and short-term users withdurationofpriorcannabisuse. comparedtocontrols,whereasdurationMMNwasreduced only in long-term users, and duration MMN amplitude 2.MaterialsandMethods reductionwasassociatedwiththedurationofregularand,in particular,dailycannabisuse[24].Consistentwithmounting 2.1. Participants. Twenty abstinent former cannabis users evidence of an association between cannabis use and risk and 42 healthy nonuser controls took part in the current for psychosis, reductions in duration and frequency MMN study.Ex-cannabisuserswererequiredtohaveusedcannabis were associated with a retrospective measure of psychotic- regularly (≥3 times/week) for at least two years prior to likesymptomsduringintoxication[24]. quitting and to have abstained for at least one month; two P50sensorygatingisalsoimpairedinbothpatientswith wereexcludedforrecentotherdruguseleavingasampleof schizophrenia [29, 30] and chronic cannabis users [31–34]. 18 ex-users. Nonuser control participants were required to Sensory gating refers to the brain’s ability to regulate its have<20lifetimeoccasionsofcannabisuseandnoneinthe response to incoming stimuli [35] or to “gate out” irrele- previous 12 months. Recruitment and exclusion criteria are vantinformationfromfurtherprocessing[36–38].(Sensory describedinSupplement1inSupplementaryMaterialavail- gating-“out”maybecontrastedwiththebrain’sabilityto“gate able online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2016/6526437. Con- in”novelornewinformation,ofwhichtheMMNisamarker trols were recruited as part of a larger study (see [24, 31]). [36,37].)P50sensorygatingisimpairedinregularcannabis Whilst ex-users completed all experimental paradigms, not userscomparedtobothnonusercontrols[31–34]andpatients allcontrolscompletedbothMMNandP50paradigms.There- withschizophreniawithandwithoutcomorbidcannabisuse fore,twooverlappingsubsamples(𝑛 = 18each)ofthemost [33]. Longer duration of use [33] and greater quantity of appropriately age-matched controls were blindly selected monthlyuse[32]wereassociatedwithmorepronouncedP50 for MMN and P50 analyses, respectively, from the nonuser gatingimpairments. sample reported in Greenwood et al. [24] (12 participants Theexistenceofbrainalterationsincannabisusersleads wereincludedinbothcontrolgroups).Allparticipantswere totheimportantquestionofwhethertheseareamenableto reimbursed AUD$50 for their participation. The study was recovery following cessation of cannabis use. Persistent IQ approved by the University of Wollongong and Illawarra declinehasbeenreportedinadultcannabisuserswhostarted ShoalhavenLocalHealthDistrictHealthandMedicalHuman using cannabis during adolescence and who had recently ResearchEthicsCommittee. abstainedorsignificantlyreducedcannabisuseforatleastone year[39].Resultsfrommoresensitiveneuroimagingstudies 2.2. Demographic and Substance Use Variables. Participants of users with at least 25 days abstinence indicate persistent underwent a comprehensive structured interview to assess alterationstobrainfunction[40–46].Wepreviouslyreported demographic information, detailed history of current and impairedERPmeasuresofselectiveattentioninformerusers previoussubstanceuse,andpsychiatrichistoryandex-users withamean2-yearabstinencethatwereassociatedwithdura- providedaurinesampletoverifyrecentabstinence.Partic- tionofprioruse[47].Pertinenttothecurrentstudy,Rentzsch ipantswereexcluded iftheyreportedapersonalorfamilial etal.[33]identifiedpersistentdeficitsinP50sensorygating (first degree relative) history of any psychotic disorder. in cannabis users who had been abstinent for at least 28 Participantsreported(i)thelasttimetheyusedanycannabis daysandinasubsequentstudy[25]persistentreductionsin andhowmuch;(ii)theirpreviouspatternsofregularuse(i.e., frequencyMMNamplitudeinformeruserswithamean19- theirusualpatternofuse,periodswhentheyusedcannabis monthabstinence.Neitherstudyinvestigatedtheassociation moreorlessfrequently,andperiodsofabstinence);and(iii) between duration of abstinence or prior cannabis use and when they stopped using cannabis regularly. This informed recoveryofbrainfunction.Therefore,althoughthesefindings twokeyvariablestodefinethedurationofabstinence:(i)time suggestpersistentcannabis-relatedalterationstoMMNand sincelastuse,definedinyearssincelastuseofanycannabis P50 sensory gating following cessation of cannabis use, and(ii)timesincelastregularuseofcannabis(>once/month further research is required to replicate these findings and forany6-monthperiod). NeuralPlasticity 3 The vocabulary and matrices subscales of the Wechsler Table1:Cannabisusemeasuresincludingabstinence,retrospective Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI [49]) were used to measuresofdependenceandpsychologicalsymptomsduringintox- estimatefullscaleIQ.TheReyAuditoryVerbalLearningTest ication,andwithdrawalsymptoms. (RAVLT)wasalsoadministeredandparticipantscompleted Ex-cannabisusers the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE Priorcannabisuse [50])andSchizotypalPersonalityQuestionnaire(SPQ[51])to assesspsychotic-likesymptoms,theBeckDepressionInven- Frequency(days/month) 30[15.0–30.0] a tory (BDI [52]), State Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI [53]), Quantity(cones /month) 517.5[52.5–3150.0] Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [54], and the Alcohol Ageofonset(years) 17[12.0–20.5] UseDisordersIdentificationTest(AUDIT[55]).Ex-cannabis Durationofregularuse(years) 13.5[4.3–30.3] users also completed the Marijuana Withdrawal Checklist Durationofdailyuse(months) 155.3[0.0–363.7] (MWC [56]), Severity of Dependence Scale (SDS [57]), Timesincelastsmoked(years) 1.5[0.1–16.0] and Cannabis Experiences Questionnaire (CEQ, [58]) to Abstinencesinceregularuse(years) 3.5[0.1–16.0] measuresymptomsofwithdrawalandretrospectiveseverity b SDS 8.50[1.0–13.0] ofdependenceandsymptomsexperiencedwhilstintoxicated. c CEQ Euphoria 40[32.0–52.0] 2.3.ExperimentalParadigms. ThepassivemultifeatureMMN Paranoia 48[28.0–87.0] paradigmandrecordingconditions,withduration(100ms), frequency (1200Hz), and intensity (90dB; standards 50ms, Aftereffects 27[15.0–51.0] 1000Hz, 80dB) deviants, are described in Greenwood et Amotivation 18.5[10.0–31.0] al. [24] and further in Supplement 1. For the MMN task Psychotic 8[4.0–20.0] participants were instructed to ignore the tones and focus MWCd 10.5[0.0–37.0] their attention on a silent film. The attended P50 paired- Notes.Dataarereportedasmedian[range]. clickparadigm(inwhichparticipantswereaskedtosilently aConesusedinwaterpipe:3conesareequivalenttoonestandardsizedjoint counttheclickpairsandrespondtoevery25thclickpairvia [48]. b button press) and recording condition (ISI 9 seconds) were SeverityofDependenceScores(SDS)(retrospective) c CannabisExperiencesQuestionnaire(CEQ)(retrospective) identicaltotheparadigmdescribedinBroydetal.[31]andin d MarijuanaWithdrawalChecklist(MWC)(retrospective). Supplement1.Participantswereseatedinacomfortablechair approximately80cmawayfromamonitorinadimlylitroom. All stimuli were presented binaurally using headphones atapproximately6joints/dayonamedianof30days/month (SennheiserHD215).MeasurementdetailsofMMNandP50 (see Table1). They had not used cannabis regularly for a aredescribedinSupplement1andGreenwoodetal.[24]and median of 3.5 years and had been totally abstinent (since Broydetal.[31]respectively. last occasion of any use) for a median of 1.5 years (range of6weeksto16yearsforbothmeasuresofabstinence).No 2.4.StatisticalAnalysis. Toinvestigatepotentialgroupdiffer- participants had used any substance other than alcohol or encesondemographicfactors(age,gender,andIQ),psycho- tobaccoregularly(definedasmorethanonce/monthfora6- logicalsymptoms,verballearning,andalcoholandtobacco monthperiod)inthepast5years. use, independent sample 𝑡-tests or Mann-Whitney 𝑈 tests (for data not normally distributed) were employed. Group 3.2. Group Comparisons on Demographic and Clinical Vari- comparisons of MMN peak amplitude were performed ables. No differences between ex-users and controls were usingrepeatedmeasuresAnalysisofVariance(rmANOVA) identified for age, gender, handedness, verbal learning (i.e., including a within-subject variable, condition (3: duration, RAVLTperformance),orsymptommeasures(Table2)other frequency,andintensitydeviant),andbetween-subjectfactor, thantheSPQscoresreportedbelow. group(2:controlandex-user).P50ratioanddifferencescores werecomparedbetweengroupsusingMann-Whitney𝑈tests MMN Group Comparisons. Ex-users had fewer years of as the data were not normally distributed. Correlational education (𝑍 = −3.64, 𝑝 < .001), tended to have lower analysis investigated associations between MMN and P50 IQ (𝑍 = −1.80, 𝑝 = .073), consumed a greater quantity of measures,cannabismeasures(durationofabstinenceandof alcohol (𝑍 = −2.08, 𝑝 = .04) with a trend toward greater prioruse),andrelevantdemographic,clinical,andcognitive frequency(𝑍=−1.71,𝑝=.09),andsmokedmorecigarettes measures in the ex-user sample. As prior research suggests thancontrols(𝑍 = −3.00,𝑝 = .003).Ex-usershadelevated thatfrequencyanddurationMMNarereducedincannabis SPQsocialanxiety(𝑍 = −2.03,𝑝 = .04)andatrendtoward users in particular [24], correlations between MMN and elevated SPQ total (𝑍 = −1.74, 𝑝 = .08) scores. None cannabis use history including duration of abstinence were of these variables were correlated with MMN amplitude in restrictedtothesedeviantconditions. any deviant condition and therefore none were included as covariatesinanyfurtheranalyses(seeSupplement1). 3.Results P50 Group Comparisons. Ex-users tended to have lower IQ 3.1. Cannabis Use History and Abstinence. Ex-users had scores(𝑍=−1.87,𝑝=.064)andhadfeweryearsofeducation previouslyusedcannabisregularlyforamedianof13.5years (𝑍 = −3.92, 𝑝 < .001) than controls. They smoked more 4 NeuralPlasticity Table2:Demographic,cognitive,andpsychologicalsymptommeasuresinex-cannabisusersandhealthynonusercontrolsforMMNand P50analyses. Ex-cannabisusers MMNcontrols P50controls (𝑛=18) (𝑛=18) (𝑛=18) Age(years) 39.1[20.8–56.0] 40.4[21.0–52.6] 31.2[20.1–52.6] Gender(male/female) 10M/8F 10M/8F 9M/9F Handedness Allright-handed Twoleft-handed Allright-handed ∗∗ ∗∗ Education(years) 12.0[10.0–16.0] 14.3[11.5–20.0] 14.5[11.5–20.0] / / IQ 105.0[89.0–126.0] 115.5[89.0–133.0] 115.0[89.0–133.0] RAVLTtrialsItoV 54.00[34.0–68.0] 54.5[29.0–69.0] 56.50[29.0–70.0] RAVLTtrialVI 11.5[5.0–15.0] 12.0[4.0–15.0] 12.0[4.0–15.0] RAVLTtrialVII 11.5[0.0–15.0] 11.0[3.0–15.0] 11.0[3.0–15.0] a / Alcoholfrequency 3.5[0.0–30.0] 1.0[0.0–12.0] 1.4[0.0–12.0] b ∗ Alcoholquantity 16.8[0.0–180.0] 5.1[0.0–28.0] 6.3[0.0–70.0] ∗ ∗ Cigarettes(perday) 3.8[0.0–35.0] 0.0[0.0–12.0] 0.0[0.0–11.0] Psychologicalsymptoms K10 15.0[12.0–31.0] 14.0[10.0–19.0] 14.0[10.0–22.0] BDI 6.5[0.0–18.0] 3.0[0.0–19.0] 3.0[0.0–19.0] STAI-I 31.0[20.0–67.0] 32.0[20.0–45.0] 27.5[20.0–45.0] STAI-II 39.0[21.0–62.0] 36.0[22.0–50.0] 32.5[20.0–45.0] CAPE Frequencytotal 58.0[46.0–76.0] 60.0[46.0–89.0] 59.0[47.0–89.0] Distresstotal 20.0[4.0–51.0] 25.5[6.0–95.0] 22.0[5.0–50.0] Negativefrequency 21.5[16.0–35.0] 21.5[12.0–31.0] 21.0[15.0–31.0] Negativedistress 11.0[2.0–30.0] 10.5[2.0–41.0] 9.0[2.0–22.0] Positivefrequency 23.5[20.0–31.0] 25.0[21.0–44.0] 26.0[20.0–44.0] Positivedistress 5.5[0.0–20.0] 6.0[1.0–29.0] 6.0[0.0–18.0] Depressivefrequency 12.5[9.0–16.0] 13.0[9.0–21.0] 12.0[9.0–21.0] Depressivedistress 6.0[2.0–15.0] 8.0[2.0–25.0] 7.0[1.0–21.0] SPQ / / Total 18.0[9.0–32.0] 14.0[5.0–32.0] 12.0[2.0–39.0] Ideasofreference 1.0[0.0–6.0] 1.0[0.0–4.0] 1.0[0.0–5.0] ∗ ∗ Socialanxiety 3.0[0.0–15.0] 2.0[0.0–6.0] 1.0[0.0–6.0] Oddbeliefs 1.0[0.0–5.0] 0.0[0.0–5.0] 0.0[0.0–7.0] Unusualperceptual 1.0[0.0–3.0] 1.0[0.0–2.0] 1.0[0.0–4.0] Oddbehaviour 2.0[0.0–5.0] 0.0[0.0–6.0] 0.0[0.0–6.0] Closefriends 2.0[0.0–8.0] 1.0[0.0–3.0] 1.0[0.0–4.0] Oddspeech 4.0[0.0–7.0] 4.0[1.0–8.0] 3.0[0.0–8.0] Constrictedaffect 1.0[0.0–5.0] 1.0[0.0–2.0] 1.0[0.0–4.0] Suspiciousness 1.0[0.0–5.0] 1.0[0.0–3.0] 1.0[0.0–3.0] Notes.Comparisonsbetweenex-cannabisusersandrespectivenonusercontrolsforeachparadigm(MMNorP50);/𝑝 < .10,∗𝑝 < .05,∗∗𝑝 < .001.Data reportedasmedian[range]. a Alcoholfrequencymeasuredasnumberofdayspermonthalcoholwasconsumed. b Alcoholquantitymeasuredasnumberofstandarddrinksconsumedpermonth. RAVLT:ReyAuditoryVerbalLearningTest;K10:KesslerPsychologicalDistressScale;BDI:BeckDepressionInventory;STAI-I:StateAnxietyIndexStatescore; STAI-II:StateAnxietyIndexTraitscore;CAPE:CommunityAssessmentofPsychicExperiences;SPQ:SchizotypalPersonalityQuestionnaire. cigarettesperday(𝑍=−2.97,𝑝=.01)butdidnotdifferfrom 3.3.MismatchNegativityinEx-UsersandControls. Mismatch controls in quantity or frequency of alcohol use (𝑝 > .10). negativity difference waveforms at Fz for duration and fre- Ex-usersagainhadelevatedSPQsocialanxiety(𝑍 = −2.43, quencyconditionsareshowninFigure1(a)andgrandmean 𝑝=.015)andatrendtowardselevatedSPQtotal(𝑍=−1.76, ERP waveforms at Fz to the standard and deviant stimuli 𝑝=.079)scores.Noneofthesevariableswerecorrelatedwith are shown in Figure1(b). Despite visible group differences P50metricsandthereforenonewereincludedascovariatesin in the standard ERP around N1 (explored in Supplement anyfurtheranalyses(seeSupplement1). 1), there were no significant differences between ex-users NeuralPlasticity 5 Duration Frequency −6 −6 −4 −4 −2 −2 −100𝜇V) 0 100 200 300 400 −100𝜇V) 0 100 200 300 400 ( 0 ( 0 2 2 4 4 Ex-user Ex-user Control Control (a) −6 Duration −6 Frequency −4 −4 −2 −2 −100𝜇V) 0 100 200 300 400 −100 0 100 200 300 400 ( 0 0 2 2 4 4 Ex-user standard Control standard Ex-user standard Control standard Ex-user deviant Control deviant Ex-user deviant Control deviant (b) Figure1:(a)Mismatchnegativity(MMN)waveformsatFzdisplayedforeachconditionforex-cannabisusers(greyline)andnonusercontrols (blackline).(b)MastoidreferenceunsubtractedERPwaveformsatFztostandard(solidline)anddevianttones(dashedlines)forex-cannabis users(greylines)andnonusercontrols(blacklines).Amplitudeisshownin𝜇Vonthe𝑦-axisandtimeinmillisecondsalongthe𝑥-axis. andcontrolsinmeanamplitudeforstandardERPsoverthe thepatternofresultsnotappreciablyaffectedbycontrolling latencywindowsusedforMMNpeakdetection. for either duration of use (partial 𝑟 = .72, 𝑝 = .001 and Ex-users had visibly smaller MMN amplitudes than 𝑟 = .74, 𝑝 = .001, resp.) or age (partial 𝑟 = .63, 𝑝 = .007 controls, although this did not reach significance as a main and𝑟 = .65,𝑝 = .005,resp.)separatelyorcombined(partial effectofgroup(𝐹(1,34)=1.02,𝑝=.32).However,therewas 𝑟 = .42, 𝑝 = .053 and 𝑟 = .47, 𝑝 = .032, resp.). However, amaineffectofcondition(𝐹(2,68) = 38.63,𝑝 < .001)anda age and duration of use were not correlated with duration significantconditionbygroupinteraction(𝐹(2,68) = 3.34, MMNaftercontrollingfortimesinceregularuse(age:partial 𝑝 = .042) (Table3(a)). Univariate ANOVAs were used to 𝑟 = −.10,𝑝 = .72;durationofregularuse:partial𝑟 = .05, decomposethesignificantcondition×groupinteractionand 𝑝 = .85)ortimesincelastuse(age:partial𝑟 = .10,𝑝 = .69; compared ex-users and controls in each deviant condition duration of regular use: partial 𝑟 = .13, 𝜌 = .61). These separately.MMNineachdeviantconditionrevealednoeffect resultsindicatesmallerdurationMMNwithlongerperiods of group for frequency MMN (𝐹(1,34) = 1.99, 𝑝 = .17) ofabstinence(Figure2). or intensity MMN (𝐹 < 1.0), although groups differed at trend level for duration MMN (𝐹(1,34) = 2.97, 𝑝 = .09; 𝑝 = .08 with age included as a covariate (see Supplement 3.4. P50 Sensory Gating in Ex-Users and Controls. Grand 1))withsmallerdurationMMNamplitudesinex-users.Age meanERPwaveformsatCztothefirst(S1)andsecond(S2) of onset of regular use, duration of regular and daily use, clickarepresentedforex-usersandcontrolsinFigure3,and and quantity and frequency of prior cannabis use were not P50amplitudestoS1andS2,P50ratio,anddifferencescores significantly correlated with either frequency or duration inTable3(b).Ex-usersdidnotdifferfromcontrolsintermsof MMNamplitude(all𝑝>.09).However,durationMMNwas P50differencescore(𝑍 = −1.23,𝑝 = .23).Therewasatrend positivelycorrelatedwithabstinence(timesinceregularuse: toward larger P50 ratios in ex-users compared to controls 𝜌=.65,𝑝=.004;timesincelastuse:𝜌=.56,𝑝=.016),with (𝑍=−1.68,𝑝=.097). 6 NeuralPlasticity −2.0 −2.0 V) −3.0 V) −3.0 𝜇 𝜇 N ( −4.0 N ( −4.0 M M M M n −5.0 n −5.0 o o urati −6.0 urati −6.0 D D −7.0 −7.0 0.0 5.0 10.0 15.0 20.0 0.0 5.0 10.0 15.0 20.0 Abstinence since regular use (years) Abstinence since last use (years) (a) (b) Figure2:ScatterplotsshowingassociationbetweendurationMMNamplitude(𝜇V)anddurationofabstinencesincelastregularcannabis use(a)andabstinencesincethelastoccasionofuse(b). Table3:(a)Mean(SD)MMNamplitude(𝜇V)andlatency(ms)at notcorrelatedwithP50metricsaftercontrollingforduration Fzand(b)median[range]P50metrics(𝜇V)atCzforex-cannabis ofregularordailyuse(timesinceregularuse:all𝑝>.3;time usersandhealthynonusercontrols. sincelastuse:all𝑝>.6). Ex-cannabis Controls (𝑛u=ser1s8) (𝑛=18) 4.Discussion (a)MMN GivenmountingevidenceofalteredMMNandP50sensory DurationMMN gatinginchroniccannabisusers,thecurrentstudysetoutto Amplitude(𝜇V) −4.60(1.26) −5.65(2.26) examine the degree to which impairments in early sensory processingmightrecoverwithabstinencefromcannabis,as Latency(ms) 183.9(20.6) 183.8(17.2) afunctionofthedurationofabstinenceandhistoryofprior FrequencyMMN use.Wereporttrendstowardimpairedsensorygating(larger Amplitude(𝜇V) −2.74(1.21) −3.34(1.35) P50 ratios) and reduced duration MMN amplitude in ex- Latency(ms) 167.6(22.2) 173.1(21.7) cannabis users compared to nonuser controls. An associa- IntensityMMN tion between the duration of prior cannabis use and P50 Amplitude(𝜇V) −3.25(1.51) −2.92(1.87) metrics suggests that persistent impairments in P50 gating may be related to prior exposure to cannabis. In contrast, Latency(ms) 153.6(27.6) 142.4(25.9) the association between MMN amplitude and duration of (b)P50 abstinence was not in the expected direction, with smaller P50S1amplitude 2.59[0.36–5.14] 2.80[0.39–9.52] amplitudes associated with longer abstinence. The P50 data P50S2amplitude 1.83[0.03–3.20] 1.27[0.0–3.62] extend the findings of Rentzsch et al. [33] who found that P50ratio 0.53[0.01–4.19] 0.37[0.0–1.41] P50 sensory gating was reduced in former cannabis users P50differencescore 1.66[−1.15–3.12] 1.83[−0.40–5.90] who were abstinent for at least 28 days: the ex-cannabis users of the present study had been abstinent for a median of 3.5 years (range of 6 weeks to 16 years). The observed association between greater P50 impairment and duration In ex-users, no correlation was observed between age ofpriorregularanddailycannabisuseinthisstudy,andin of onset of regular cannabis use, quantity or frequency of Rentzsch et al. [33], and no association with the duration priorcannabisusepermonth,timesincelastregularuseor ofabstinence,highlightsthepossibilitythatthesepersistent time since last use, and either P50 ratio or difference score effectsmayresultfromprolongedexposuretocannabisand (all 𝑝 > .10). However, duration of prior regular use and may not recover with abstinence. The findings concur also durationofpriordailyusewerebothsignificantlycorrelated with previous research in current cannabis users (with 12– with P50 ratio (regular use: 𝜌 = .61, 𝑝 = .007; daily use: 24hoursofabstinencetocontrolforacuteeffects[31,32,34, 𝜌 = .50, 𝑝 = .033, Figure4) and P50 difference score 59]), implicating an association between cannabis exposure (regularuse: 𝜌 = −.60, 𝑝 = .008; daily use: 𝜌 = −.55, 𝑝 = andfunctioningofneuronalgeneratorsinvolvedinsensory .019,Figure4).Thesecorrelationsremainedsignificantafter gating.UnlikeRentzschetal.[25],thecurrentstudydidnot controllingforthedurationofabstinencesinceregularusein find frequency MMN amplitude to be reduced in ex-users partialcorrelations(P50ratio,regularuse:𝑟 = .63,𝑝 = .006; relativetocontrols. dailyuse:𝑟 = .57,𝑝 = .02;P50differencescore,regularuse: There is increasing evidence that regular exposure to 𝑟=−.64,𝑝=.006;dailyuse:𝑟=−.55,𝑝=.02).Further,time cannabisoveranextendedperiodoftimeisassociatedwith sincelastregularuseortimesincelastoccasionofanyusewas reduced frequency MMN [24–26] and more recently, in NeuralPlasticity 7 P50 to first click (S1) P50 to second click (S2) −2 −2 −1 −1 −100 0 100 200 300 −100 0 100 200 300 0 0 V) 1 V) 1 𝜇 𝜇 ( ( 2 2 3 3 4 4 Controls Controls Ex-users Ex-users (a) (b) Figure3:ERPwaveformstothefirst(S1;(a))andsecond(S2;(b))clickatCzforex-cannabisusers(grey)andnonusercontrols(black). Amplitudeisshownin𝜇Vonthe𝑦-axisandtimeinmillisecondsalongthe𝑥-axis. 4.5 4.5 4.5 3.5 3.5 3.5 o 2.5 o 2.5 o 2.5 rati rati rati 50P 1.5 50P 1.5 50P 1.5 0.5 0.5 0.5 −0.5 −0.5 −0.5 0.0 10.0 20.0 30.0 0.0 100.0 200.0 300.0 400.0 0.0 5.0 10.0 15.0 20.0 Duration of regular cannabis use (years) Duration of daily cannabis use (months) Abstinence since regular use (years) (a) P50ratio 4.0 4.0 4.0 3.0 3.0 3.0 ore ore ore nce sc 21..00 nce sc 12..00 nce sc 21..00 differe 0.0 differe 0.0 differe 0.0 0 0 0 5 −1.0 5 −1.0 5 −1.0 P P P −2.0 −2.0 −2.0 0.0 10.0 20.0 30.0 0.0 100.0 200.0 300.0 400.0 0.0 5.0 10.0 15.0 20.0 Duration of regular cannabis use (years) Duration of daily cannabis use (months) Abstinence since regular use (years) (b) P50differencescore Figure4:ScatterplotsshowingassociationbetweenP50metricsanddurationofregularcannabisuseinyears(left),durationofdailycannabis useinmonths(middle),anddurationofabstinencesinceregularuse(right)forex-cannabisusers.P50ratioshownonthetoprow(a)and P50differencescoreshownonthebottomrow(b). long-termusers,attenuateddurationMMNaswell[24].We alterationstobrainfunctionassociatedwithsoundduration havearguedthatreductionindurationMMNmaymanifest processing following more prolonged exposure to cannabis onlyafterprotracted(andespeciallydaily)cannabisuse.We [24].Inthecurrentstudyofformerusers,wedidnotobserve suggested that attenuated frequency MMN in short- and anassociationbetweendurationofregular(ordaily)cannabis long-term users may be related to altered gyrification and useandattenuateddurationMMN,although,unexpectedly, cortical thinning in temporal and frontal regions observed wefoundthatlongerdurationsofabstinence(bothsincelast to be unrelated to the extent of exposure to cannabis in regularuseandsincethelastoccasionofuse)werecorrelated users[60],whiledurationMMNmaybesensitivetobroader with greater reductions in duration MMN amplitude. This 8 NeuralPlasticity relationship is difficult to interpret, as it is not possible use,andforthisreasonitwasnotpossibletosuperviseabsti- in the current sample to definitively dissociate aging from nenceinawayconsistentwithprotocolsemployedinprior length of abstinence (or duration of cannabis exposure). research examining short durations of abstinence (e.g., 28 Nevertheless,partialcorrelationscontrollingforageandfor days)[68].Assuch,thedurationofabstinenceinthissample durationofpriorregularuserevealedthatageandduration wasbroadlydistributed,butmorethanhalfhadabstainedfor of prior regular use did not account for this relationship. theperiodbetween6weeksand5years,whichshouldhave Thisassociationspeakstothreepossiblehypotheses:first,that capturedanyevidenceforeitherabruptorgradualrecovery theassociationbetweendurationMMNandabstinencemay duringtheearlyperiodfollowingcessationofuse,aswellas reflectacceleratedaging,withpersistentandnonlineareffects enablingdetectionofevidenceforanygradualrecoverythat beyondcessationofcannabisuseinformerusers.Second,itis mightrequireasignificantperiodofabstinence.Noevidence possiblethatindividualswhohaveusedcannabisandsubse- for any such patterns was observed; however, this requires quentlychosentoabstaindifferfundamentallyfromnonus- replication in a larger sample of ex-cannabis users. Second, ing individuals on a third unmeasured variable for which despiteourobservationthatalteredP50metricsweresignif- durationofabstinenceisactingasproxy.Neverthelessfurther icantlyassociatedwiththedurationofexposuretocannabis, analysis (reported in Supplement 1) suggests that duration the extent to which these alterations to brain function are of abstinence was not correlated with neuropsychological present prior to cannabis exposure remains unknown and functioning,alcoholorcigaretteuse,psychosis-proneness,or maybeansweredbylongitudinalstudies.Third,weacknowl- priorcannabisuse.Athirdhypothesismaythereforebethat edgeanargumentthatwhileeducationandIQdidnotcorre- cannabis use may have “medicated” a preexisting deficit in latewithourERPmeasuresandwerethereforeinappropriate formerusersandthatthisdeficitisprogressivelyunmasked ascovariatesinouranalyses,thegroupdifferencesonthese with ongoing abstinence. That duration MMN, previously measuresmaypointtodifferencesincognitivefunctionthat demonstrated by us to correlate with longer duration of couldaffectpotentialrecoveryorevenpredatecannabisuse. cannabis use (particularly daily use) [24], suggests that any This cannot be easily ruled out. Nevertheless, ex-cannabis potentialpreexistingdeficitwasneverthelessexacerbatedby usersinthecurrentstudydidnotdifferfromnonusercontrols long-term cannabis use, perhaps interacting with the aging in terms of verbal learning and memory performance, and process.Currentlyspeculative,furtherresearchisrequiredto there were very few differences between groups in terms (dis)confirmthesehypotheses. of psychological wellbeing, including psychosis-proneness. Theprecisemechanismsbywhichprolongedexposureto Future research should examine the functional significance cannabismightaltertheneuronalsubstrateswhichunderpin of these apparently persistent alterations in sensory gating P50 indices of sensory gating remain unclear [61]. The inlong-termformercannabisusersandtheextenttowhich generatorsoftheP50evokedpotentialhavebeenlocalisedto theymayimpactuponfunctioningindailylifeand/orpredate Heschl’sgyrusintheprimaryauditorycortex[62–64].Less cannabis use affecting recovery of function. We acknowl- clear are the neurobiological substrates involved in sensory edge that the sample size of the ex-user group was small. gating[63].Currentlyitisthoughtthatinhibitoryinputsfrom Nevertheless, the sample represents a unique population of theCA3regionwithinthehippocampus,whilstnotdirectly clean cannabis users (unconfounded by other drug use or related to P50 generation [65], may act to suppress activity psychopathology) with long histories of prior cannabis use in the primary auditory cortex linked with P50 generation and with prolonged abstinence, a sample that is difficult to to the second click [63]. Potential generators in the frontal recruitandisrarelyreportedintheliterature. lobe [63] and superior temporal gyrus [66] have also been implicated, in addition to several neurotransmitter systems 5.Conclusions includingdopaminergic,serotonergic,andglutamatergicsys- tems[67].Apossibility,therefore,isthatTHCimpactsupon Insummary,thetrendlevelpatternofresultsofthecurrent inhibitoryinputsfromthehippocampusviadenselylocated study suggests that cannabis-related alterations to MMN CB1 receptors alter an individual’s sensory gating capacity. and P50 may not fully recover following cessation of long- Furthermore,aswehavesuggestedpreviously[31],theremay termcannabisuse.Instead,thecurrentdatasuggestthat(i) be a threshold of exposure associated with the duration of prolongedexposuretocannabisaltersP50sensorygatingand regular or daily cannabis use over which impairments in thesealterationsmaypersistbeyondcessationofuseand(ii) sensorygatingariseandthismayaffectthedegreetowhich MMN amplitude may reduce further with increasing absti- these deficits persist after cessation of use and similarly the nence.Theresultsraisethepossibilityofpersistentalterations degree to which they recover. Future research might take a totheregulatoryroleoftheendocannabinoidsystemonbrain longitudinalapproachtofurtherexamineP50metricsbefore function,affectingthebrain’sabilitytoregulateitsresponse andaftercessationofcannabisuse,toassesspotentialchanges toincomingstimuliortofilterirrelevantinformation.These overtimewithinthesameindividuals. resultsrequirereplicationinfuturestudiesofbrainfunctional There are a number of limitations in the current study plasticity-relatedrecoveryinabstinentcannabisusers. which may be addressed by future work. First, as a cross- sectionalandnaturalisticstudywereliedonparticipantself- ConflictofInterests reportforthedurationofabstinenceandhistoryofcannabis use.ThecurrentstudysetouttoexamineMMNandP50met- Allauthorsreportnobiomedicalfinancialinterestsorpoten- ricsasafunctionofabroaddurationofabstinenceandprior tialconflictofinterests. NeuralPlasticity 9 Acknowledgments [12] D.C.D’Souza,R.A.Sewell,andM.Ranganathan,“Cannabis and psychosis/schizophrenia: human studies,” European This study was funded by the National Health and Medical ArchivesofPsychiatryandClinicalNeuroscience,vol.259,no.7, Research Council (Project Grant 1007593) and the Aus- pp.413–431,2009. tralian Research Council (FT110100752). Preliminary data [13] C.Henquet,M.diForti,P.D.Morrison,R.Kuepper,andR.M. and results from this study were presented at the following Murray, “Gene-environment interplay between cannabis and conferences:2ndNationalCannabisConference,2012,Bris- psychosis,”SchizophreniaBulletin,vol.34,no.6,pp.1111–1121, bane, Australia;BiologicalPsychiatry AustraliaConference, 2008. 2012, Melbourne, Australia; Australasian Psychophysiology [14] R. M. Murray, P. D. Morrison, C. Henquet, and M. D. Forti, Society Conference, 2013, Wollongong, Australia; and they “Cannabis, the mind and society: the hash realities,” Nature werepublishedinthefollowingabstract:Websteretal.[69]. ReviewsNeuroscience,vol.8,no.11,pp.885–895,2007. [15] M. I. Garrido, J. M. Kilner, K. E. Stephan, and K. J. Friston, “Themismatchnegativity:areviewofunderlyingmechanisms,” References ClinicalNeurophysiology,vol.120,no.3,pp.453–463,2009. [1] L. Degenhardt and W. Hall, “Extent of illicit drug use and [16] J. Todd, L. Harms, P. T. Michie, and U. Schall, “Mismatch dependence, and their contribution to the global burden of negativity:translatingthepotential,”FrontiersinPsychiatry,vol. disease,”TheLancet,vol.379,no.9810,pp.55–70,2012. 4,article171,2013. [2] C. Henquet, L. Krabbendam, J. Spauwen et al., “Prospective [17] R. Na¨a¨ta¨nen, “The mismatch negativity: a powerful tool for cohortstudyofcannabisuse,predispositionforpsychosis,and cognitiveneuroscience,”EarandHearing,vol.16,no.1,pp.6–18, psychoticsymptomsinyoungpeople,”BritishMedicalJournal, 1995. vol.330,no.7481,pp.11–14,2005. [18] I. Winkler, “Interpreting the mismatch negativity,” Journal of [3] R.Kuepper,J.vanOs,R.Lieb,H.-U.Wittchen,M.Ho¨fler,and Psychophysiology,vol.21,no.3-4,pp.147–163,2007. C.Henquet,“Continuedcannabisuseandriskofincidenceand [19] R.Na¨a¨ta¨nenandS.Ka¨hko¨nen,“Centralauditorydysfunctionin persistence of psychotic symptoms: 10 year follow-up cohort schizophreniaasrevealedbythemismatchnegativity(MMN) study,”TheBritishMedicalJournal,vol.342,articled738,2011. and its magnetic equivalent MMNm: a review,” International [4] S. Zammit, P. Allebeck, S. Andreasson, I. Lundberg, and JournalofNeuropsychopharmacology,vol.12,no.1,pp.125–135, G. Lewis, “Self reported cannabis use as a risk factor for 2009. schizophreniainSwedishconscriptsof1969:historicalcohort [20] D. Umbricht and S. Krljesb, “Mismatch negativity in schizo- study,”BritishMedicalJournal,vol.325,no.7374,pp.1199–1201, phrenia:ameta-analysis,”SchizophreniaResearch,vol.76,no. 2002. 1,pp.1–23,2005. [5] H. D. Burns, K. Van Laere, S. Sanabria-Boho´rquez et al., [21] F.Jessen,T.Fries,C.Kucharskietal.,“Amplitudereductionof 18 “[ F]MK-9470,apositronemissiontomography(PET)tracer themismatchnegativityinfirst-degreerelativesofpatientswith for in vivo human PET brain imaging of the cannabinoid-1 schizophrenia,”NeuroscienceLetters,vol.309,no.3,pp.185–188, receptor,”ProceedingsoftheNationalAcademyofSciencesofthe 2001. UnitedStatesofAmerica,vol.104,no.23,pp.9800–9805,2007. [22] P. T. Michie, H. Innes-Brown, J. Todd, and A. V. Jablensky, [6] N.SolowijandP.T.Michie,“Cannabisandcognitivedysfunc- “Durationmismatchnegativityinbiologicalrelativesofpatients tion:parallelswithendophenotypesofschizophrenia?”Journal withschizophreniaspectrumdisorders,”BiologicalPsychiatry, ofPsychiatry&Neuroscience,vol.32,no.1,pp.30–52,2007. vol.52,no.7,pp.749–758,2002. [7] R. E. Hampson, F. Miller, G. Palchik, and S. A. Deadwyler, [23] A. E. S¸evik, A. E. A. Yagcioglu, S. Yagcioglu, S. Karahan, N. “Cannabinoid receptor activation modifies NMDA receptor Gu¨rses,andM.Yildiz,“Neuropsychologicalperformanceand mediated release of intracellular calcium: implications for auditoryeventrelatedpotentialsinschizophreniapatientsand endocannabinoid control of hippocampal neural plasticity,” theirsiblings:afamilystudy,”SchizophreniaResearch,vol.130, Neuropharmacology,vol.60,no.6,pp.944–952,2011. no.1–3,pp.195–202,2011. [8] B.D.HeifetsandP.E.Castillo,“Endocannabinoidsignalingand [24] L.-M.Greenwood,S.J.Broyd,R.Croftetal.,“Chroniceffects long-termsynapticplasticity,”AnnualReviewofPhysiology,vol. ofcannabisuseontheauditorymismatchnegativity,”Biological 71,pp.283–306,2009. Psychiatry,vol.75,no.6,pp.449–458,2014. [9] A. F. Hoffman, M. Oz, R. Yang, A. H. Lichtman, and C. R. [25] J. Rentzsch, E. Buntebart, A. Stadelmeier, J. Gallinat, and M. Lupica,“OpposingactionsofchronicΔ9-tetrahydrocannabinol C. Jockers-Scheru¨bl, “Differential effects of chronic cannabis andcannabinoidantagonistsonhippocampallong-termpoten- use on preattentional cognitive functioning in abstinent tiation,”LearningandMemory,vol.14,no.1,pp.63–74,2007. schizophrenic patients and healthy subjects,” Schizophrenia [10] E. Puighermanal, A. Busquets-Garcia, R. Maldonado, and A. Research,vol.130,no.1–3,pp.222–227,2011. Ozaita,“Cellularandintracellularmechanismsinvolvedinthe [26] P. Roser, B. Della, C. Norra, I. Uhl, M. Bru¨ne, and G. Juckel, cognitiveimpairmentofcannabinoids,”PhilosophicalTransac- “Auditory mismatch negativity deficits in long-term heavy tionsoftheRoyalSocietyofLondonSeriesB,BiologicalSciences, cannabis users,” European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical vol.367,no.1607,pp.3254–3263,2012. Neuroscience,vol.260,no.6,pp.491–498,2010. [11] N.Fan,H.Yang,J.Zhang,andC.Chen,“Reducedexpressionof [27] D. Impey, N. El-Marj, A. Parks, J. Choueiry, D. Fisher, and glutamatereceptorsandphosphorylationofCREBarerespon- V. J. Knott, “Mismatch negativity in tobacco-na¨ıve cannabis sible for in vivo Δ9-THC exposure-impaired hippocampal users and its alteration with acute nicotine administration,” synapticplasticity,”JournalofNeurochemistry,vol.112,no.3,pp. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, vol. 136, pp. 73–81, 691–702,2010. 2015. 10 NeuralPlasticity [28] N. Pesa, D. F. Hermens, R. A. Battisti, M. Kaur, I. B. Hickie, [44] D. A. Eldreth, J. A. Matochik, J. L. Cadet, and K. I. Bolla, and N. Solowij, “Delayed preattentional functioning in early “Abnormalbrainactivityinprefrontalbrainregionsinabstinent psychosispatientswithcannabisuse,”Psychopharmacology,vol. marijuanausers,”NeuroImage,vol.23,no.3,pp.914–920,2004. 222,no.3,pp.507–518,2012. [45] K.I.Bolla,K.Brown,D.Eldreth,K.Tate,andJ.L.Cadet,“Dose- [29] E.Bramon,R.J.Croft,C.McDonaldetal.,“Mismatchnegativity relatedneurocognitiveeffectsofmarijuanause,”Neurology,vol. inschizophrenia:afamilystudy,”SchizophreniaResearch,vol. 59,no.9,pp.1337–1343,2002. 67,no.1,pp.1–10,2004. [46] K.I.Bolla,D.A.Eldreth,J.A.Matochik,andJ.L.Cadet,“Neural [30] O. M. de Wilde, L. J. Bour, P. M. Dingemans, J. H. T. substrates of faulty decision-making in abstinent marijuana M. Koelman, and D. H. Linszen, “A meta-analysis of P50 users,”NeuroImage,vol.26,no.2,pp.480–492,2005. studies in patients with schizophrenia and relatives: differ- [47] N.Solowij,“Docognitiveimpairmentsrecoverfollowingcessa- encesinmethodologybetweenresearchgroups,”Schizophrenia tionofcannabisuse?”LifeSciences,vol.56,no.23-24,pp.2119– Research,vol.97,no.1–3,pp.137–151,2007. 2126,1995. [31] S. J. Broyd, L.-M. Greenwood, R. J. Croft et al., “Chronic [48] L. Sobell and M. Sobell, Timeline Followback User’s Guide: A effectsofcannabisonsensorygating,”InternationalJournalof CalendarMethodforAssessingAlcoholandDrugUse,Addiction Psychophysiology,vol.89,no.3,pp.381–389,2013. ResearchFoundation,Toronto,Canada,1996. [32] C.R.Edwards,P.D.Skosnik,A.B.Steinmetz,B.F.O’Donnell, [49] D.Wechsler,WechslerAbbreviatedScaleofIntelligence(Wasi), and W. P. Hetrick, “Sensory gating impairments in heavy HarcourtAssessement,SanAntonio,Tex,USA,1999. cannabisusersareassociatedwithalteredneuraloscillations,” [50] N.C.Stefanis,M.Hanssen,N.K.Smirnisetal.,“Evidencethat BehavioralNeuroscience,vol.123,no.4,pp.894–904,2009. threedimensionsofpsychosishaveadistributioninthegeneral [33] J. Rentzsch, A. Penzhorn, K. Kernbichler et al., “Differen- population,”PsychologicalMedicine,vol.32,no.2,pp.347–358, tial impact of heavy cannabis use on sensory gating in 2002. schizophrenicpatientsandotherwisehealthycontrols,”Experi- [51] A.Raine,“TheSPQ:ascalefortheassessmentofschizotypal mentalNeurology,vol.205,no.1,pp.241–249,2007. personality based on DSM-III-R criteria,” Schizophrenia Bul- [34] G.PatrickandF.A.Struve,“ReductionofauditoryP50gating letin,vol.17,no.4,pp.555–564,1991. responseinmarihuanausers:furthersupportingdata,”Clinical [52] A.T.Beck,C.H.Ward,M.Mendelson,J.Mock,andJ.Erbaugh, EEG(Electroencephalography),vol.31,no.2,pp.88–93,2000. “An inventory for measuring depression,” Archives of General [35] D. L. Braff and M. A. Geyer, “Sensorimotor gating and Psychiatry,vol.4,pp.561–571,1961. schizophrenia.Humanandanimalmodelstudies,”Archivesof GeneralPsychiatry,vol.47,no.2,pp.181–188,1990. [53] C.D.Spielberger,R.L.Gorsuch,R.Lushene,P.R.Vagg,and G. A. Jacobs, Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, [36] N. N. Boutros and A. Belger, “Midlatency evoked potentials ConsultingPsychologistsPress,PaloAlto,Calif,USA,1983. attenuationandaugmentationreflectdifferentaspectsofsen- sory gating,” Biological Psychiatry, vol. 45, no. 7, pp. 917–922, [54] R.C.Oldfield,“Theassessmentandanalysisofhandedness:the 1999. Edinburghinventory,”Neuropsychologia,vol.9,no.1,pp.97–113, 1971. [37] K.Gjini,C.Arfken,andN.N.Boutros,“Relationshipsbetween sensory ‘gating out’ and sensory ‘gating in’ of auditory [55] J.B.Saunders,O.G.Aasland,T.F.Babor,J.R.DelaFuente, evokedpotentialsinschizophrenia:apilotstudy,”Schizophrenia and M. Grant, “Development of the alcohol use disorders Research,vol.121,no.1–3,pp.139–145,2010. identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on earlydetectionofpersonswithharmfulalcoholconsumption [38] L.Hu,N.N.Boutros,andB.H.Jansen,“Sensorygating-outand II,”Addiction,vol.88,no.6,pp.791–804,1993. gating-in in normal and schizophrenic participants,” Clinical EEGandNeuroscience,vol.43,no.1,pp.23–31,2012. [56] A. J. Budney, J. R. Hughes, B. A. Moore, and R. Vandrey, “Reviewofthevalidityandsignificanceofcannabiswithdrawal [39] M. H. Meier, A. Caspi, A. Ambler et al., “Persistent cannabis syndrome,”AmericanJournalofPsychiatry,vol.161,no.11,pp. users show neuropsychological decline from childhood to 1967–1977,2004. midlife,”ProceedingsoftheNationalAcademyofSciencesofthe UnitedStatesofAmerica,vol.109,no.40,pp.E2657–E2664,2012. [57] M. Gossop, J. Marsden, and D. Stewart, “Dual dependence: assessmentofdependenceuponalcoholandillicitdrugs,and [40] S. F. Tapert, A. D. Schweinsburg, S. P. A. Drummond et therelationshipofalcoholdependenceamongdrugmisusers al., “Functional MRI of inhibitory processing in abstinent topatternsofdrinking,illicitdruguseandhealthproblems,” adolescentmarijuanausers,”Psychopharmacology,vol.194,no. Addiction,vol.97,no.2,pp.169–178,2002. 2,pp.173–183,2007. [58] E.J.Barkus,J.Stirling,R.S.Hopkins,andS.Lewis,“Cannabis- [41] J. T. Sneider, H. G. Pope Jr., M. M. Silveri, N. S. Simpson, S. induced psychosis-like experiences are associated with high A.Gruber,andD.A.Yurgelun-Todd,“Differencesinregional schizotypy,”Psychopathology,vol.39,no.4,pp.175–178,2006. bloodvolumeduringa28-dayperiodofabstinenceinchronic cannabis smokers,” European Neuropsychopharmacology, vol. [59] G.PatrickandF.Struve,“ReducedP50sensorygatinginheavy 18,no.8,pp.612–619,2008. marihuanausers,”InternationalJournalofPsychophysiology,vol. [42] S. S. Pillay, J. Rogowska, G. Kanayama et al., “Cannabis and 45,pp.33–34,2002. motorfunction:fMRIchangesfollowing28daysofdiscontin- [60] I.Mata,R.Perez-Iglesias,R.Roiz-Santian˜ezetal.,“Gyrification uation,”ExperimentalandClinicalPsychopharmacology,vol.16, brain abnormalities associated with adolescence and early- no.1,pp.22–32,2008. adulthoodcannabisuse,”BrainResearch,vol.1317,pp.297–304, [43] L.Chang,R.Yakupov,C.Cloak,andT.Ernst,“Marijuanause 2010. isassociatedwithareorganizedvisual-attentionnetworkand [61] D.W.N.Dissanayake,R.Mason,andC.A.Marsden,“Sensory cerebellarhypoactivation,”Brain,vol.129,no.5,pp.1096–1112, gating,cannabinoidsandschizophrenia,”Neuropharmacology, 2006. vol.67,pp.66–77,2013.

Description:
2Illawarra Health and Medical Research Institute, University of Wollongong, Wollongong, NSW 2522, P50 gating correlated with prior duration of cannabis use. [37] K. Gjini, C. Arfken, and N. N. Boutros, “Relationships between.
See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.