Repositorio Institucional de la Universidad Autónoma de Madrid https://repositorio.uam.es Esta es la versión de autor del artículo publicado en: This is an author produced version of a paper published in: American Journal of Physical Anthropology 165.4 (2018): 834-854 DOI: http://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.23357 Copyright: © 2018 Wiley Periodicals El acceso a la versión del editor puede requerir la suscripción del recurso Access to the published version may require subscription Page 1 of 61 American Journal of Physical Anthropology 1 2 3 Human life course biology: A Centennial Perspective of scholarship on the human 4 5 pattern of growth and capacity for culture. 6 7 8 Barry Bogin1, Carlos Varea2, Michael Hermanussen3, Christiane Scheffler4 9 10 11 1 School of Sport, Exercise & Health Sciences, Loughborough University LE11 3TU 12 13 UK [email protected] TEL: +44 (0)1509 228819 14 15 16 2 Physical Anthropology Group, Department of Biology, Universidad Autónoma de 17 18 Madrid, Madrid, Spain [email protected] 19 20 21 3 Aschauhof 3, 24340 Altenhof, Germany, [email protected] 22 23 24 4 Universität Potsdam, Institut für Biochemie und Biologie, Maulbeerallee 1, 14469 25 26 27 Potsdam [email protected] 28 29 30 Corresponding author: Barry Bogin 31 32 33 Number of text pages – 32 34 35 Figures – 4 36 37 Tables – 4 38 39 Key Words: Life History, Childhood, Adolescence, Menopause, Senescence 40 41 42 43 44 Word count, main text: 9,629 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 l 1 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. American Journal of Physical Anthropology Page 2 of 61 1 2 3 The great lesson that comes from thinking of organisms as life cycles is that it is the 4 5 life cycle, not just the adult, that evolves (Bonner, 1993, p. 93). 6 7 8 We structure our Centennial Perspective on human life course biology in the 9 10 following manner: 1) the discovery of the interplay between evolutionary processes 11 12 with physical growth and development; 2) the recognition of novel features of human 13 14 15 growth and development, and several ways these may be organized into a 16 17 continuum of ontogenetic events; 3) evidence that the human life course biology 18 19 establishes the foundation for the capacity for human culture and biocultural 20 21 reproduction; 4) the interactive nature of human life course biology with the social, 22 23 economic, and political environment. Our essay is not an exhaustive review, rather it 24 25 26 highlights critical research and scholarship, emphasizes articles published in the 27 28 AJPA and the Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, and mentions research by the 29 30 authors where appropriate (‘all is vanity’ Ecclesiastes 1:2). 31 32 33 Part 1: The interplay between evolutionary processes with physical growth and 34 35 development 36 37 38 Evolution and growth 39 40 Ever since Darwin (Gould, 1977) we know natural selection is one of the mechanisms 41 42 shaping biology. Ever since Dobzhansky we know that, “Nothing in biology makes 43 44 sense except in the light of evolution” (Dobzhansky, 1973). The pattern of human 45 46 growth makes sense when illuminated by evolution, especially the twin engines of 47 48 49 natural selection – differential fertility and mortality. Other mechanisms of evolution, 50 51 mutation, migration, genetic drift, epigenetic assimilation (Waddington, 1957; 52 53 Hallgrimsson et al., 2002; Fuentes, 2010; Bogin, 2013), and sexual selection (Gray, 54 55 2013), also play their roles. Today we study human growth in the context of life 56 57 58 history theory -- the study of the evolution and function of life stages and of behaviors 59 60 l 2 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Page 3 of 61 American Journal of Physical Anthropology 1 2 3 related to these stages (Bogin and Smith, 1996, 2012; Kaplan et al., 2000; Varea and 4 5 Bernis, 2013). The life history of a species may be defined as, “Ithe evolutionary 6 7 adaptations used to allocate limited resources and energy toward growth, 8 9 maintenance, reproduction, raising offspring to independence, and avoiding death” 10 11 12 (Bogin, 1999, p.154). Life history patterns of species are often a series of trade-offs 13 14 between growth versus reproduction, quantity versus quality of offspring, death 15 16 sooner or later after reproduction and other biological possibilities given the limited 17 18 time and resources available to all living things (Stearns, 1992; Charnov and 19 20 Berrigan, 1993). 21 22 23 Humans share some life history characteristics with other relatively large and long- 24 25 living mammals. Other such species, weighing more than 30 kg and known to have 26 27 lived 50 or more years in the wild or captivity, are elephants, whales, Baikal and 28 29 Caspian seals, dugongs, orangutans, gorillas, and chimpanzees, bonobos and the 30 31 32 light weight exception (<4 kg), the white-throated capuchin monkey 33 34 (http://www.earthlife.net/mammals/age.html). Notable characteristics of species with 35 36 longevity potentials of 50+ years is that they live in social groups, most are known to 37 38 use tools (not reported for seals, bats or dugongs), and have relatively large brains. 39 40 They also have slow life histories, taking a relatively long time (years vs. months) to 41 42 43 grow from birth to reproductive maturity. These features and life history traits are the 44 45 result of biological selection for age-related and sex-specific trade-offs in adaptations 46 47 to habitats and ecological niches (see Table 1 for a list of life history biology traits and 48 49 trade-offs). 50 51 52 Table 1 about here 53 54 Humans have one additional adaptive trait - culture. We define human culture as 55 56 having three equally important components: 1) technology, 2) sociology, 3) ideology, 57 58 a trilogy of terms from Leslie White (1949), but with the needed modifications in 59 60 l 3 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. American Journal of Physical Anthropology Page 4 of 61 1 2 3 meaning and biological rigor as provided by Henrich and Henrich (2007), Boyd and 4 5 Richerson (2009), and other contemporary theorists of culture. Medium- and long- 6 7 lived mammalian species may have elements of technology (tool use, tool 8 9 manufacture), sociology (live in social groups, cooperate in feeding or offspring care), 10 11 12 or both. Only the human species, so far as we are aware, has ideology, which 13 14 includes symbolic language, religion, marriage and formal kinship systems, with kin 15 16 terminology and its associated behavioral obligations. We do not debate here when 17 18 and where human culture appeared in the 6-7 million years of evolutionary history 19 20 since hominins diverged from other primate lineages. Rather, our purpose is to 21 22 23 demonstrate a few of the ways in which the human capacity for culture is inextricably 24 25 linked with the human species-specific pattern of life course biology. 26 27 The inseparable biocultural nature of the human species is its primary evolutionary 28 29 engine. Dobzhansky, once again, stated this most clearly, “Iit is precisely because 30 31 32 we know that [humankind] changes so greatly culturally that we can be so confident 33 34 that it changes to some extent also geneticallyI The potentialities for rapid evolution 35 36 of the human species have not been depleted, since the environment continues to 37 38 change and the genetic variance remains plentiful. [Humankind] assuredly continues 39 40 to evolve, both culturally and biologically (Dobzhansky, 1963, p. 147, ‘humankind’ 41 42 43 substituted for the original ‘mankind’). 44 45 46 47 Growth and evolution 48 49 The study of biological growth in relation to evolution has a venerable history. D’arcy 50 51 52 Thompson (1860-1948) used mathematics and principles of mechanics to show in a 53 54 formal and scientific manner the physical and geometrical constraints on 55 56 developmental biology (Thompson, 1917). Thompson took issue with natural 57 58 selection as the only force of evolution and as the primary ’lathe of evolution’, that is, 59 60 l 4 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Page 5 of 61 American Journal of Physical Anthropology 1 2 3 the process that shapes biological form to any functional adaptation. Instead, some 4 5 biological forms derive from simple mechanical rules. The hexagonal shape of 6 7 honeycomb chambers of the bee hive, for example, is the shape that minimizes 8 9 surface area to volume ratio. Charles Darwin (1809-1882) thought that the 10 11 12 honeycomb is the product of natural selection operating on the genetic basis of the 13 14 bees’ behavior, but Thompson showed that the shape results simply from the 15 16 process of individual bees putting globe-shaped cells together. The hexagon shape 17 18 results from the minimization of the forces of compression and strain on each cell 19 20 (Hales, 1999). There is no selection on the bee’s genetic basis of behaviour for 21 22 23 hexagon construction; highly similar boundary shapes are created in a field of soap 24 25 bubbles. Thompson further supported his mechanical explanation by noting that 26 27 queen honey cells, which are constructed singly, are irregular in shape with no 28 29 evidence of efficiency. 30 31 32 A human example of biological development which requires no natural selection is 33 34 the growth in size and pattern of gyrification (folding) of the human fetal brain. The 35 36 human brain is relatively large for total body mass and is already so at birth (Varea 37 38 and Bernis, 2013; Bogin and Varea, 2017). The surface of the fetal brain is initially 39 40 smooth and then folds as it grows. This is mechanically efficient as a folded structure 41 42 43 can occupy a smaller space than if that same structure were flat or smooth. Much 44 45 research focuses on the genetic and molecular determinants of growth in size and 46 47 the pattern of gyrification of the human brain. A recent article, however, shows that 48 49 pattern of gyri formation of the human fetal brain, “Iis an inevitable mechanical 50 51 52 consequence of constrained cortical expansionI” (Tallinen et al., 2016, p. 591). The 53 54 authors built a layered 3D-printed gel mimic of a pre-folded human fetal brain based 55 56 on magnetic resonance images of real brains. The mimic was immersed in a solvent 57 58 59 60 l 5 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. American Journal of Physical Anthropology Page 6 of 61 1 2 3 that caused outer layer to swell relative to the inner layer, replicating the actual 4 5 process of cortical brain growth. The relative expansion of the ‘cortical’ layer induced 6 7 mechanical compression and led to the formation of sulci and gyri like those in actual 8 9 fetal brains. This process was repeated via numerical simulations and the results 10 11 12 were essentially identical. The authors do not rule out a role for natural selection to 13 14 influence the functional coordination of brain regions that are in close physical 15 16 proximity, but they conclude that, “Ithe size, shape, placement and orientation of the 17 18 folds arise through iterations and variations of an elementary mechanical instability 19 20 modulated by early fetal brain geometry” (p. 588) – in other words, this is the way that 21 22 23 brain tissue bends when squashed against the inside of the skull. In his book On 24 25 Growth and Form, Thompson provided similar examples of purely mechanical folding 26 27 in biology and in pastry cooking (pellets of dough, 1917, p. 84). 28 29 Thompson also demonstrated that differences in body shape and size between 30 31 32 adults of various closely related species may be due to differences in growth rates 33 34 from an initially highly similar embryonic or newborn form. For this he used the 35 36 system of Cartesian transformational grids. Thompson included examples of primate 37 38 growth and perhaps the most well-known is his transformational grid illustration of 39 40 age changes in the chimpanzee and human skull from between birth and adulthood 41 42 43 (Figure 1). 44 45 FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE 46 47 In these cases, Thompson visualized the force of natural selection to bring about new 48 49 patterns of growth and development. Per Figure 1, selection was for large canine 50 51 52 teeth for the chimpanzee versus selection for a larger brain for the human. Thompson 53 54 anticipated the powerful impact of data visualization, which became more readily 55 56 available to science with the advent of digital computers. The publication of On 57 58 Growth and Form celebrated its own centennial in 2017 59 60 l 6 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Page 7 of 61 American Journal of Physical Anthropology 1 2 3 (https://www.ongrowthandform.org/news/). The book remains in print and continues 4 5 to inspire biologists, mathematicians and philosophers. 6 7 One inspired scholar is John Tyler Bonner (1920- ), whose 1965 book Size and Cycle 8 9 was an homage to Thompson’s early 20th century work on growth and form. The 10 11 12 book also synthesized the evolution of growth in size and shape with life cycle 13 14 biology. Bonner’s essential observation was that, “Evolution [is] the alteration of life 15 16 cycles through timeI” (Bonner, 1965, p. 3). Biologists may define species by the 17 18 adult (reproductive) morphology, such as the anatomy of an adult dog, chimpanzee, 19 20 or oak tree. But, each species is distinct from the moment of fertilization, through its 21 22 23 life cycle and even after death as a carcass of fallen tree. The ‘dog’, ‘chimpanzee’, or 24 25 ‘oak tree’ are, in fact, the entire life cycle of the organism. Bonner understood that the 26 27 life cycle of each species is part of its adaption to the physical and biological 28 29 environment, or more accurately the ecological niche, which is composed of nonliving 30 31 32 objects as well as other life cycles. The life cycle of the human species is both 33 34 adapted to the human niche and, due to our biocultural nature, allows people to 35 36 modify the niche to enhance adaptiveness, measured in fertility, longevity, material or 37 38 social complexity, and ideological productivity. 39 40 The human life cycle is derived from mammalian, especially primate, ancestors. This 41 42 43 ancestry places some Thompson-like constraints on how much natural selection can 44 45 modify ancient patterns of growth and development. The transformational grids of 46 47 Figure 1 are an example of constraint under selection, that is, retention of nearly 48 49 identical skeletal-dental components in adulthood, despite change in size and shape, 50 51 52 due to the shared ancestry of chimpanzees and humans. 53 54 Primate growth and evolution 55 56 Biological and evolutionary comparisons of humans with non-human primates are 57 58 known from the 19th century, but the systematic study of primate growth and 59 60 l 7 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. American Journal of Physical Anthropology Page 8 of 61 1 2 3 development in relation to human evolution began with the work of Adolph Schultz 4 5 (1891-1976). Schultz (1924) published in the AJPA the article ‘Growth studies in 6 7 primates bearing upon man’s evolution.’ A year earlier, Schultz published in the AJPA 8 9 a detailed analysis of human fetal growth (Schultz, 1923) and two year later 10 11 12 expanded this topic to include non-human primates (Schultz, 1926). These articles 13 14 are primarily a descriptive mix of Schultz’s quantitative and qualitative assessments 15 16 of primate ontogeny, based on careful measurement and dissection of cadavers of 17 18 fetuses, neonates, immatures, and adults. Schultz does not cite On Growth and Form 19 20 in these articles, but he summarizes his analysis with the Thompsonian statement 21 22 23 that, “Ithere will remain the forcible conclusion that the many striking resemblances 24 25 between man, ape, and, monkey in early development, and their frequently closely 26 27 corresponding growth changes can only be explained by one common origin, from 28 29 which they all inherited the tendency for the same ontogenetic processesI” (p. 163). 30 31 32 In later publications in the AJPA and elsewhere Schultz (Schultz,1935, 1960, 1969) 33 34 pioneered the analysis of dental maturation and tooth eruption timing as life history 35 36 markers. Before Schultz, there are publications by Wilton M Krogman (1903-1987) on 37 38 the eruption of teeth in Old World monkeys and apes (Krogman, 1930). He reported 39 40 that the first permanent molar (M1) is always the first of the permanent teeth to erupt 41 42 43 in all the primates studied. Krogman also discovered that humans take about three 44 45 times longer than non-human primates to progress from M1 to M3 eruption. 46 47 Anecdotal evidence had led Arthur Keith (1866-1955) and Solly Zuckerman (1904- 48 49 1993) to incorrectly report that apes and humans were nearly identical in the timing of 50 51 52 dental eruption. Finally, Krogman also seems to be the first anthropologist to report 53 54 that the correlation between, “Iepiphyseal union with tooth eruption indicates that 55 56 the growth process in the Anthropoids, while similar in pattern to that of Man, is 57 58 completed in shorter time” (1930, p. 312). Krogman’s discovery of: 1) the primacy of 59 60 l 8 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Page 9 of 61 American Journal of Physical Anthropology 1 2 3 M1 eruption in all primates, 2) the significant human delay in molar eruption 4 5 sequence, and 3) the overall delay in skeletal maturation of humans were major 6 7 findings that became the basis of all life history research with living and extinct 8 9 primate species (Smith, 1991). 10 11 12 By 1924, in the AJPA, Schultz was using the words embryonic, fetus, newborn, 13 14 infant, juvenile, and adult as names for distinct stages or phases of primate growth 15 16 and development. He used the words ‘child’ and ‘children’ to denote human pre- 17 18 adults of any age. The word ‘adolescence’ is mentioned one time in 1926 (Schultz, 19 20 1926). Schultz does not define clearly any of these stages of development, rather the 21 22 23 words are used as if the reader understands the meaning. He does state (1935) that 24 25 eruption of the first permanent teeth, most often the M1, indicates the end of the 26 27 infantile period and, presumably, the start of the juvenile period. 28 29 The use of these names for developmental stages became more formally associated 30 31 32 with the timing of permanent molar eruption when Schultz published his well-known 33 34 illustration of comparative primate life history (Schultz, 1960). In this figure, Schultz 35 36 defines the Infantile Period from birth, “Ito the first permanent teeth”, the Juvenile 37 38 Period, “Ito last permanent teeth”, and the Adult Period, “Ito end of mean 39 40 longevity”. More detailed technical definitions of mammalian life course stages were 41 42 43 published in the 1960s and we discuss these later in this article. Schultz’s 1960 figure 44 45 is widely copied (Lovejoy, 1981; Smith, 1992; Bogin, 1999; Leigh, 2001). The original 46 47 included a speculative column on the life periods of ‘Early Man’ and is so doing firmly 48 49 established the use of molar eruption sequence in the study of the evolution of 50 51 52 human life course biology. 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 l 9 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Description: