ebook img

Public Communication and Behavior. Volume 2 PDF

370 Pages·1989·6.85 MB·English
Save to my drive
Quick download
Download
Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.

Preview Public Communication and Behavior. Volume 2

Public Communication and Behavior VOLUME 2 Edited by GEORGE COMSTOCK School of Public Communications Syracuse University Syracuse, New York ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Publishers San Diego New York Berkeley Boston London Sydney Tokyo Toronto COPYRIGHT © 1989 BY ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. NO PART OF THIS PUBLICATION MAY BE REPRODUCED OR TRANSMITTED IN ANY FORM OR BY ANY MEANS, ELECTRONIC OR MECHANICAL, INCLUDING PHOTOCOPY, RECORDING, OR ANY INFORMATION STORAGE AND RETRIEVAL SYSTEM, WITHOUT PERMISSION IN WRITING FROM THE PUBLISHER. ACADEMIC PRESS, INC. San Diego, California 92101 United Kingdom Edition published by ACADEMIC PRESS LIMITED 24-28 Oval Road, London NW1 7DX ISBN 0-12-543202-X (alk. paper) ISSN 0887-932X This publication is not a periodical and is not subject to copying under CONTU guidelines. PRINTED IN THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA 89 90 91 92 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Contributors Numbers in parentheses indicate the pages on which the authors' contributions begin. BRANDON S. CENTERWALL (1), Departments of Epidemiology and Psychiatry, University of Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195 DENNIS K. DAVIS (59), Department of Speech Communication, Southern Illinois University, Carbondale, Illinois 62901 ALETHA C. HUSTON (103), Center for Research on the Influence of Television on Children (CRITC), Department of Human Development, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas 66045 NEIL M. MALAMUTH (159), Communications Studies, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, California 90024 W RUSSELL NEUMAN (205), Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts 02139 STUART OSKAMP (291), Faculty in Psychology, Claremont Graduate School, Claremont, California 91711 JOHN P. ROBINSON (59), Survey Research Center, Department of Sociology, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland 20742 JOHN C. WRIGHT (103), Center for Research on the Influence of Television on Children (CRITC), Department of Human Development, University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas 66045 Preface In the first volume of this series, the editor made explicit his debt to Leonard Berkowitz's Advances in Experimental Social Psychology as a model for the present undertaking. That series, begun almost three decades ago, has now reached Volume 22; its contributions to theory and empirical inquiry, by stating what can be said currently about a shifting array of topics, have been an important part of the continuing education of its readers. The present series has the same ambition, but because this is only Volume 2, we offer it as a goal and a hope, with achievement very much yet in the future. As did Berkowitz in his Volume 2 (in 1965), however, it is the proper time to com ment on what the contents so far tell us about the field purportedly covered. That field was defined in Volume 1 as "the study of communicatory behavior that has a public or social character." More concretely, it encom passes research and theory designated as "within a range of disciplines and fields—advertising, child development, education, journalism, political science, sociology, and wherever else such scholarly activity occurs including, of course, social psychology." The contents of the first two volumes attest to the appropriateness of (or at least to the ability of the editor to search out) such a varied representation. So, too, do the contributions underway for inclusion in future volumes. This is a vigorous, lively field, if one that life often seems to outpace in its orderly cataloguing and precise distinctions. It will be interesting to see how much help in this latter respect comes from the forthcoming four-volume The International Encyclopedia of Communica tions, edited by Erik Barnouw; certainly it will attest to the liveliness that makes such niceties a problem. It is absolutely amazing that only three decades ago there appeared in Public Opinion (Quarterly a set of essays which, on the whole (with the notable exception of a contribution by the late Wilbur Schramm), saw "communications research," the predecessor of much current activity, as withering away, depending on the point of view, from having done all that could be done or from not having done enough to establish itself as a worthwhile endeavor. If this be so, the present series, the Encyclopedia, xi Xll Preface and the work they describe should not exist. Obviously, something was overlooked—perhaps everything: the ability of scholars to reformulate theories to revivify failures, the readiness of social scientists to reexamine questions using new methods or concepts, the ability of old questions to lure newcomers into seeking new answers, and, above all, the enormous and rapid communica­ tions technology changes that have refused to allow most questions to ap­ pear settled for long. Thus, "null effects," "only reinforcement," and the "obstinate audience" have become transformed into important outcomes amenable to—arguably demanding—continuing research. In his Volume 2, Berkowitz made similar comments about the diversity and health of social psychology. He saw it as having strong roots in psychology in general, with experimentation (hence his title) as its preferred means of investigation. The study of communicatory behavior, public or social in character, has roots in both psychology in general and social psychology in particular. In the present volume, the former is represented by the work of Huston and Wright and the latter by that of Malamuth. However, it also has a much wider and more mixed parentage. These are exemplified by the synthesis of earlier trends in sociological inquiry with the current agenda for communications research by Neuman and the application of empirical data to the construction of theory in behalf of more effective news reporting—a communications topic if there ever was one—by Davis and Robinson. The experiment similarly has no status as a preferred means of investigation, and this is made clear here by the contributions of Oskamp and Centerwall. The former draws together a large number of studies of a singular media event, the made-for-TV film The Day After, and attempts to draw from this array an empirically based picture of what transpired when this controversial broad­ cast occurred. It could be thought of as a literature review; but because many of these studies would be lost to attention without the intensive search in advance of any publication, it should be thought of as one of those innova­ tion efforts to create a literature, a form of activity which this series hopes to encourage. The latter forcefully attests to the readiness to use methods other than the experiment. Topically, Centerwall thinks "the unthinkable" in giving serious consideration to the possibility that on a widespread basis the mass media have increased violent behavior. The most intriguing aspect, however, is his application of an epidemiological model to a communications issue; that is the kind of boundary crossing for which this series stands. In short, this volume proceeds with the original ambition of the series intact. Along with Volume 1, it amounts to some evidence that it may be achievable. In any case, none of it has been boring for the editor, and he hopes the same can be said for the contributors and (and here he obviously speaks for them as well) for the readers. George Comstock Contents of Previous Volume Volume 1 An Evaluation of the Models Used to Evaluate Television Series Thomas D Cook and Thomas R. Curtin A Synthesis of 1043 Effects of Television on Social Behavior Susan Hearold More Than Meets the Eye: TV News, Priming, and Public Evaluations of the President Shanto Iyengar and Donald R. Kinder The Myth of Massive Media Impact: Savagings and Salvagings William J. McGuire The Found Experiment: A New Technique for Assessing the Impact of Mass Media Violence on Real-World Aggressive Behavior David P. Phillips Index Exposure to Television as a Cause of Violence BRANDON S. CENTERWALL Departments of Epidemiology and Psychiatry University of Washington Seattle, Washington 98195 I. Television and Homicide in South Africa, Canada, and the United States, 1945-1974 1 A. Introduction 1 B. Methods 3 C. Results 5 D. Discussion 15 II. Testing Falsifiable Hypotheses 24 A. Introduction 24 B. Methods 25 C. Results 29 D. Discussion 44 III. Natural Exposure to Television as a Cause of Aggression: A Review of the Literature 48 References 53 I. TELEVISION AND HOMICIDE IN SOUTH AFRICA, CANADA, AND THE UNITED STATES, 1945-1974 A. Introduction Whether exposing children to television increases their physical aggressiveness has been a source of ongoing concern to physicians and public health practitioners (Feinbloom, 1976; Feingold & Johnson, 1977; Holroyd, 1985; Ingelfinger, 1976; Liebert, 1986; Rothenberg, 1975; Singer, 1985; Somers, 1976a,b; Zuckerman & Zuckerman, 1985). Rothenberg (1975) first brought the issue of television violence, and its effect on children's behavior and mental well-being, to the attention of the medical community. Somers (1976a,b) and PUBLIC COMMUNICATION AND BEHAVIOR Copyright © 1989 by Academic Press, Inc. Volume 2 All rights of reproduction in any form reserved. 2 Brandon S. Centerwall Ingelfinger (1976) soon raised the possibility that the imprint of television violence may not be limited to childhood—that it may lead to increased rates of later violence such as assault, rape, robbery, and homicide. The effect of natural exposure to television upon children's physical aggressiveness, as compared to no exposure, has been recently elucidated by Joy, Kimball, and Zabrack (1986). In 1973, a small Canadian town ("Notel") acquired television for the first time. The acquisition of television at such a late date was due to problems with signal reception rather than any hostility toward television. Joy, Kimball, and Zabrack studied the effect o fthe introduc tion of television into this virgin community upon rates of physical aggres sion among Notel children. The control groups were two otherwise similar communities that already had television ("Unitel" and "Multitel"). Prior to the introduction of television into Notel, 60 first and second graders (20 in Notel; 40 in Unitel and Multitel) were observed at random intervals in the school playgrounds for rates of predetermined measures of noxious physical aggression (e.g., hitting, shoving, and biting). Two years after the introduction of television into Notel, 45 of the original cohort (16 in Notel; 29 in Unitel and Multitel) were followed up and observed again for rates of physical aggression, using the same definitions as before. A new set of first and second graders was also observed. Children and observers were blind to the hypothesis being tested, and a new set of observers was used in the follow-up. Rates of physical aggression did not change significantly over the 2-year span for children followed longitudinally in Unitel and Multitel. Rates of physical aggression among children followed longitudinally in Notel increased 160% (p < .001). At the time of follow-up, first and second graders in Unitel and Multitel did not differ significantly in rates of physical aggression from first and second graders 2 years previously. In contrast, at the time of follow- up, first and second graders in Notel had rates of physical aggression 51% greater than first and second graders 2 years previously (p < .05). Unable to randomize Notel children to television exposure versus non- exposure, the researchers used two external control groups (the children of Unitel and Multitel) to assess whether exposure to television increases physical aggression in children. Although the possibility of a confounding third variable cannot be absolutely excluded, the double-blind, prospective study design permits a strong inference of causality (Bailar, Louis, Lavori, & Polansky, 1984). The present study uses a similar research design to assess whether exposure of populations to television increases rates of homicide in adults. The South African government did not permit television broadcasting prior to 1975, even though South African whites were a prosperous, industrialized Western society (Tartter, 1981). Amidst the hostile tensions between the Afrikaner and English white communities, it was generally conceded that any South African television broadcasting industry would have to rely on British Exposure to Television as a Cause of Violence 3 and American imports to fill out its programming schedule. Afrikaner leaders felt this would provide an unacceptable cultural advantage to the English- speaking South Africans. Rather than negotiate a complicated compromise, the Afrikaner-controlled government chose to finesse the issue by forbidding television broadcasting entirely. Thus, an entire population of two million whites—rich and poor, urban and rural, educated and uneducated—was nonselectively and absolutely excluded from exposure to television for a quarter century after the medium was introduced into other Western nations. The South African government sought to prevent any social changes that might occur secondary to the introduction of television (Tartter, 1981). Whether the intervention was (inadvertently) successful with respect to homicide rates is assessed in a case-control study of white homicide rates in South Africa prior to 1975, with the white population of the United States and the entire population of Canada [97% white in 1951 (Leacy, 1983)] serving as external control groups. Other potentially causal variables are also examined. B. Methods /. Study Design The population intervention study design compares a case population with external control groups to determine the effect of interventions upon the real world (Morgenstern, 1982). The design requires comparable case and control populations, a reliable index of effect, baseline rates for both case and control populations, and empirical data gathered prospectively, independent of the hypothesis in question. As with population intervention studies in general, issues of aggregation bias and ecological fallacy are rendered moot (Centerwall, 1989; Morgenstern, 1982). What are desired are not inferences about individuals but about groups—in this case, the effect of collective television ownership, and its pro hibition, upon collective homicide rates. 2. Data Sources a. Changes in Homicide Rates. Data on homicide victimization rates are from the vital statistics registries of South Africa [Bureau of Census and Statistics (BCS), 1960; Bureau of Statistics (BS), 1964, 1965, 1968; Depart ment of Statistics (DS), 1970, 1972, 1974, 1976], Canada [Dominion Bureau of Statistics (DBS), annual volumes; World Health Organization (WHO), annual volumes], and the United States [Grove & Hetzel, 1968; National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS), annual volumes]. The data are for homicide deaths among whites in South Africa and the United States, and for total homicide deaths in Canada. To smooth out the data, the temporal 4 Brandon S. Centerwall trends in annual homicide rates are graphed as a 3-year moving average from 1945 to 1973. Data on white homicide rates in South Africa for 1943-1948 are presented as a 6-year average because this is how the data are presented in the vital statistics tables (BCS, 1960). Annual rates of court convictions for murder among South African whites are from the South African police statistics (BCS, 1960; BS, 1964, 1965, 1968; DS, 1970). b. Changes in Television Ownership, Data on the percentage of Canadian households owning television sets in 1951, 1961, and 1971 are from the Domin ion Bureau of Statistics (DBS, 1953, 1963); Industry and Merchandising Divi sion (IMD, 1953) and Statistics Canada (SC, 1973). Data on the percentage of U.S. households owning television sets, 1945-1974, are from the Television Bureau of Advertising (TBA, 1982) and the U.S. Bureau of the Census (USBC, annual volumes). Television broadcasting was not permitted in South Africa prior to 1975 (Tartter, 1981). c. Changes in Age Distribution, Data on changes in age distribution of the white populations of South Africa (BS, 1964; DS, 1974) and the United States (USBC, 1975), and of the entire population of Canada (Leacy, 1983), are from the respective national censuses. d. Changes in Urbanization. Data on changes in urbanization of the white populations of South Africa (BS, 1964; DS, 1974) and the United States (USBC, 1975), and of the entire population of Canada (Leacy, 1983) are from the respective national censuses. To assess the direct effect of urbanization upon rates of homicide in the United States between 1960 and 1970, city- population-specific rates of criminal homicide from 1975 [U.S. Federal Bureau of Investigation (USFBI), annual volumes] are applied to the observed distributions of the U.S. population by city population in the 1960 and 1970 censuses (USBC, 1975). Because the same city-population-specific rates are applied to both census years, any change in the projected rate of criminal homicide between 1960 and 1970 is solely due to the effect of urbanization per se. This method permits a quantitative measure of the effect of urbaniza tion independent of other factors; it replicates a statistical approach devel oped for the National Commission on the Causes and Prevention of Violence to investigate the effect of urbanization upon homicide rates in the United States between the years 1950 and 1965 (Mulvihill, Tumin, & Curtis, 1969). Criminal homicides are a subset of all homicides, excluding legally justifiable homicides (USFBI, annual volumes). e. Changes in Economic Conditions. Data on changes in annual real per capita product in South Africa (DS, 1976), Canada (Leacy, 1983), and the United States (USBC, annual volumes) are from the respective statistical sum maries. These are expressed as real per capita gross domestic product (1963 Rands) for South Africa, real per capita gross national product (1971 Cana dian dollars) for Canada, and real per capita gross national product (1958 U.S. dollars) for the United States. Data on changes in real per capita product

See more

The list of books you might like

Most books are stored in the elastic cloud where traffic is expensive. For this reason, we have a limit on daily download.