PROCESSINGANDPRODUCINGHEAD-FINALSTRUCTURES STUDIES IN THEORETICAL PSYCHOLINGUISTICS VOLUME38 ManagingEditors LynFrazier,Dept.ofLinguistics,UniversityofMassachusettsatAmherst ThomasRoeper,Dept.ofLinguistics,UniversityofMassachusettsatAmherst KennethWexler,Dept.ofBrainandCognitiveScience,MIT,Cambridge,Mass. EditorialBoard RobertBerwick,ArtificialIntelligenceLaboratory,MIT,Cambridge,Mass. MatthewCrocker,SaarlandUniversity,Germany JanetDeanFodor,CityUniversityofNewYork,NewYork AngelaFriederici,MaxPlanckInstituteofHumanCognitiveandBrainSciences,Germany MerrillGarrett,UniversityofArizona,Tucson LilaGleitman,SchoolofEducation,UniversityofPennsylvania ChrisKennedy,NorthwesternUniversity,Illinois ManfredKrifka,HumboldtUniversity,Berlin,Germany HowardLasnik,UniversityofConnecticutatStorrs YukioOtsu,KeioUniversity,Tokyo AndrewRadford,UniversityofEssex,U.K. Forfurthervolumes: http://www.springer.com/series/6555 PROCESSING AND PRODUCING HEAD-FINAL STRUCTURES Editedby HIROKOYAMASHITA RochesterInstituteofTechnology,Rochester,NY,USA YUKIHIROSE UniversityofTokyo,Tokyo,Japan and JEROMEL.PACKARD UniversityofIllinoisatUrbana-Champaign,IL,USA 1 3 Editors HirokoYamashita YukiHirose DepartmentofModernLanguages DepartmentofLanguage andCultures andInformationSciences RochesterInstituteofTechnology TheUniversityofTokyo 92LombMemorialDrive 3-8-1Komaba,Meguro-ku Rochester,NY,14623USA Tokyo,153-8902 Japan [email protected] [email protected] JeromeL.Packard DepartmentofEastLanguages andCultures UniversityofIllinoisat Urbana-Champaign 2090AForeignLanguageBuilding 707S.Mathews Urbana,IL,61801USA [email protected] ISSN1873-0043 ISBN978-90-481-9212-0 e-ISBN978-90-481-9213-7 DOI10.1007/978-90-481-9213-7 SpringerDordrechtHeidelbergLondonNewYork LibraryofCongressControlNumber:2010937565 #SpringerScienceþBusinessMediaB.V.2011 Nopartofthisworkmaybereproduced,storedinaretrievalsystem,ortransmittedinanyformorby anymeans,electronic,mechanical,photocopying,microfilming,recordingorotherwise,withoutwritten permissionfromthePublisher,withtheexceptionofanymaterialsuppliedspecificallyforthepurposeof beingenteredandexecutedonacomputersystem,forexclusiveusebythepurchaserofthework. Printedonacid-freepaper SpringerispartofSpringerScience+BusinessMedia(www.springer.com) Acknowledgements Wewouldliketothankallcontributorstothecurrentvolume.Thisbookisthe fruitoftheInternationalConferenceonProcessingHead-finalStructuresheld at the Rochester Institute of Technology in September, 2007. Building on the seminalSymposiumofJapaneseSentenceProcessingheldatDukeUniversity in 1991 and the Workshop of Japanese Sentence Processing held at the Ohio State University in 1999, the International Conference on Processing Head- final Structures was the first of its kind, comprehensively dealing with the processingandproductionofhead-finalstructuresinmultiplelanguages. The conference was made possible by grants from the National Science Foundation (BCS-0642367), Center for Language Sciences at University of Rochester, University of Richmond, 21st Century Center of Excellence Program at the University of Tokyo, Center for Evolutionary Cognitive Science at the University of Tokyo, College of Liberal Arts at Rochester Institute of Technology and the Department of Modern Languages and Cultures at Rochester Institute of Technology. The editing of this book was funded by the National Science Foundation (BCS-0642367), and the Office of the Provost for Research at the Rochester Institute of Technology. Any opinions, findings and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of theNationalScienceFoundationorotherfundingsources. v Head-Direction and its Effect on Comprehension and Production ‘‘Whatabouthead-finalstructures?’’Thisquestion,whichbegantoberaisedin the late 1980s, has expanded the depth and breadth of human sentence processingresearch. Earlier work on sentence comprehension in the 1970s revealed the remarkable efficiency of the human language parser. In daily life, listeners and readers receive linguistic input in linear fashion over the time course of a sentence. As information comes in, we encounter many potential lexical and syntactic ambiguities that cause a sentence fragment to have more than one possibleinterpretation. InEnglish,forexample,afragmentsuchasThelawyerinvestigated...could beginasimplexsentencewithadirectobject,suchasThelawyerinvestigatedthe case,oritmaybethebeginningofasentencewitharelativeclause,Thelawyer investigatedfortaxevasionwasinterviewedonthemorningshow.Likewise,the multiple selectional restriction options on the verb knew lead the sentence fragmentJohn knew thegirl... tothe two possiblecompletions John knew the girl or John knew the girl was from New York. Such possibilities raise the question of how humans can so effortlessly process language in spite of such greatpotentialforambiguity. As a means of navigating such ambiguity, researchers have proposed that the parser tends to follow certain principles. For example, the parser may hypothesize the simplest possible structure consistent with the input data – an approach known as the Minimal Attachment Principle (Frazier, 1978) – or it may attempt to associate adjacent words as clause-mates, referred to as the Local Association Principle (Frazier & Fodor, 1978). Researchers have also found that information in the input stream tends to be exploited early on for comprehension purposes. Of particular prominence are phrasal heads – especially verbs – which provide critical information about syntactic constituency (e.g., Boland, Tanenhaus, & Garnsey, 1990; Trueswell, Tanenhaus, & Kello, 1993). Verbs do the important work of specifying selectional restrictions and assigning thematic roles to complements. In a head-initial language like English, the verb appears early, not only marking the onset of the VP but also providing selectional restriction and probabilistic information on the type vii viii Head-DirectionanditsEffectonComprehensionandProduction of complements the verb may take. For example, Garnsey, Perlmutter, Meyers, and Lotocky (1997) found that English readers utilize the statistical preference of the verb, expecting (1b) as the likely continuation when they read the verb argued but expecting (2a) when they read discovered. (1) Thedivorcelawyerarguedtheissue... a.Thedivorcelawyerarguedtheissue. b.Thedivorcelawyerarguedtheissuewasirrelevanttothecase. (2) Thescubadiverdiscoveredthewreck... a.Thescubadiverdiscoveredthewreck. b.Thescubadiverdiscoveredthewreckwascausedbyacollision. This example, in which the parser exploits information contained in the information-laden verb head, involves the notion of head-driven parsing (Abney, 1989; Pritchett, 1988, 1992). Head-driven parsing posits that the parsing of an incoming phrase critically depends on the appearance of the phrasal head, because the phrasal head provides key information about syntactic constituency. According to this theory, the parser waits until the phrasal head appears in the input stream before it assigns a structural interpretationtothephrase. As the examples in (1) and (2) demonstrate, information contained in the verb head is especially important for head-initial languages like English, making the head-driven parsing theory a natural one for head-initial structures. But it also raises an obvious question: if sentence processing is head-driven, then how are structures processed when the head follows a constituent rather than occurring in phrase-initial position (e.g., Inoue, 1991; Inoue&Fodor,1995;Mazuka&Lust,1990;Yamashita,1994)? Languages such as Japanese, Korean, Hindi and Basque are predominantly head-final, with the information contained in the head naturally not available to the parser until the head appears in the input stream. In addition to the early unavailability of head information in those languages, there is also often no clear marking of the beginning of embedded clauses–so-called ‘‘left-edge marking.’’ In such cases the parser has less information about, for example, the degree of embedding within a sentence, until all phrase-final heads are processed. Often these languages have flexible word-order and phonologically null pronouns, both contributing more ambiguity to the parse (e.g., Inoue, 1991; Mazuka & Lust, 1990). To illustrate, the two arguments in Japanese marked by nominative and dativecasemarkersin(3)maybethebeginningofasimplexclauseasin(4),part ofarelativeclauseasin(5),orwithaphonologicallynullpronoun(indicated Head-DirectionanditsEffectonComprehensionandProduction ix here as e) they may be two arguments that belong respectively to matrix and subordinateclauses,asin(6). (3) John-ga Mary-ni... John-NOMMary-DAT (4) John-ga Mary-ni pen-o ageta. John-NOMMary-DATpen-ACCgave. ‘JohngaveMaryanapple.’ (5) [[John-ga Mary-ni ageta]pen]-ga kowareta. John-NOMMary-DATgave pen-NOMbroke ‘ThepenthatJohngavetoMarybroke.’ (6) John-ga [eMary-ni pen-o ageta]-to itta. John-NOM Mary-DATpen-ACCgave-comp said ‘JohnsaidthathegaveapentoMary.’ Thequestionofinterestis:atwhatpointintheprocessingofsentenceslike (4)–(6) does the parser commit itself to a simplex clause analysis or posit embedded structures? With information from the syntactic head available only at clause-final position, the possibility suggested by head-driven parsing is that the parser waitsto posit syntactic structure until the information from theheadbecomesavailable. Asanalternativetohead-drivenparsing,itwasproposedthattheparseris more opportunistic, using all available information to build structure even before the information from the head is available. This fully incremental parsing approach (e.g., Crocker, 1994; Gorrell, 1995; Lombardo & Sturt, 2002; Stabler, 1994; Sturt & Crocker, 1996) entails that every incoming word incrementally contributes its information to the structure of the phrase under construction.Themultiplicityofpossibleanalysesinherentinsuchanapproach entails that the parser frequently revises its initial hypotheses, potentially resulting in increased processing difficulty. In 1995 Inoue and Fodor introduced ‘‘Information–paced Processing,’’ in which the ease of revising incorrect initial parses depends upon the reliability of the existing analysis. Thus even though the delayed availability of information from the head in head-final languages might lead to more parsing possibilities than in head- initial languages, it was suggested that head-final languages may have more tolerance for adjusting initial analyses. Those questions involving head-final parsing led to a flurry of processing studies in languages with head-final structures (e.g., Berwick & Fong, 1995; Konieczny, Hemforth, Scheepers, & Strube, 1997; Miyamoto, 1999; Nakayama, 1995; Sakamoto, 1991; Vasishth, 2003;Yamashita,1994). As the field continued to develop based on the syntax-based approach followed in the 1970s, sentence processing researchers examined factors such x Head-DirectionanditsEffectonComprehensionandProduction asdiscourse(Altmann&Steedman,1988;Crain&Steedman,1985),stochastic information(Merlo&Stevenson,2002)andprosody(Beach,1991;Kjelgaard& Speer, 1999). New experimental procedures were devised, with eye-tracking, event-relatedpotential(ERP),brainimagingandmostrecentlycorpusstudies, coming onto the scene and used alongside the reliable standard self-paced moving-window technique. Head-final sentence processing research developed concomitantly, with some studies focusing on areas common to head-initial and head-final structures. They include the studies on the immediate utilization of case and other morphological markings or verb information for predicting (a) forthcoming structure(s) (e.g., Kamide & Mitchell,1999;Yamashita,1995;Yoshida,2006),theroleofthematicrolesat the different stages of processing (Hirose, 2002; Hirose & Inoue, 1998), individual differences in comprehension (Jincho, Namiki, & Mazuka, 2008) and preference on ambiguous relative clause interpretation (relative clause attachment) (e.g., Gibson, Perlmutter, Gonzalez, & Hickok, 1996; Hemforth, Konieczny, Scheepers, & Strube, 1998; Miyamoto, Gibson, Pearlmutter, Aikawa, & Miyagawa, 1999). Some researchers used head-final structures to posequestionsnoteasilyaddressedinhead-initiallanguages,suchasdetecting or processing relative clauses, where the head is at the final position of the construction (e.g., Hsiao & Gibson, 2003; Miyamoto & Nakamura, 2003; Ueno&Garnsey,2007).Andsomeinvestigatedtheprocessingthatisuniqueto languageswithhead-finalstructuressuchaseffectsofscramblinginprocessing (Kim, 1999; Mazuka, Itoh, & Kondo, 2002; Miyamoto & Takahashi, 2002; Yamashita, 1997). These approaches were critical in advancing the field of psycholinguistics, as they drew commonalities among different types of languagesorhighlightedtheeffectsoflanguage-specificlinguisticphenomena onhumanlanguageprocessing. Studies on head-final languages are no longer uncommon in the field of sentence processing but at the same time the increasing number of topics and variety of examined structures signaled the need for a comprehensive examination of head-final sentence processing research. The International Conference on Processing Head-final Structures held at the Rochester InstituteofTechnologyinSeptember2007servedthatpurpose. Our conference was intended to bring together cutting-edge research on head-final sentence processing and production, to generate comparisons with head-initiallanguagessuchasEnglishandtohaveahandinsettingtheresearch agenda over the next several years. The chapters in this book find their provenanceinthepresentationsatthatconference,offeringexperimentaldata andtheoreticaldiscussionsfromhead-finalstructuresinlanguagesasdiverseas Basque,Chinese,Hindi,Japanese,KoreanandGerman. ThechaptersinPartIfocusonthecomprehensionofhead-finalstructures and its incremental nature. Although the details remain a matter of debate, resultsonhead-finalprocessingsuggestthattheparserisatworkincrementally employing all available sources of information – such as case, agreement, or thematicroles–wellbeforetheheadisencountered.Evidenceforincremental